please write a short survey based on the following criteria

User Generated

nobav21

Humanities

MAMP507A

Adler School of Professional Psychology

Description

[6.1] Constructing and Evaluating Surveys Discussion

No unread replies.No replies.

After reading Babbie (chapters 9 & 14) and Onwuegbuzie & Leech (2005), please write a short survey based on the following criteria:

  1. Choose a topic for your survey which is related to the topic for your research proposal.
  2. Create 3 to 5 questions. Formulate your questions to ensure clarity and focus.

After writing your survey, please respond to the following:

  1. Identify and describe the purpose of your survey.
  2. Provide an analysis of at least two strengths and two weaknesses of your survey.
  3. Evaluate any potential ethical concerns that could be involved in the distribution of your survey

the research is on veterans with PTSD and how recreational therapy can help.

Unformatted Attachment Preview

11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 1/10 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 2/10 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 3/10 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 4/10 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 5/10 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 6/10 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 7/10 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 8/10 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 9/10 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 10/10 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 1/10 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 2/10 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 3/10 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 4/10 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 5/10 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 6/10 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 7/10 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 8/10 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 9/10 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 10/10 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 1/10 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 2/10 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 3/10 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 4/10 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 5/10 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 6/10 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 7/10 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 8/10 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 9/10 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 10/10 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 1/7 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 2/7 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 3/7 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 4/7 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 5/7 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 6/7 11/30/2018 The Practice of Social Research, 14e PRINTED BY: alexbonilla813@gmail.com. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781305445567/cfi/272!/4/4@0.00:0.00 7/7 DOCUMENT RESUME ED 482 462 AUTHOR TITLE PUB DATE NOTE PUB TYPE EDRS PRICE DESCRIPTORS TM 035 389 Onwuegbuzie, Anthony J.; Leech, Nancy L. On Becoming a Pragmatic Researcher: The Importance of Combining Quantitative and Qualitative Research Methodologies. 2003-11-00 25p.; Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Mid-South Educational Research Association (Biloxi, MS, November 5-7, 2003). Reports Descriptive (141) Speeches/Meeting Papers (150) EDRS Price MF01/PCO2 Plus Postage. *Pragmatics; *Qualitative Research; *Research Methodology; *Researchers ABSTRACT The last 100 years have witnessed a fervent debate in the United States about quantitative and qualitative research paradigms. Unfortunately, this has led to a great divide between quantitative and qualitative researchers, who often view themselves in competition with each other. Clearly, this polarization has promoted purists, i.e., researchers who restrict themselves exclusively to either quantitative or qualitative research methods. Mono-method research is the biggest threat to the advancement of the social sciences. As long as researchers stay polarized in research they cannot expect stakeholders who rely on their research findings to take their work seriously. The purpose of this paper is to explore how the debate between quantitative and qualitative is divisive, and thus counterproductive for advancing the social and behavioral science field. This paper advocates that all graduate students learn to use and appreciate both quantitative and qualitative research. In so doing, students will develop into what is termed "pragmatic researchers." (Contains 41 references.) (Author/SLD) Reproductions supplied by EDRS are the best that can be made from the original document. On Becoming a Pragmatic Researcher Running head: ON BECOMING A PRAGMATIC RESEARCHER U.S. DEPARTMENT OF EDUCATION Office of Educational Research and Improvement EDUCATIONAL RESOURCES INFORMATION CENTER (ERIC) 61/This document has been reproduced as received from the person or organization originating it. PERMISSION TO REPRODUCE AND DISSEMINATE THIS MATERIAL HAS BEEN GRANTED BY A. Onwuegbuzie 0 Minor changes have been made to improve reproduction quality. Points of view or opinions stated in this document do not necessarily represent official OERI position or policy. TO THE EDUCATIONAL RESOURCES INFORMATION CENTER (ERIC) On Becoming a Pragmatic Researcher: The Importance of Combining Quantitative and Qualitative Research Methodologies Anthony J. Onwuegbuzie 71- University of South Florida 00 Nancy L. Leech University of Colorado at Denver Correspondence should be addressed to Anthony J. Onwuegbuzie, Department of Educational Measurement and Research, College of Education, University of South Florida, 4202 East Fowler Avenue, EDU 162,Tampa, Florida 33620-7750 or E-Mail: (tonyonwuecibuzieaol.com). co of) Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Mid-South Educational Research cz) Association, Biloxi, MS, November 5-7, 2003. 'BEST COPY AVM LAkF 1 On Becoming a Pragmatic Researcher 2 Abstract The last 100 years has witnessed a fervent debate in the United States about quantitative and qualitative research paradigms. Unfortunately, this has led to a great divide between quantitative and qualitative researchers, who often view themselves as in competition with each other. Clearly, this polarization has promoted purists, namely, researchers who restrict themselves exclusively either to quantitative or to qualitative research methods. Mono-method research is the biggest threat to the advancement of the social sciences. Indeed, as long as we stay polarized in research, how can we expect stakeholders who rely on our research findings to take our work seriously? Thus, the purpose of this paper is to explore how the debate between quantitative and qualitative is divisive and, hence, counterproductive for advancing the social and behavioral science field. This paper advocates for all graduate students to learn to utilize and to appreciate both quantitative and qualitative research. In so doing, students will develop into what we term as pragmatic researchers. On Becoming a Pragmatic Researcher 3 On Becoming a Pragmatic-Researcher: The Importance of Combining Quantitative and Qualitative Research Methodologies Throughout the 20th century, social and behavioral science researchers in the U.S. have witnessed a great divide between two opposing camps of researchers. Specifically, these camps have comprised positivists on one side and interpretivists on the other side. Interestingly, as noted by Sechrest and Sidani (1995), it is only in the social and behavioral sciences that the merits of both research paradigms are so vehemently debated. The quantitative versus qualitative contest often has been so divisive that many social and behavioral science students who graduate from American educational institutions with an aspiration to gain employment in the world of academia or research, are left with the impression that they have to pledge allegiance to one research school of thought or the other. Even more importantly, these students ire led to believe in the Incompatibility Thesis (Howe, 1988), which posits that quantitative and qualitative research paradigms and methodologies cannot and must not be mixed. Yet, there is a general tendency among researchers to treat epistemology and method as being synonymous (Bryman, 1984). This is far from being the case because the epistemology does not dictate what specific data collection and data analytical methods should be used by researchers. Thus, the purpose of this paper is to explore how the debate between quantitative and qualitative is divisive and, hence, counterproductive for advancing the social and behavioral science field. Instead, we advocate that all graduate students learn to utilize and to appreciate both quantitative and qualitative research. In so doing, students will develop into what we term as pragmatic researchers. Fundamental Differences Between Quantitative and Qualitative Research Paradigm Proponents of both camps (i.e., purists) tend to focus on the differences between 4 On Becoming a Pragmatic Researcher 4 the quantitative and qualitative philosophies rather than on the similarities. According to purists, distinctions exist between quantitative and qualitative researchers with respect to ontology, epistemology, axiology, rhetoric, logic, generalizations, and causal linkages. With respect to ontological differences, theoretically, positivists believe that there is a single reality and that this reality, which is expressed in terms of variables, can be measured in a reliable and valid manner using a priori operational and standardized definitions, whereas interpretivists believe that there are multiple-constructed realities (i.e., relativist) and that multiple interpretations are available from different researchers that are all equally valid. In the field of the social and behavioral sciences, interpretivists posit that these realities are socially constructed, that they are products of human intellects, and that they alter as their constructors change. Moreover, qualitative purists believe that researchers should study the social world from the view of the actor. With regard to epistemological differences, positivists contend that the researcher (i.e., knower) and object of study (i.e., known) are independent. As such, according to these proponents, researchers should remain objective in studying phenomena. Conversely, for interpretivists, the researcher and object of study are dependent. As such, qualitative researchers attempt to position themselves as closely as possible to what is being studied. The role of values (i.e., axiology) are supposed to play a different role in quantitative and qualitative research. Specifically, positivists contend that inquiry should be value-free, whereas interpretivists believe that research is influenced greatly by the values of investigators. In other words, interpretivists believe that inquiry is value-bound. As such, the rhetoric used by both sets of purists tends to have distinct features. Positivists advocate rhetorical neutrality, involving an exclusively formal writing style using the impersonal voice and specific terminology. On the other hand, interpretivists' writing style predominantly is informal, using the personal voice and limited definitions. 5 On Becoming a Pragmatic Researcher 5 Another difference that quantitative and qualitative researchers emphasize relate to the reasoning process. The hallmark of positivism is the use of deductive reasoning, which is a system for organizing known facts in order to reach a conclusion. In general, deductive reasoning is a cognitive process in which researchers proceed from general to specific conclusions using established rules of logic. Under the deductive reasoning framework, conclusions are true only if the premises on which they are based are true. Thus, positivists emphasize the importance of a priori hypotheses and theories. Interpretivists tend to incorporate inductive reasoning, in which observations are made on particular events, and then, on the basis of these observed events, inferences are made. In other words, conclusions are reached by observing examples and then making generalizations from the examples. As such, inductive reasoning is the reverse of deductive reasoning. Because a major goal in quantitative research is to generalize findings to the population from which the sample was drawn, samples typically are larger than that for qualitative research, wherein use of relatively few cases is more the norm. Further, the preferred sampling method of positivists is that of (probability) random sampling (i.e., simple random sampling, stratified random sampling, cluster random sampling, systematic random sampling, and multi-stage random sampling), interpretivists tend to select purposive (i.e., judgmental), nonprobability samples in which individuals are chosen because of their ability to provide thick, rich data. In qualitative research, generality usually is less of a goal in deciding upon the sample, than who or what can facilitate understanding of the underlying phenomenon. In fact, of the 16 types of qualitative sampling techniques identified by Miles and Huberman (1994), only one method is probability based. Positivists believe that real causes to social scientific outcomes can be determined reliably, and, as such, findings are replicable. These causal agents are On Becoming a Pragmatic Researcher 6 assumed to be either temporally precedent to or simultaneous with effects. In experimental research, the researcher manipulates at least one independent variable (i.e., the hypothesized cause), attempts to control potentially extraneous (i.e., confounding) variables, and then measures the effect(s) on one or more dependent variables. According to this line of reasoning, valid cause-effect relationships are established, if results obtained are due only to the manipulated independent variable (i.e., possess internal validity). In contrast, however, interpretivists believe that it is impossible to distinguish cause from effects. From their perspective, an experiment, at best, represents a piecemeal attempt to understand the relationships between variables. As such, they believe that experimentation does not identify cause-effect relationships because reality cannot be broken down into component parts without running the risk of distorting the findings--thereby justifying holistic analyses that are generated by qualitative research (Rist, 1977). As noted by Rossman and Wilson (1985), from the quantitative-qualitative paradigm wars have evolved three major schools of thought, namely: purists, situationalists, and pragmatists. The difference between these three perspectives relates to the extent to which each believes that quantitative and qualitative approaches co-exist and can be combined. These three camps can be conceptualized as lying on a continuum, with purists and pragmatists lying on opposite ends, and situationalists lying somewhere between purists and pragmatists. Purists posit that quantitative and qualitative methods stem from different ontologic, epistemologic, and axiologic assumptions about the nature of research (Bryman, 1984; Collins, 1984; Tashakkori & Teddlie, 1998). Moreover, for purists, the assumptions associated with both paradigms are incompatible about how the world is viewed and what is important to know. Purists, such as Smith (1983) and Smith and Heshusius (1986), contend that quantitative and qualitative approaches cannot and 7 On Becoming a Pragmatic Researcher should not be mixed. As such, they advocate mono-method studies. Situationalists maintain the mono-method (paradigmatic) stance held by purists, but also contend that both methods have value. However, they believe that certain research questions lend themselves more to quantitative approaches, whereas other research questions are more suitable for qualitative methods. Thus, although representing very different orientations, the two approaches are treated as being "complementary" (Vidich & Shapiro, 1955, p. 33). Finally, at the other end of the continuum, pragmatists, unlike purists and situationalists, contend that a false dichotomy exists between quantitative and qualitative approaches (Denzin, 1970). These proponents believe that quantitative methods are not necessarily positivist, nor are qualitative techniques necessarily hermeneutic (Cook & Reichardt, 1979; Daft, 1983; Miller & Fredericks, 1991; Sieber, 1973). As such, pragmatists advocate integrating methods within a single study (Creswell, 1995). Moreover, Sieber (1973) articulated that because both approaches have inherent strengths and weaknesses, researchers should utilize the strengths of both techniques in order to understand better social phenomena. Indeed, pragmatists ascribe to the philosophy that the research question should drive the method(s) used, believing that "epistemological purity doesn't get research done" (Miles & Huberman, 1984, p. 21). In any case, researchers who ascribe to epistemological purity disregard the fact that research methodologies are merely tools that are designed to aid our understanding of the world. The differences between the three major research paradigms are outlined in Figure 1. This figure represents a bi-dimensional diagram portraying two sets of poles, namely, (a) a vertical pole with the quantitative research paradigm and the qualitative research paradigm at the opposite ends of the pole, and (b) a horizontal pole with quantitative methods and qualitative methods at the opposite ends of the pole. That is, 8 7 On Becoming a Pragmatic Researcher 8 the vertical pole is at the level of logic of justification (Smith & Heshusius, 1986), reconstructed logic (Kaplan, 1964), or epistemology (Bryman, 1984), whereas the horizontal pole is at the methodological level (Smith & Heshusius, 1986), logic in use (Kaplan, 1964), or technical level (Bryman, 1984). In Figure 1, the upper left quadrant, labeled as "(1)," represents quantitative purists, who believe that research should be undertaken via the exclusive use of the quantitative framework, adhering to the positivist assumptions and utilizing only quantitative research methodologies. In stark contrast, the bottom right quadrant, labeled as "(4)," represents qualitative purists, who ascribe exclusively to hermeneutical principals using only qualitative techniques. The upper right quadrant, labeled as "(2)," represents a direct challenge to positivism because it involves the use of qualitative methods to test hypotheses and the like. Finally, the lower left quadrant, marked as "(3)," represents a direct challenge to interpretivist dogma because it entails the use of quantitative methods to discover meaning of social phenomena. Situationalists advocate the separate use of quadrants "(1)" and "(4)," but do not believe that the combinations represented by quadrants "(2)" and "(3)" are possible. On the other hand, pragmatists believe that regardless of the research paradigm, quantitative and qualitative methodologies should be mixed, if the research question lends itself to this format. As such, pragmatists champion the simultaneous use of quadrants "(1)" and "(2)," as well as the combined use of quadrants "(2)" and "(4)." Insert Figure 1 about here Misconceptions Held by Purists and Situationalists Many of the differences that are perceived to prevail between quantitative and qualitative research stem from the misconceptions and mis-claims of proponents of both 9 On Becoming a Pragmatic Researcher 9 camps. On the positivist side, the barriers that they have built arises from their narrow definition of the concept of "science." As noted by Onwuegbuzie (2002), positivists claim that the essence of science is objective verification, and that their methods are objective. However, positivists disregard the fact that many research decisions are made throughout the research process that precede objective verification decisions. For example, in developing instruments that yield empirical data, psychometricians select items in an attempt to represent the content domain adequately (Onwuegbuzie & Daniel, 2002). Yet, choosing these items represents a subjective decision at every stage of the instrument-development process. Thus, although the final version of the instrument can lead to objective scoring, because of the subjectivity built into its development, any interpretations of the scores yielded cannot be 100% objective. Simply put, SUBJECTIVITY + OBJECTIVITY = SUBJECTIVITY Moreover, although in the natural sciences, many properties of objects can be measured with near-perfect reliability, in the social sciences, the vast majority of measures yield scores that are, to some degree, unreliable. This is because constructs of interest in the social science fields typically represent abstractions (e.g., personality, achievement, intelligence, motivation, locus of control) that must be measured indirectly (Onwuegbuzie & Daniel, 2002). Failure to attain 100% score reliability implies measurement error, which, in turn, introduces subjectivity into any interpretations. In the social science field, at least, the techniques used by positivists are no more inherently scientific than are the procedures utilized by interpretivists. Interpretivists also are not safe from criticism. In particular, their claim that multiple, contradictory, but valid accounts of the same phenomenon always exist is extremely misleading, inasmuch as it leads many qualitative researchers to adopt an "anything goes" relativist attitude, thereby not paying due attention to providing an On Becoming a Pragmatic Researcher 10 adequate rationale for interpretations of their data. That is, many qualitative methods of analyses "often remain private and unavailable for public inspection" (Constas, 1992, p. 254). Yet, without standards, when do we know whether what we know is trustworthy? Similarities Between Quantitative and Qualitative Research Approaches Indubitably the most disturbing feature of the paradigm wars is the relentless focus on the differences between the two orientations. As noted by Onwuegbuzie (in press, p. 2), "much of the quantitative-qualitative debate has involved the practice of polemics, which has tended to obfuscate rather than to clarify, and to divide rather than to unite educational researchers." Indeed, the two dominant research paradigms have resulted in two research subcultures, "one professing the superiority of 'deep, rich observational data' and the other the virtues of 'hard, generalizable' survey data" (Sieber, 1973, p. 1335). Yet, there are overwhelmingly more similarities between quantitative and qualitative perspectives than there are differences. First and foremost, both quantitative and qualitative procedures involve the use of observations to address research questions. As noted by Sechrest and Sidani (1995, p. 78), both methodologies "describe their data, construct explanatory arguments from their data, and speculate about why the outcomes they observed happened as they did." Not emphasized by purists is the fact that both sets of researchers use techniques that are relatively analogous at some level of specificity. Most researchers incorporate safeguards into their research in order to minimize confirmation bias and other sources of invalidity that have the potential to prevail in every research study (Sandelowski, 1986). For example, both quantitative and qualitative researchers often attempt to triangulate their data. Further, like interpretivists, to some degree, quantitative data analysts attempt to provide explanations as to their findings (McLoughlin, 1991) as well as to make interpretive, narrative conclusions pertaining to 11 On Becoming a Pragmatic Researcher 11 the implications of their findings (Dzurec & Abraham, 1993). As noted by Dzurec and Abraham (1993), meaning is not a function of the type of data collected (i.e., quantitative vs. qualitative). Rather, meaning results from the interpretation of data, whether represented by numbers or words. Whereas quantitative researchers utilize statistical techniques and subjective inferences to make decisions about what their data mean in the context of an a priori theoretical or conceptual framework, qualitative researchers use phenomenological procedures and their views of reality to discover meaning (Dzurec & Abraham, 1993). Both sets of researchers select and use analytical techniques that are designed to obtain the maximal meaning from their data, and manipulate their data so that findings have utility with respect to their respective views of reality (Dzurec & Abraham, 1993). Moreover, both types of inquirers attempt to explain complex relationships that exist in the social science field. To this end, quantitative researchers utilize multivariate techniques (Elmore & Woehlke, 1998), whereas qualitative researchers incorporate the collection of rich, thick data into their design via prolonged engagement, persistent observation, and other strategies (Lincoln & Guba, 1985). Additionally, both quantitative and qualitative investigators utilize techniques to verify their data. The former incorporate a myriad of control procedures and random sampling techniques to maximize internal and external validity, respectively, with the latter using an array of methods for assessing the auditability or credibility of qualitative research. Such techniques include triangulation, prolonged engagement, persistent observation, leaving an audit trail, member checking, weighting the evidence, checking for representativeness of sources of data, checking for researcher effects, making contrasts/comparisons, checking the meaning of outliers, using extreme cases, ruling out spurious relations, replicating a finding, assessing rival explanations, looking for negative evidence, obtaining feedback from informants, peer debriefing, clarifying 12 On Becoming a Pragmatic Researcher 12 researcher bias, and thick description (Creswell, 1998). Interestingly, data reduction typically is an important part of the data analysis process for both quantitative and qualitative researchers. Whereas statisticians utilize data-reduction methods such as factor analysis and cluster analysis, interpretivists conduct thematic analyses. Thus, factors that emerge from multivariate analyses are analogous to emergent themes from thematic analyses. Indeed, Onwuegbuzie (in press) demonstrated how themes emerging from qualitative data analyses can be factor analyzed to obtain meta-themes that subsume the original themes, thereby describing the relationship among these themes. Additionally, the popularization of complex multivariate analyses (e.g., structural equation modeling and hierarchical linear modeling), coupled with the increased emphasis on generalizability theory, allow quantitative researchers to pay better attention to context effects than previously has been the case. As noted by Newman and Benz (1998), rather than representing bi-polar opposites, quantitative and qualitative research represent an interactive continuum. Moreover, the role of theory is central for both paradigms. Specifically, in qualitative research, the most common purposes are that of theory initiation and theory building, whereas in quantitative research, the most typical objectives are that of theory testing and theory modification (Newman & Benz, 1998). Clearly, neither tradition is independent of the other, nor can either school encompass the whole research process. Thus, both quantitative and qualitative research are needed to gain a more complete understanding of phenomena (Newman & Benz, 1998). Another way in which quantitative and qualitative research are congruent lies in the fact that both empirical and qualitative data are interchangeable. That is, just as it could be contended that all data are basically qualitative (Berg, 1989) inasmuch as they represent an attempt to capture a raw experience, so it could be argued that all data On Becoming a Pragmatic Researcher 13 can be quantified (Sechrest & Sidana, 1995). More specifically, all data can be binarized, a term coined by Onwuegbuzie (in press) to describe dichotomously expressing a variable in binary form (i.e., "1" vs. "0"). Indeed, just as experimental, quasi-experimental, and correlation research designs can incorporate the collection of observational and interview data, so can qualitative designs include the collection of empirical data. As aptly stated by Kaplan (1964, p. 207), "Quantities are of qualities, and a measured quality has just the magnitude expressed in its measure." Additionally, Onwuegbuzie illustrated how inferential statistics can be utilized in qualitative data analyses. According to this author, "this can be accomplished by treating words arising from individuals, or observations emerging from a particular setting, as sample units of data that represent the total number of words/observations existing from that sample member/context" (p. 2). Onwuegbuzie argued that inferential statistics can be used to provide more complex levels of verstehen than is presently undertaken in qualitative research. Building on Onwuegbuzie's work, Onwuegbuzie and Teddlie (2002) outlined different ways of conducting mixed methodological analyses. However, quantification should not be viewed as an end to itself, but instead as a means of utilizing existing techniques that provide incremental validity to thematic analyses (Weinstein & Tamur, 1978). Further, it should be stressed that mixed method analyses always are possible or even appropriate. Indeed, the challenge is knowing when it is useful to count and when it is difficult or inappropriate to count (Gherardi & Turner, 1987). As discussed above, many parallels exist between quantitative and qualitative research. Regardless of orientation, all research in the social sciences represents an attempt to understand human beings and the world around them. Thus, it is clear that although, presently, certain methodologies tend to be associated with and utilized by one particular research tradition or the other, as stated by Dzurec and Abraham (1993, 14 On Becoming a Pragmatic Researcher 14 p. 75), "the objectives, scope, and nature of inquiry are consistent across methods and across paradigms." Indeed, the purity of a research paradigm is a function of the extent to which the researcher is prepared to conform to its underlying assumptions. If differences exist between quantitative and qualitative researchers, these discrepancies do not stem from different goals but because these two groups of researchers have operationalized their strategies differently for reaching these goals (Dzurec & Abraham, 1993). This suggests that methodological pluralism should be promoted. The best way for this to occur is for as many investigators as possible to become pragmatic researchers. Barriers Affecting the Pragmatic researcher Movement. The lack of epistemological ecumenism that appears to prevail in the United States in the behavioral and social science field in general, and in the field of educational research in particular, stems from several factors. The century-long trend of doctoral students graduating with basic competency in only one research orientation (i.e., purists) has arisen from American graduate-level instruction in which quantitative and qualitative research methodologies are taught as two independent and isolated fields, rather than as parts of a holistic, interactive, and unifying process. Further, graduate-level programs of study tend to minimize students' exposure to quantitative and qualitative content, by including no or inadequate instruction in mixed methodological techniques. Other barriers include the promotion of various misleading "mythologies" about the nature of quantitative and qualitative research; increasing numbers of instructors teaching research methodology courses when they are not qualified to do so, and from an inability, unwillingness, or even refusal to accept that the epistemological purity that was popularized in previous decades no longer represent best practices and, moreover, may now be considered inappropriate, unreliable, invalid, or outmoded. 15 On Becoming a Pragmatic Researcher 15 Many purists feel alienated by researchers with different orientations. This feeling of alienation is exacerbated by the terminology used by writers from both disciplines. The language used in quantitative research is particularly problematic for students. In fact, Onwuegbuzie, DaRos, and Ryan (1997) found that for many graduate students, statistics anxiety stems from the conventions of notation and terminology. These learners find the language and structure to be unusual. Also, some students report an uneasiness at being asked to accept certain assumptions, formulas, and concepts, as is common in statistical analyses. Finally, as noted above, the dearth of pragmatic researchers also appears to stem from researchers' faulty perceptions of a one-to-one relationship between epistemology and method. Interestingly, Snizek (1976), who analyzed many published research articles, concluded that the research techniques utilized cannot be directly extrapolated from a knowledge of the investigator's epistemological underpinnings and assumptions. More recently, Gueulette, Newgent, and Newman (1999), who analyzed 339 randomly selected studies that were labeled by their authors as representing qualitative research, found that 44.1% of these articles actually involved the blending of qualitative and quantitative methodologies. This latter finding illustrates the existing blurred line between quantitative and qualitative research. Towards Methodological Pluralism In order to become pragmatic researchers, the barriers mentioned above must be dismantled or at least minimized. One step towards accomplishing this is to re-frame the concept of research in the social and behavioral sciences by de-emphasizing the terms quantitative and qualitative research and, instead, sub-dividing research into exploratory and confirmatory methods (Onwuegbuzie & Teddlie, 2002). According to Onwuegbuzie and Teddlie (2002), such a re-conceptualization unites quantitative and qualitative data collection and data analytical procedures under the same framework. 16 On Becoming a Pragmatic Researcher 16 Onwuegbuzie and Teddlie (2002) conceptualized a model in which quantitative data analysis techniques are labeled as exploratory (e.g., descriptive statistics, exploratory factor analysis, and cluster analysis), and exploratory qualitative data analysis involves the traditional thematic analyses. With regard to confirmatory methods, quantitative data-analytical techniques incorporate the assortment of inferential statistics, whereas qualitative data-analytic methods involve confirmatory thematic analyses, in which replication qualitative studies are conducted to assess the replicability of previous emergent themes (i.e., research driven) or to test an extant theory (i.e., theory driven), when appropriate (Onwuegbuzie & Teddlie, 2002). Using Onwuegbuzie and Teddlie's (2002) framework, quantitative and qualitative research courses can be re-designed as courses in exploratory and confirmatory techniques that teach quantitative and qualitative methodologies within each course, either simultaneously or in a sequential manner. Qualitative and quantitative research faculty team-teaching a course would be truly creative and exciting. Moreover, such courses would send a strong message to students that both applied quantitative and qualitative research, for the most part, have the same goal: to understand phenomena systematically and coherently. As such, students enrolled in these courses will come to regard research as being a collaborative undertaking. Additionally, these courses would allow students to focus on the similarities of quantitative and qualitative research outlined above, rather than on the differences. However, most importantly, such courses will help to develop pragmatic researchers equipped to utilize both quantitative and qualitative techniques. Advantages of Being a Pragmatic researcher Becoming a pragmatic researcher offers a myriad of advantages for individuals. First and foremost, it enables researchers to be flexible in their investigative techniques, as they attempt to address a range of research questions that arise. Pragmatic 17 On Becoming a Pragmatic Researcher 17 researchers also are more likely to promote collaboration among researchers, regardless of philosophical orientation. Based on Newman and Benz's (1998) conceptualization of the role of theory in quantitative and qualitative inquiries, pragmatic researchers are more likely to view research as a holistic endeavor that requires prolonged engagement, persistent observation, and triangulation (Lincoln & Guba, 1985). By having a positive atfitude towards both techniques, pragmatic researchers are in a better position to use qualitative research to inform the quantitative portion of research studies, and vice versa. For example, the inclusion of quantitative data can help compensate for the fact that qualitative data typically cannot be generalized. Similarly, the inclusion of qualitative data can help explain relationships discovered by quantitative data. Pragmatic researchers also are more able to combine empirical precision with descriptive precision (Onwuegbuzie, in press). Also, armed with a bi-focal lens (i.e., both quantitative and qualitative data), rather than with a single lens, pragmatic researchers are able to zoom in to microscopic detail or to zoom out to indefinite scope (Willems & Raush, 1969). As such, pragmatic researchers have the opportunity to combine the macro and micro levels of a research issue. As noted by Madey (1982), combining quantitative and qualitative research helps to develop a conceptual framework, to validate quantitative findings by referring to information extracted from the qualitative phase of the study, and to construct indices from qualitative data that can be used to analyze quantitative data. Further, because quantitative research typically is motivated by the researcher's concerns, whereas qualitative research often is driven by a desire to capture the participant's voice, pragmatic researchers are able to merge these two emphases within a single investigation. 18 On Becoming a Pragmatic Researcher 18 Because pragmatic researchers utilize mixed methodologies within the same inquiry, they are able to delve further into a dataset to understand its meaning and to use one method to verify findings from the other method. Indeed, building on Rossman and Wilson's (1985) work, Greene, Caracelli, and Graham (1989) outlined the following five broad purposes of mixed-methodological studies: (a) Triangulation (i.e., seeking convergence and corroboration of results from different methods studying the same phenomenon); (b) Complementarity (i.e., seeking elaboration, enhancement, illustration, clarification of the results from one method with results from the other method); (c) Development (i.e., using the results from one method to help inform the other method); (d) Initiation (i.e., discovering paradoxes and contradictions that lead to a re-framing of the research question); and (e) Expansion (i.e., seeking to expand the breadth and range of inquiry by using different methods for different inquiry components). Greene et al.'s framework, as well as those outlined in Tashakkori and Teddlie's (2002) book, entitled, Handbook of mixed methods in social and behavioral research, offer potential for developing pragmatic researchers. Conclusions The last 100 years has witnessed a fervent debate about quantitative and qualitative research paradigms. Unfortunately, this has led to a great divide in the United States between quantitative and qualitative researchers, who often view themselves as in competition with each other. Clearly, this polarization has promoted purists, namely, researchers who restrict themselves exclusively either to quantitative or to qualitative research methods. Yet, relying on only one type of data (i.e., number or words) is extremely limiting. As such, mono-method research is the biggest threat to the advancement of the social sciences. Indeed, as long as we stay polarized in research, how can we expect stakeholders who rely on our research findings to take our work seriously? 19 On Becoming a Pragmatic Researcher 19 It has been shown throughout this paper that a false dichotomy exists between quantitative and qualitative research. In fact, as noted by Tashakkori and Teddlie (1998), all distinctions between quantitative and qualitative research methods lie on continua. For example, the extent to which an independent variable is manipulated lies on a continuum ranging from situations in which the investigator is the agent of change in the "treatment" to cases where the investigator has no control over such changes. Similarly, the research setting used lies on a continuum ranging from natural to controlled. Indeed, experiments can occur in natural settings (e.g., field experiments), while case studies can occur in controlled settings (e.g., clinical case studies). Additionally, hypotheses lie on a continuum ranging from exploratory to confirmatory. These are just a few examples that illustrate the false dichotomy prevailing between both traditions. Indeed, if a construct is measured using only one research method, then it would be difficult to differentiate the construct from its particular mono-method operational definition (Tashakkori & Teddlie, 1998). As noted by Sechrest and Sidani (1995), a growth in the pragmatic researcher movement has the potential to reduce some of the problems associated with singular methods. By utilizing quantitative and qualitative techniques within the same framework, pragmatic researchers can incorporate the strengths of both methodologies. Most importantly, pragmatic researchers are more likely to be cognizant of all available research techniques and to select methods with respect to their value for addressing the underlying research questions, rather than with regard to some preconceived biases about which paradigm is a hegemony in social science research. 0 On Becoming a Pragmatic Researcher 20 References Berg, B. L. (1989). Qualitative research methods for the social sciences. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon. Bryman, A. (1984). The debate about quantitative and qualitative research: A question of method or epistemology? British Journal of Sociology, 35, 78-92. Collins, R. (1984). Statistics versus words. In R. Collins (Ed.), Sociological theory (pp. 329-362). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Constas, M. A. (1992). Qualitative data analysis as a public event: The documentation of category development procedures. American Educational Research Journal, 29, 253-266. Cook, T. D., & Reichardt, C. S. (Eds.) (1979). Qualitative and quantitative methods in evaluation research. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Creswell, J. W. (1995). Research design: Qualitative and quantitative approaches. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Creswell, J. W. (1998). Qualitative inquiry and research design: Choosing among five traditions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Daft, R. L. (1983). Learning the craft of organizational research. Academy of Management Review, 8, 539-546. Denzin, N. K. (1970). The research act: A theoretical introduction to sociological methods. New York: Praeger. Dzurec, L. C., & Abraham, J. L. (1993). The nature of inquiry: Linking quantitative and qualitative research. Advances in Nursing Science, 16, 73-79. Elmore, P. B., & Woehlke, P. L. (1988). Statistical methods employed in American Educational Researcher and Review of Educational Research from 1978 to 1987. Educational Researcher, 17(9), 19-20. Gherardi, S., & Turner, B. A. (1987). Real men don't collect soft data. Quarderno 13, On Becoming a Pragmatic Researcher 21 Dipartimento di Politica Sociale, Università di Trento. Greene, J. C., Caracelli, V. J., & Graham, W. F. (1989). Toward a conceptual framework for mixed-method evaluation designs. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 11, 255-274. Gueulette, C., Newgent, R., & Newman, I. (1999, December). How much of qualitative research is really qualitative? Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Association for the Advancement of Educational Research (AAER), Ponte Vedra, Florida. Howe, K. R. (1988). Against the quantitative-qualitative incompatability thesis or dogmas die hard. Educational Researcher, 17, 10-16. Kaplan, A. (1964). The conduct of inquiry. San Francisco, CA: Chandler. Lincoln, Y. S., & Guba, E. G. (1985). Naturalistic inquiry. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Madey, D. L. (1982). Some benefits of integrating qualitative and quantitative methods in program evaluation, with some illustrations. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 4, 223-236. McLoughlin, Q. (1991). Relativistic naturalism: A cross-cultural approach to human science. New York: Praeger. Miles, M., & Huberman, M. (1984). Qualitative data analysis: An expanded sourcebook. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Miles, M., & Huberman, M. (1994). Qualitative data analysis: An expanded sourcebook (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Miller, S. I., & Fredericks, M. (1991). Uses of metaphor: A qualitative case study. Qualitative Studies in Education, 1(3), 263-272. Newman, I., & Benz, C. R. (1998). Qualitative-quantitative research methodology: Exploring the interactive continuum. Carbondale, Illinois: Southern Illinois University Press. 92 On Becoming a Pragmatic Researcher 22 Onwuegbuzie, A. J. (in press). Effect sizes in qualitative research: A prolegomenon. Quality & Quantity: International Journal of Methodology. Onwuegbuzie, A. J. (2002). Positivists, post-positivists, post-structuralists, and post-modernists: Why can't we all get along? Towards a framework for unifying research paradigms. Education, 122, 518-530. Onwuegbuzie, A. J., & Daniel, L.G. (2002). A framework for reporting and interpreting internal consistency reliability estimates. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 35, 89-103. Onwuegbuzie, A. J., DaRos, D. A., & Ryan, J. (1997). The components of statistics anxiety: A phenomenological study. Focus on Learning Problems in Mathematics, 19, 11-35. Onwuegbuzie, A. J., &Teddlie, C. (2002). A framework for analyzing data in mixed methods research. In A. Tashakkori & C. Teddlie (Eds.), Handbook of mixed methods in social and behavioral research (pp. 351-383). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Rist, R. (1977). On the relations among educational research paradigms: From disdain to detente. Anthropology and Educational Quarterly, 8, 42-49. Rossman, G.B., & Wilson, B.L. (1985). Numbers and words: Combining quantitative and qualitative methods in a single large-scale evaluation study. Evaluation Review, 9, 627-643. Sandelowski, M. (1986). The problem of rigor in qualitative research. Advances in Nursing Science, 8(3), 27-37. Sechrest, L., & Sidana, S. (1995). Quantitative and qualitative methods: Is there an alternative? Evaluation and Program Planning, 18, 77-87. Sieber, S. D. (1973). The integration of fieldwork and survey methods. American Journal of Sociology, 73, 1335-1359. 23 On Becoming a Pragmatic Researcher 23 Smith, J. K. (1983). Quantitative versus qualitative research: An attempt to clarify the issue. Educational Researcher, 12, 6-13. Smith, J. K., & Heshusius, L. (1986). Closing down the conversation: The end of the quantitative-qualitative debate among educational inquirers. Educational Researcher, 15, 4-13. Snizek, W. E. (1976). An empirical assessment of sociology: A multiple paradigm science. The American Sociologist, 11, 217-219. Tashakkori, A., & Teddlie, C. (1998). Mixed methodology: Combining qualitative and quantitative approaches. Applied Social Research Methods Series (Vol. 46). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Tashakkori, A., & C. Teddlie (Eds.) (2002), Handbook of mixed methods in social and behavioral research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Vidich, A. J., & Shapiro, G. (1955). A comparison of participant observation and survey data. American Sociological Review, 20, 28-33. Weinstein, E. A., & Tamur, A.J. (1978). Meaning, purposes, and structural resources in social interaction. In J.G. Manis & B.N. Meltzer (Eds.). Symbolic interaction (3rd ed., pp. 138-140). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon. Willems, E. P., & Raush, H. L. (1969). Naturalistic viewpoints in psychological research. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston. 24 On Becoming a Pragmatic Researcher 24 Figure /. Bi-dimensional representation of purist, situationalist, and pragmatist philosophies. Quantitative Paradigm (2) Quantitative Methods Qualitative Methods (4) (3) Qualitative Paradigm 25 BEST COPY AVA1ABLE U.S. Department of Education Office of Educational Research and Improvement (OER1) National Library of Education (NLE) Educational Resources Information Center (ERIC) E IC Educollonol Resources Inhumation Center REPRODUCTION RELEASE (Specific Document) TM035389 I. DOCUMENT IDENTIFICATION: p Title: UN.) "Z) r Author(s): Corporate Source: C A -11-( / iZt 17.1 Ni CV\ (4 if it'Lt;1 1 r ;,...N.117 Publication Date: cs L L irvi r , II. REPRODUCTION RELEASE: In order to disseminate as widely as possible timely and significant materials of interest to the educational community, documents announced in the monthly abstract journal of the ERIC system, Resources in Education (RIE), are usually made available to users in microfiche, reproduced paper copy, and electronic media, and sold through the ERIC Document Reproduction Service (EDRS). Credit is given to the source of each document, and, if reproduction release is granted, one of the following notices is affixed to the document. If permission is granted to reproduce and disseminate the identified document, please CHECK ONE of the following three options and sign at the bottom of the page. The sample sticker shown below will be affixed to all Level 1 documents The sample sticker shown below will be affixed to all Level 2A downents The sample sticker shown below will be &lured to all Level 20 documents PERMISSION TO REPRODUCE AND DISSEMINATE THIS MATERIAL IN MICROFICHE, AND IN ELECTRONIC MEDIA FOR ERIC COLLECTION SUBSCRIBERS ONLY, HAS BEEN GRANTED BY PERMISSION TO REPRODUCE AND DISSEMINATE THIS MATERIAL HAS BEEN GRANTED BY PERMISSION TO REPRODUCE AND DISSEMINATE THIS MATERIAL IN MICROFICHE ONLY HAS BEEN GRANTED BY c4tTO THE EDUCATIONAL RESOURCES INFORMATION CENTER (ERIC) TO THE EDUCATIONAL RESOURCES INFORMATION CENTER (ERIC) TO THE EDUCATIONAL RESOURCES INFORMATION CENTER (ERIC) 2A 1 2B Level 1 Level 2A Check Mire for Level 1 release, permitting reproduction and dissemination in microfiche or other ERIC archtval media (e.g., electronic) end paper copy. Check here for Level 2A release, permitting reproduction end dissemmation in microfiche and in electronic media for ERIC archival collection subscribers only V- Level 28 Check here for Level 28 release, permitting reproduction and dissemination in microfiche only Documents will be processed as imlicated provided reproduction quality Permits If permissOn to reproduce is granted, but no box is Checked, documents will be processed at Level 1 4 .2 re.) , Sign here, please " e ,:c1 I hereby grant to the Educational Resources Information Center (ERIC) nonexclusive permission to reproduce and disseminate this document as indicated above. Reproduction from the ERIC microfiche or electronic media by persons other than ERIC employees and its system contractors requires permission from the copyright holder. Exception is made for non-profit reproduction by libraries and other service agencies to satisfy information needs of educators in response to discrete inquiries. Spnature Organizationdddreu ; , Rnnted Nernells Montt fr. f& Ce-j Ad44.q L, 4 leff- Tilerre `6R FAX. E-Mail Address: (1 414.4 4., 6:7-4 1 iC Date: ; k4".t6 \t.1) ca.\ / ILLeir III. DOCUMENT AVAILABILITY INFORMATION (FROM NON-ERIC SOURCE): If permission to reproduce is not granted to ERIC, or, if you wish ERIC to cite the availability of the document from another source, please provide the following information regarding the availability of the document. (ERIC will not announce a document unless it is publicly available, and a dependable source can be specified. Contributors should also be aware that ERIC selection criteria are significantly more stringent for documents that cannot be made available through EDRS.) Publisher/Distributor: Address: Price: IV. REFERRAL OF ERIC TO COPYRIGHT/REPRODUCTION RIGHTS HOLDER: If the right to grant this reproduction release is held by someone other than the addressee, please provide the appropriate name and address: Name: Address: V. WHERE TO SEND THIS FORM: Send this form to the following ERIC Clearinghouse: However, if solicited by the ERIC Facility, or if making an unsolicited contribution to ERIC, return this form (and the document being contributed) to: ERIC Processing and Reference Facility 4483-A Forbes Boulevard Lanham, Maryland 20706 Telephone: Toll Free: FAX: e-mail: WWW: EFF-088 (Rev. 2/2003) 301-552-4200 800-799-3742 301-552-4700 info@ericfac.piccard.csc.com http://ericfacility.org
Purchase answer to see full attachment
User generated content is uploaded by users for the purposes of learning and should be used following Studypool's honor code & terms of service.

Explanation & Answer

HI, please find attached. I am here if you need me to make any changes or to offer clarification. Thanks!

Running Head: PTSD AND RECREATIONAL THERAPY

Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder on Veterans and Recreational Therapy
Student name:
Affiliated Institution:
Professor name:
Date

1

PTSD AND RECREATIONAL THERAPY

A Survey on Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder on veterans and the role of recreational
therapy
Please answer the questions using the numeric numbers.
A. Do you suff...


Anonymous
Really helped me to better understand my coursework. Super recommended.

Studypool
4.7
Trustpilot
4.5
Sitejabber
4.4

Related Tags