1
To appear in Partisan Canons, Anna Brzyski (ed.), Duke University Press.
Mere Exposure, Reproduction, and the Impressionist Canon
James Cutting
Every act of writing or curatorial practice, whenever it gets to the point of naming a name, is
participating in a certain level of canon formation, no matter what the intent of its author, no
matter whether it represents a challenge to the status quo or a confirmation of it.
Russell Ferguson1
It has been said that canons are the “legitimating backbone of cultural and political
identity.”2 They are also the bread and butter of what is taught in the humanities, and in art they
are what the general public flocks to see. Art museums and art historians feature canonical
images. They reproduce them in great quantity such that these images are now seen in greater
numbers, and by greater proportions, of people than ever before. Blockbuster exhibits guarantee
continuing links among particular images, publicity, and capital—both cultural and otherwise.
But what exactly are the contents of a given canon? How do we determine which works
are canonical? And how did they attain that status? In an attempt to answer these questions, I will
focus on the canon of French Impressionism, and argue that canons are cultural constructs
created, in part, through repeated reproduction and exposure. That is, following the epigraph of
Russell Ferguson, curator at the Museum of Contemporary Art, Los Angeles, I claim that art
historians have been complicitous in the formation and maintenance of the Impressionist canon,
and that over decades their wares have fed a sustaining public whose opinions, along with those
of art historians themselves, have crystallized and now drag heavily against systematic change.3
There are several reasons for choosing French Impressionism as a case study. First, it is a
relatively recent, well documented, and well defined art phenomenon. These facts make it easier
to outline its formation, maintenance, and structure. Second, it is sufficiently old that virtually
the entire corpus of works is known and accounted for. The artists collectively identified as the
Impressionists have been dead for about a century, and about thirty-five percent of their work is
currently owned by museums.4 The remainder is in private hands, but as I will demonstrate no
canonical images come those holdings.
Third, and perhaps the most significant reason for this study, is that French
Impressionism was extraordinarily popular across the twentieth century and continues to be so
today.5 The Impressionist images bequeathed to the State of France were gathered into the
Louvre by the mid-1930s and then sent to the Jeu de Paume in 1947.6 There, overcrowding soon
became a problem. In the 1960s through the mid-1980s the Musée du Jeu de Paume was the most
heavily trafficked museum per square meter in the world. Significantly, many of those visitors
were from the US. In 1986, the contents were then moved to the Musée d'Orsay, which was
instantly one of the most visited museums in the world, receiving over four million people
annually.7 To be sure, there are also well-known and frequently seen collections of French
Impressionist paintings elsewhere in Europe and, in particular, in the US. In addition, French
2
Impressionist paintings have often commanded the highest sale prices at art auctions throughout
the twentieth century, and over the course of the 1980s and 1990s they were featured in some of
the largest and best-attended exhibitions.8 Moreover, French Impressionism’s high public profile,
popularity, and importance for the history of modern art generated, over the course of the
twentieth century, a thick texture of literature on the artists and their oeuvres that I will draw
upon.
Finally, Impressionism is deeply embedded in contemporary American popular culture.
Consider two sources of supporting evidence. First, in his 1987 book, Cultural literacy: What
every American should know, Edward Hirsch listed thirty-one artists across all of time that he felt
US citizens should know. Five are Impressionists.9 Hirsch’s volume, however odd it may appear,
can be interpreted as an early and politically charged salvo in the “culture for dummies”
offensive. His “requirement” for Americans to learn about Impressionism was surely motivated
by its historic visibility in the US. Many of its most avid collectors were American, and those
collectors tended to give works to American museums. Indeed, five of the seven museums with
the largest, most diverse Impressionist collections are in the United States—the Metropolitan
Museum of Art in New York, the National Gallery of Art in Washington, the Philadelphia
Museum of Art, the Art Institute of Chicago, and the Museum of Fine Arts in Boston. The works
within their galleries—along with those in the Musée d’Orsay and the National Gallery of Art in
London—are icons of modernism and are deeply embedded in Western visual culture.10 They
even appear on towels, scarves, coasters, t-shirts, refrigerator magnets, and textbook covers.
Consider a second source—Impressionism as displayed within contemporary US mailorder catalogs. Among the hundreds of catalogs my household received in 2003, many had
images of rooms with furniture, lamps, and rugs for sale, but they also showed books on shelves,
coffee tables, and desks. These books were not for sale, but were included gratuitously to create
a particular ambience in each room, populating it with appropriate tokens of concretely
identifiable social aspirations. The presence of such books is not accidental. They are almost
always about food, travel, or art—three aspects of conspicuous American consumption. The
books on single artists are the most interesting. After taking care to exclude duplicate images, I
focused on seventeen catalogs. In those, I found books on just nineteen different painters,
excluding a few from the mid- and late-twentieth century. Van Gogh was the most common—six
different catalogs had different images of books with the title Van Gogh on its cover or spine;
Cézanne was second (5); next were Picasso (4), Rembrandt (4), Leonardo (3), Michelangelo (3),
Mondrian (2), and Sargent (2). Artists singly featured were Breugel, Cassatt, Duchamp, Gauguin,
Goya, Monet, Renoir, Seurat, Toulouse-Lautrec, Velázquez, Vermeer, and Whistler. French
Impressionists (4) and artists associated with broader late-nineteenth and early-twentieth century
French art (8 more) dominated in these portrayals of upscale American domesticity. Anecdotal as
it may be, such a survey clearly demonstrates that certain high art is part of popular culture. It is
linked to the commerce of goods catering to upwardly mobile, middle class tastes, where demand
for high quality is met and reinforced through association with canonical art.
Mere Exposure, Preference, and Value
Mere exposure is a key term in psychology, my own field of research. Robert Zajonc
discovered the phenomenon in late 1960s. Among other things, he demonstrated that the more
3
times a nonsense word like dilikli was repeated, the more likely a listener would later think it
meant something good rather than bad. Since then, laboratory studies with many kinds of
materials—unfamiliar graphic characters, nonsense geometric constructions, photographs of
unfamiliar people, as well as melodies and musical passages—have confirmed the validity of the
mere exposure effect.11 The basic finding is that, other things being equal, we tend to like things
we have seen before, indeed we tend to value them more.12 My own research has shown that this
also holds true for art. Repeated exposure to particular images creates and reinforces preferences.
I conducted a set of experiments on the effects of mere exposure to paintings and pastels.
I presented pairs of French Impressionist images to undergraduates (only 17 percent of whom
had previously taken an art history course), older adults (graduate students and faculty in the
Cornell psychology department), and children ages 6 to 10.13 I chose images widely, but they
were generally by Paul Cézanne, Edgar Degas, Edouard Manet, Claude Monet, Camille Pissarro,
Pierre-Auguste Renoir, and Alfred Sisley.14 I selected sixty-six pairs of paintings. The images in
each pair were by the same artist, produced roughly in the same year, and fell within the same
genre—portrait, group portrait, landscape, cityscape, seascape, still life, nude. Some images are
part of the Impressionist canon, as I will discuss later, but most are seldom reproduced and are
familiar only to the specialist.
Because I was interested in mere exposure, I needed a gauge for how likely and often
individuals might have seen each image before. The one I chose was indirect, but I think
compelling: I counted every occurrence of each of the 132 images in all the books that Cornell
University owns.15 I found these images in almost a thousand different books, published between
1901 and 2002, mostly in English and French, some in German, and a few in Italian, Dutch,
Spanish, Danish, and Japanese. Results of those tallies ranged from two reproductions (one of
which was always from a catalogue raisonné) to almost three hundred (an image included in
over a quarter of the books). I do not presume that any of my subjects spent time thumbing
through the art books in Cornell’s libraries, but I do assume that the relative reproduction
frequency of images well approximates the relative likelihood of their having been seen before.
I asked observers to indicate their preference between the two images. The result,
consistent with the mere exposure thesis, was that individuals preferred the more frequently
reproduced image in each pair about sixty percent of the time. Moreover, the greater the
discrepancy in relative frequency of reproduction, the more likely viewers preferred the more
reproduced image.16 Interestingly, few participants in this study recognized particular paintings,
and recognition was unrelated to preferences. This latter result was not a surprise. The effects of
mere exposure are not a rational response to one’s surroundings. Research has shown we often
cannot express the reasons for what we like, but all evidence here points to the fact that what we
prefer, we have likely seen before and seen more often.17
There is, of course, the thorny issue of quality. One might claim that paintings that are
reproduced more often are “better” paintings and that people respond to quality in art. This latter
idea, associated with Kantian aesthetics, was occasionally embraced by modernist art history,
but it is not likely to be true.18 In another experiment I presented the same images to students in
my undergraduate course on perception. I randomly interspersed them among the slides in my
PowerPoint presentations on various scientific topics. I presented them as nonsequitors for a few
4
seconds, each without comment. Across two dozen lectures I showed my class four times the
images that were less frequently reproduced in each pair. I presented the more frequently
reproduced images only once. At the end of the semester, I performed the same experiment with
this class and found that I had reversed preferences. The students now slightly preferred the
images that they had seen more often in class.19
Which Images Are in the Impressionist Canon?
Let me offer a thesis contrary to that of Walter Benjamin in “The Work of Art in the Age
of Mechanical Reproduction.” Canonical images—the images that hold the greatest prestige and
that are considered to be the most significant, and therefore most valued by experts and the
public alike—are those images that have been reproduced most often.20 That is, the aura of art in
the age of reproduction does not wither but, as Michael Camille also pointed out, is reinforced.21
To determine which images are canonical for French Impressionism, I consulted a second
sample, which included all books on Impressionism in the Cornell University libraries—a total
of nearly one hundred publications covering the breadth of the twentieth century. “As Harry
Abrams once said … about such publications, nobody reads the text anyway; it’s all about the …
reproductions.”22 Prompted by the epigraph of Russell Ferguson, I tabulated all images by title
and by artist.23 But first, how many French Impressionist paintings are there?
If one counts images in catalogues raisonnés of Cézanne, Degas, Manet, Monet, Pissarro,
Renoir, and Sisley, the answer is about 9,000. If one includes others—Frédéric Bazille, Gustave
Caillebotte, Mary Cassatt, Eva Gonzalès, Armand Guillaumin, and Berthe Morisot—the answer
is closer to 12,000.24 How many of these 12,000 appeared in the books included in my sample?
The answer is just over 2500. More interestingly, about 1400 images appeared only once, about
975 between two and nine times, and only 138 appeared ten times or more. Figure 1, which
presents these data in a graphic form, reveals a veritable iceberg of Impressionism. It has a vast
number of unreproduced images at the bottom, floating beneath the surface of visibility. The
uppermost spire represents the most frequently reproduced images—the core canon of
Impressionism. Notice that it is miniscule in proportion to the entire Impressionist corpus. The
spire also demonstrates the extreme consensus with which scholars reproduce images to tell the
story of Impressionism. For the purpose of further analysis, I will define the core canon of
French Impressionism as the fifty most frequently reproduced images.25
Who Originally Owned the Core Canon?
The bulk of the core canon was owned by a very small number of people. The most
important of these was Gustave Caillebotte, whose story has been told many times.26 Inheriting
his father’s wealth, Caillebotte was a millionaire and a painter at age 28. He was invited by
Renoir and by Henri Rouart to join the second Impressionist exhibition. He participated in,
organized, and bankrolled it and four others. More importantly here, he also bought his friends’
paintings. In 1894 Caillebotte died suddenly of stroke, and left his collection to the French state.
His only condition was that the collection should be exhibited intact. Haggling went on for years
taxing Renoir, the executor of Caillebotte’s will. Eventually thirty-nine artworks were accepted,
plus two of Caillebotte’s own paintings donated by the family. This part of his collection was
installed in the Musée du Luxembourg in Paris in 1897. Strikingly, Caillebotte owned eight
images now in the core canon, all in the Musée d’Orsay—Degas’ Femmes à la terrasse d’un
5
Figure 1: A graphical depiction of all of the images of Impressionism by their frequency of
appearance in ninety-five books, a veritable iceberg.
café, le soir (1877), Manet’s Le balcon (1869), Monet’s Régates à Argenteuil (1872) and his La
gare Saint-Lazare (1877), Pissarro’s Toits rouges (1877) and his Printemps à Pontoise, potager
et arbres en fleurs (1879), and Renoir’s Bal au Moulin de la Galette (1876) and his Balançoire
(1876).27
Only one other collector comes close to Caillebotte, the financier Isaac de Camondo.
Camondo was a leading banker in fin de siècle France and a quiet member of an important
Turkish-immigrant family. He began buying Impressionist works in 1893 and collected until his
death in 1911. By his will, its endowment, and his considerable political clout, his collection
went straight to the Louvre.28 Camondo owned six core canon images, all now in the
Orsay—Cézanne’s La maison du pendu (1873), Degas’ Absinthe (1876) and his Repasseuses
(1884), Manet’s Le fifre (1866), the most popular of Monet’s Cathédrales de Rouen series (1891,
he owned four), and the most popular of Sisley’s l’Inondation à Port-Marly series (1876, he
owned two).29
In England two collectors and benefactors promoted Impressionism. The first was Hugh
Lane, an Irishman and successful London art dealer. In 1908 he established Municipal Gallery of
Modern Art in Dublin, now known as the Hugh Lane Gallery. Lane had a small but remarkably
important Impressionist collection. He died on board the Lusitania in 1915, and by his official
will his collection was given to the National Gallery of Art. But in an unwitnessed codicil Lane
stated that the paintings should go to the new gallery in Dublin. The court did not honor the
amendment and the paintings went to London. Years of furor and negotiation followed. Finally,
an agreement was reached in 1959 and, at least ostensibly, the works are now shared between the
two museums. Three Lane images are in the core canon—Manet’s La musique aux Tuileries
(1862), Renoir’s Les parapluies (1881-1886), and Morisot’s Eté (1879)—but several more are
not far behind.30
The other English patron was Samuel Courtauld, the silk magnate. He is important here
for two reasons. First, impressed by Lane’s collection, he established a fund for the National
6
Gallery and the Tate to purchase modern works, and he oversaw that fund. Between 1923 and
1926, the Courtauld Fund purchased ten Impressionist paintings, and other more recent works.
Second, he also established his own museum in what is now the Courtauld Institute. After the
death of his wife in 1931 he gave his paintings, the lease to his house, and an endowment to the
University of London.31 Combining those purchased through his fund and those from his private
collection, Courtauld accounts for three images in the core canon—Manet’s Le bar aux FoliesBergère (1881, Courtauld Institute), Renoir’s La loge (1874, Courtauld Institute), and Monet’s
La plage à Trouville (1870, National Gallery, London).
Finally, the Havemeyers were the most important US benefactors of Impressionism—
with bequests to the Metropolitan Museum of Art and, through their children, to other museums.
In particular, Louisine Elder Havemeyer was active in the women’s suffrage movement, a close
friend of Mary Cassatt, and intensely interested in Impressionism. The Havemeyers owned over
one hundred fifty Impressionist works, and more than sixty went to the Met. Three Havemeyer
paintings are in the core canon—Monet’s La Grenouillère (1869, Metropolitan), Manet’s En
bateau (1874, Metropolitan), and his Le chemin de fer (1873, National Gallery of Art,
Washington).32
So few hands controlled so much of the French Impressionist canon. Five collectors—
Gustave Caillebotte, Isaac de Camondo, Hugh Lane, Samuel Courtauld, and Louisine
Havemeyer—account for almost half of it. Interestingly, other collectors who left important
bequests to various museums—Etienne Moreau-Nélaton (mostly in 1906) to the state of France;
the Palmers to the Art Institute of Chicago (1922), Antonin Personnaz to the Louvre (1937); Paul
Gachet fils to the Louvre (through the 1950s); the Tysons to the Philadelphia Museum of Art
(1956); and Chester Dale (1963), Paul Mellon (1970-1999) and Ailsa Mellon Bruce (1970) to the
National Gallery Washington—together account for only three images in the core canon. These
are Manet’s Le déjeuner sur l’herbe (1863, Moreau-Nélaton bequest to the French state),
Cézanne’s pastiche, Une moderne Olympia (1872, Gachet bequest to the Louvre), and Renoir’s
Les grandes baigneuses (1887, Tyson bequest to the Philadelphia Museum of Art). Most of these
donations generally came later than those discussed earlier. In the case of Moreau-Nélaton’s
bequest, it languished for thirty years neither in the Luxembourg or the Louvre, and in the case
of the Palmers’ it simply achieved less publicity.33 Moreover, as one can see in Figure 2, the
accrual of canonical images in museums peaked early, in the decades of the 1910s and 1920s. As
Impressionism became accepted, even mainstream, fewer of the images given to museums would
achieve canonical status.
Who Sold the Core Canon?
Dealers handled fewer than half of these core images. This contrasts with the larger
corpus of 12,000 images, where nearly two-thirds were controlled at one time or another by
sixteen different dealers.34 Accounting for half of the core canon images are friends who bought
directly from the artists (like Caillebotte and Paul Gachet père), the families of the artists (like
Marc Bazille, nephew of Frédéric, for Atelier de l’artiste, rue Condamine (1869) and Réunion de
famille (1867); the De Gas family for La famille Bellelli, 1858-1861, held back from the fourth
Degas estate sale; and the Pontillon family of Morisot’s sister for Le berceau, 1872) and the
families of friends (like the Dihau family, featuring the oboist in Degas’ Orchestre à l’Opéra,
1870-71). A few others were direct museum purchases (Degas’ Portraits dans un bureau,
7
Figure 2: A graphical depiction of the museum accrual by decade of what would later be
the fifty Impressionist images in the core canon.
Nouvelle-Orléans, 1873, Musée des Beaux-Arts, Pau), or gifts by subscription (e.g. for Manet’s
Olympia, led by Monet and John Singer Sargent to support Manet’s widow and to keep the
painting from going to the US).
Thus, after one omits those paintings not handled by dealers, only half remain. Despite
this, one third of these fifty images—or two-thirds of those remaining—were handled by Paul
Durand-Ruel. From 1871 until his death in 1922 Durand-Ruel made his living by selling both the
works of Impressionists and the more acceptable Salon painters. For sales of works now in the
core canon, the next most important dealers were the Galeries Bernheim-Jeune, Paul Rosenberg,
Georges Petit, Ambrose Vollard, and Wildenstein in that order, but the sum of these five is only
half the total of Durand-Ruel.35
Perhaps Durand-Ruel had extraordinary taste, guiding his buyers to the best images and
allowing him to play a huge role in forging the Impressionist canon? This is possible, but not
likely. Without denying his importance for the dissemination of Impressionism,36 there is little
evidence that he specifically controlled which paintings would be canonical. Yes, he dealt with a
third of these fifty images, but he also dealt with one third of all the images produced by the
Impressionist artists. Over a period of fifty years he sold more than four thousand paintings by
thirteen Impressionist artists. By this account he was an enormously successful salesman for
Impressionism, but he had no special impact on its core canon.
Who Publicized The Core Canon?
The Impressionist canon grew incrementally. Beginning just after the turn into the
twentieth century, scholars began to reproducing images in the core canon in their books. In
order to determine the history of the Impressionist canon’s reproduction, I analyzed a third
sample of twenty five books published in French, English, and German between 1901 and 1949
8
Figure 3. A scatter plot of ninety-five books on Impressionism published between 1901 and
2001, and the number of images they included that are now in the core canon.
that reproduced any Impressionist images at all.37 On average, each book introduced one or two
new images that would later become canonical. But two books clearly stand out – Charles
Borgmeyer’s The Master Impressionists (1913),38 an otherwise minor work that introduced nine
new canonical images, all from the Caillebotte and Camondo legacies; and John Rewald’s
History of Impressionism (1946), which introduced ten. Moreover, Rewald’s perspicacity
extended beyond the presentation of new images. He also republished two dozen core images
that appeared in earlier books. This means that he published two-thirds of the core canon. Before
him even Borgmeyer had published only one third. Shown in Figure 3 are the numbers of now
core canon images that appeared in all ninety-five books. Notice the pattern—a gradual
coalescence of agreement about what is in the canon across the twentieth century, and Rewald
led the way. His accomplishment was not eclipsed in a single volume for thirty-five years.39
Clearly, Rewald played an extraordinary role in shaping the canon. What he published, several
generations of art historians who followed also published. Indeed, no Impressionist scholar of
last half-century could ignore what Rewald presented.
Has the Core Canon Changed?
Over the course of the twentieth century the discipline of art history may have changed as
much or more than any other. It began the twentieth century with biographies and
connoisseurship, it ended it with sophisticated social histories and theoretically driven analyses.
Did the reproduction of canonical images change as well? No, or at least very little. Analyses of
my databases show that eighty-five percent of the images that were in the core canon in the
books published in the first half of the century are still there today. Thus one could argue that
even though the arguments are relatively fresh in the field of art history, the images are not.
9
To be sure, some images are published a bit less often. Renoir’s Bal au Moulin de la
Galette was by far and away the most reproduced Impressionist image in the first half of the
twentieth century. It is now merely among the top four. And Degas’ Etoile, danseuse sur la
scène (1876-78, also from the Caillebotte bequest) was once among the top dozen images, but
has subsequently fallen out of the top fifty. Many other images are also now published more
often. For example, those of Bazille, Caillebotte, Cassatt, Gonzalès, and Morisot now appear
more frequently, but they are largely not in the core canon. But consider two other images whose
reproduction has burgeoned. Since it was given to the Musée Marmottan in 1957 Monet’s
Impression, soliel levant (1873, Donop de Monchy bequest) has become the most reproduced
Impressionist image, even outstripping in this context Manet’s Olympia (1863) and his Déjeuner
sur l’herbe (1863). This is in large part due to the retelling of the semi-apocryphal tale—first
popularized by John Rewald—of this image, the art critic Louis Leroy, his derisive review of the
first Impressionist exhibition, and his supposed naming of the group.40 In addition, as a
centennial purchase for the Met in 1967 Monet’s Terrasse à Sainte-Adresse (1867) has also leapt
into the core canon. Nonetheless, such images are exceptions. The Impressionist canon, perhaps
like all others, is relatively fossilized. Scholars continue to display images once owned by
Caillebotte, Camondo, Lane, Courtauld, and the Havemeyers, and they continue to follow
Rewald. Interestingly, the art promoter least encumbered by this hegemonic uniformity, showing
most often and most consistently noncanonical images, is Sister Wendy Beckett.41
Why Reproduction Matters?
Let me return to mere exposure. There is another group that plays an important role in
maintaining the canon —the general public, the “audience” of art. Members of the public may
acquire knowledge about the canon through overt study of individual paintings, but more broadly
and more importantly they are influenced by mere exposure. All other things being equal, the
more often they see or hear something—so long as this is distributed across time, rather than
massed in a small amount of time—the more they will tend to like it. This mostly unconscious
exposure shapes their preferences and, as noted by Patricia Mainardi, this “audience exerts a
profound influence on the kind of art history we produce.”42
Equally importantly, scholars are no different from the public in the effects of mere
exposure. From childhood through college and throughout adulthood, we are all exposed to
hundreds of thousands of images. Some are representations of art; others, as during a museum
visit, are the artworks themselves. We don’t remember each occurrence of each image, or where
we saw it. We often will not even recognize the image if we see it again, but its trace can
influence our future assessments. These are not overt cognitive responses on our part. They are
not directly related to the formal part of our education, but they are very much a part of our
general and higher education. The effects of mere exposure are quite automatic and independent
of what we pay attention to in our day to day activities. They accrue simply as the result of being
a member of a culture, experiencing cultural artifacts. If all other things are equal, the more
canonical images are published, the more likely everyone—the public as well as the
scholar—tends to like them. Walter Benjamin suggested that “reproduction of art changes the
reaction of the masses toward art.”43 Indeed, I would argue, it has effects far beyond, and in
opposite direction of, those he considered.
10
Endnotes
I thank Claudia Lazzaro for many discussions over several years on the topic of canons; and Anna Brzyski for her
unrelenting eye for editorial detail, her unsurpassed enthusiasm, and her patience with my own professional
deformities. This chapter is based, in part, on my monograph Impressionism and Its Canon (Lanham, MD:
University Press of America, 2006).
1
Russell Ferguson, “Can we still use the canon?” The Art Journal, 58 (1999), p.4.
2
Griselda Pollock, Differencing the Canon: Feminist Desire and the Writing of Art’s Histories (London: Routledge,
1999), p. 3.
3
This argument bears some resemblance to ideas in the sociology of art (see, for example, Janet Wolff, The Social
Production of Art (New York: New York University Press, 1981), except that it applies not simply to the
production of individual images, but to the relationships among all images in the canon as a whole. This approach
is also very much against the zeitgeist of work within Impressionist scholarship, which generally regards the
corpus of canonical images as given and not shaped by their own efforts, or those of collectors, curators, and
chance. Among the hundred books on Impressionism that I have surveyed, few authors mention what governed
their choice of images. Michael Howard, in his Encyclopedia of Impressionism (London: Carleton Press, 1997, p.
6) comes closest to what I present here: “The works reproduced in this book are among the best known and best
loved paintings … in the world and our appetite for them is boundless… These familiar images have kept their
power to enthrall … They are kept before our eyes by a flood of advertising campaigns.”
4
The basis of this assertion comes first from the number of images in museums at the time of a given artist’s
catalogue raisonnés, given in those volumes. This number is then modified by amount of time that has passed
since their publication. The modification is based on the fact that both Cézanne and Manet have had two
catalogues raisonné. With two, one can look at the accession rates into museums between those publications.
These relative rates are essentially the same for these two painters and, although different painters are
differentially popular, the rates can be applied to the thirteen Impressionists considered here—Bazille, Caillebotte,
Cassatt, Cézanne, Degas, Gonzalès, Guillaumin, Manet, Monet, Morisot, Pissarro, Renoir, and Sisley—and
extrapolated to 2005.
5
Robert Herbert, in his essay “Impressionism, originality, and laissez-faire” in his From Millet to Léger: Essays in
Social Art History (New Haven, CT: Yale University Press, pp. 91-97) expands on the many reasons for
Impressionism embedding itself in popular culture.
6
The Jeu de Paume was technically a part of the Louvre and not a separate museum, although 400 meters down the
Rue du Rivoli. Part of the rationale for this was that the will of Isaac de Camondo, whom I will discuss later,
insisted that his paintings be hung in the Louvre. See, for example, Germain Bazin, Impressionist Paintings in the
Louvre, S. Cunliffe-Owen, Trans. (London: Thames & Hudson, 1958).
7
For the volume of visitors to the Jeu de Paume and the Orsay, see Andrea Kupfer Schneider, Creating the Musée
d’Orsay: The Politics of Culture in France (University Park, PA: Pennsylvania State University Press, 1998) pp.
12 & 106.
8
On Impressionist art sales, see The Art Newspaper (February 2000, XI, No. 100, p. 61), which reported that six of
the twelve most expensive paintings sold at auction in 1999 were Impressionist works—three Cézannes, two
Monets, and a Degas. The Art Newspaper (September 2001, XI, No. 106, p. 70, Art Market) also commented on
the skyrocketing sales prices of Impressionist art over the decade of the 1990s. In addition, among the four most
expensive paintings ever sold (through 2002) one was a Cézanne and another a Renoir. Even more striking is the
tally of artists with most works sold at auction for over one million dollars through 2001: First was Picasso with
272, but the next five were Impressionists—Monet (218), Renoir (196), Degas (100), Cézanne (80), and Pissarro
(74) (Russell Ash, The Top 10 of Everything, New York: Dorling Kindersley, 2002). On art exhibitions, see The
Art Newspaper (February 2001, XII, No. 111, p. 20), which reported that 2000 was the first year since 1994 that
there wasn't an Impressionist exhibit in the top ten most frequented exhibitions worldwide. In 1999 there were
three in the top ten, and in 1998 there were two. See also, Patricia Mainardi, “Repetition and Novelty: Exhibitions
Telling Tales” in Charles W. Haxthausen (Ed.) The Two Art Histories (New Haven, CT: Yale University Press,
2002), pp. 81-86, on the force of Impressionism in museums; and see Gary Tinterow, “Blockbuster, Art History,
and the Public: The Case of Origins of Impressionism” in the same volume (pp. 143-153).
9
Edward D. Hirsch, Cultural Literacy: What Every American Needs to Know (Boston: Houghton-Mifflin, 1987). In
his appendix, which he compiled with collaborators Joseph Kett and James Trefil, he included Cassatt, Cézanne,
Degas, Manet, and Renoir, plus Gauguin (who also exhibited at four of the Impressionist exhibitions).
Interestingly, he did not include Monet.
11
10
The Philadelphia Museum of Art and the National Gallery London have just under one hundred Impressionist
paintings and pastels. The Musée Marmottan in Paris and the Barnes Foundation in Merion, Pennsylvania, have
more than this, but these collections are not diversified. The Marmottan has almost 100 Monets but few paintings
by other Impressionists, and the Barnes more than 60 Cézannes and more than 40 Renoirs, but again few by
others.
11
Robert Zajonc, “Attitudinal Effects of Mere Exposure,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9 (1968),
1-27. The very many studies on the phenomenon in its first two decades were then summarized by Robert
Bornstein, “Exposure and Affect: Overview and Meta-analysis of Research, 1968-1987,” Psychological Bulletin,
106 (1989) 265-289. For recent results with music, see Karl K. Szpunar, E. Glenn Schellenberg, and Patricia
Plinar, “Liking and Memory for Musical Stimuli as a Function of Exposure,” Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30 (2004), 370-381.
12
Two additional points should be made. First, for mere exposure to affect positively the evaluation of a given item
it should be initially perceived as at least neutral. Unpleasant items typically become more unpleasant with
exposure. Second, mere exposure works best either when one doesn’t pay much attention to each presentation, or
when the intervals between presentations are relatively long. Otherwise, overexposure and boredom can result.
13
These studies are reported in detail in James E. Cutting, “Gustave Caillebotte, French Impressionism, and Mere
Exposure,” Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10 (2003), 319-343.
14
I recognize that strictly speaking Manet was not an Impressionist, and that arguments against the inclusion of
Cézanne can be made as well. Nonetheless, both are very part of the Impressionist story.
15
Cornell University is a major research institution and its libraries hold more than six million volumes. It’s Fine
Arts collection is extremely extensive, and is likely to hold all of the major works within any artistic subdiscipline.
16
For example, images that were up to 40% more frequent than their mates were preferred 57% of the time, those
about twice as frequent were preferred 60% of the time, and about four times as frequent were preferred 63% of
the time.
17
See, for example, Richard E. Nisbett and Lee Ross, Human Inference: Strategies and Shortcomings of Social
Judgments (Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1980). Note also that children did not show the adult preference
pattern. To be sure, they enjoy art and have strong preferences, but those do not match rates of reproduction.
Children have simply not lived long enough in Western culture to benefit from the effects of mere exposure to art.
18
For statements outside art history concerning the perception of quality in art, see for example Immanuel Kant,
Critique of Aesthetic Judgment. J. Meredith, Trans. (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1952, original work
published in 1794); Robert Pirsig, Zen and the Art of Motorcycle Maintenance (New York: Morrow, 1974); and
Richard Feynman, “Surely You’re Joking, Mr. Feynman”: Adventures of a Curious Character (New York:
Norton, 1985); or for statements within art history, see for example Jakob Rosenberg, On Quality in Art: Criteria
of Excellence, Past and Present (Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press, 1967); and Susan Woodford, Looking
at Pictures (Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 1983). For counters to the notion of artistic quality see
Mika Bal and Norman Bryson, “Semiotics and Art History,” Art Bulletin, 73 (1991), 174-208; Mark A. Cheetham,
Kant, Art, and Art History (Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 2002); and Keith Moxey, The Practice
of Theory: Poststructuralism, Cultural Politics, and Art History (Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press 1994).
19
Viewers preferred the less frequently published images 52% of the time. In previous studies the same images in
the same pairs were preferred 40% of the time. The difference was statistically highly reliable.
20
Walter Benjamin, “The Work of Art in the Age of Mechanical Reproduction,” in his Illuminations, H. Zohn,
Trans. (New York: Schocken Books, 1968), pp. 217-251.
21
Michael Camille, “Rethinking the Canon,” The Art Bulletin, 78 (1996), 198-201.
22
Mainardi, pp. 83-84.
23
A somewhat similar analysis was done by Daniel W. Galenson, Painting Outside the Lines: Patterns of Creativity
in Modern Art (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 2001). Galenson, in part of his study, analyzes all of
the images that appear in general art history textbooks dealing with his topic. However, most of his work deals
with market forces on the sales of images by 125 modern painters and inferences that might be made about
creativity. Robert Jenkins, in his “Measuring canons: Reflections on innovations and the nineteenth-century canon
of European Art” (this volume), follows in this tradition.
24
The exact number of Impressionist images is not known, in part, because Renoir’s catalogue raisonné is
unfinished. Nonetheless, these totals are based on tallies from the other catalogues and an estimate for Renoir
based on his productivity in a comparable period to Cézanne, Degas, and Monet. The basis for claiming that these
seven painters were the ‘major’ Impressionists comes from the tally of thirty books on Impressionism published
throughout the twentieth century. Works by these seven artists were included in at least twenty-nine of them. Only
12
Cézanne and Sisley were missing from one each. In comparison works by Bazille appeared in nineteen,
Caillebotte fourteen, Cassatt twenty-one, and Morisot twenty-five.
25
A cutoff of 50 is completely arbitrary, but the same general patterns as those reported here recur at 25, 100, or
even 350 images.
26
For histories, see Marie Bérhaut, Gustave Caillebotte: Catalogue Raisonné des Peintures et Pastels (Paris: La
Bibliothèque des Arts, 1994); Michael Marrinan, “Caillebotte as Professional Painter: From Studio to the Public
Eye,” in Norma Broude, Ed., Gustave Caillebotte and the Fashioning of Identity in Impressionist Paris (New
Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press, 2002), pp. 21-65; Philip Nord, Impressionists and Politics (London:
Routledge, 2000); and Kirk Varnedoe, Gustave Caillebotte (New Haven, CT: Yale University Press, 1987).
27
Except where otherwise indicated, all paintings are currently in the Musée d’Orsay.
28
On Camondo, see G. Migeon, P. Jamot, R. Vitry, & C. Dreyfus, La Collection Isaac de Camondo au Musée du
Louvre (Paris: Gazette des Beaux-Arts, 1914). Camondo was also a friend of Georges Clemenceau, and his
bequest made Degas, Monet, and Renoir the only living artists ever to have works in the Louvre.
29
The full title of the particular Rouen Cathedral image is La cathédrale de Rouen, le portail et la tour SaintRomain, plein soleil, harmonie bleue et or.
30
On Lane, see http://www.hughlane.ie/about/hugh.shtml. Since 1979 Manet’s Portrait d’Eva Gonzalès (1870),
Morisot’s Eté (1979), and Renoir’s Parapluies (1881-1886) have been in Dublin, but Degas’ Bain de mer. Petite
fille peignée par sa bonne (1868-1869) and Manet’s La musique aux Tuileries (1862) have stayed in London.
31
For data on the Courtauld fund see the website of the National Gallery of Art, London:
http://www.nationalgallery.uk.org . On the Courtauld institute, see John Murdock, The Courtauld Gallery at
Somerset House (London: Thames and Hudson, 1998). Also, the collections of the National Gallery and the Tate
were shuffled in 1996, with all Impressionist images going the National Gallery, the Tate afterwards being
reserved for British works. Courtauld images just missing this list include Degas’ Mlle La La au cirque Fernando
(1879, National Gallery London) and Manet’s La serveuse de Bocks (1878-1879, National Gallery London).
32
On Louisine Havemeyer, see Alice C. Frelinghuysen, Gary Tinterow, S. A. Stein, G. Wold, and J. Meech,
Splendid Legacy: The Havemeyer Collection (New York: The Metropolitan Museum of Art, 1993).
33
The most reproduced of the Palmer bequest is Monet’s Au bords de l’eau, Bennecourt (1868, Art Institute of
Chicago); that of Personnaz is the most popular of Monet’s several Pont d’Argenteuil (1874, Musée d’Orsay); that
of Dale is Morisot’s Dans la salle à manger (1886, National Gallery, Washington); that of Mellon is Manet’s La
prune (1877, National Gallery, Washington); and that of Mellon Bruce is Renoir’s Le pont neuf (1872, National
Gallery, Washington). But again, none of these is in the most frequent fifty. Two earlier bequests to the French
State with core canon images were those of Marc Bazille in 1904, Frédéric’s nephew (Atelier de l’artiste, rue
Condamine,1869, and Réunion de famille, 1867), and Etienne Moreau-Nélaton in 1906 (for Manet’s Le déjeuner
sur l’herbe, 1863).
34
This analysis is based on the study of the catalogues raisonnés of the Impressionist painters. In addition, see
Harrison White and Cynthia A. White, Canvases and Careers: Institutional Change in the French Painting World
(Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1993, 2nd ed) for an account of the role of dealers in the changing art
world of the mid-late nineteenth century.
35
Again, the calculations are based on going through the catalogues raisonné of each artist, or museum catalogues,
checking for provenance.
36
See Pierre Assouline, Grâces lui soient rendues, Durand-Ruel le marchand des Impressionistes (Paris: Plon,
2002).
37
The list was compiled largely based on an appendix in John Rewald, The History of Impressionism (New York:
Museum of Modern Art, 1946).
38
Charles Borgmeyer, The Master Impressionists (Chicago: Arts Press, 1913).
39
That book was Diane Kelder’s The Great Book of French Impressionism (New York: Abbeville, 1981).
40
The story of Louis Leroy’s naming of Impressionism stems from a widely cited account that Monet gave in an
interview in 1900. For an important revisiting of this account see Jane Mayo Roos, Early Impressionism and the
French State 1866-1874 (New York: Cambridge University Press, 1996).
41
See Wendy Beckett and Patricia Wright, The Story of Painting (London: Dorling-Kindersley, 1994), and Wendy
Beckett and Patricia Wright, Sister Wendy’s 1000 Masterpieces (London: Dorling-Kindersley, 1999).
42
Mainardi, p. 81.
43
Benjamin, p. 234.
Purchase answer to see full
attachment