Energy Conservation Nudges And Environmentalist Ideology Report Paper

User Generated

kvatlhaznb1997

Economics

Description

Read the PDF and write a 1.5 pages report, single space, avoid plagiarism.

Requirement is the picture.

Unformatted Attachment Preview

Dora L. Costa Matthew E. Kahn University of California, Los Angeles University of California, Los Angeles Abstract “Nudges” are being widely promoted to encourage energy conservation. We show that the popular electricity conservation “nudge” of providing feedback to households on own and peers’ home electricity usage in a home electricity report is two to four times more effective with political liberals than with conservatives. Political conservatives are more likely than liberals to opt out of receiving the home electricity report and to report disliking the report. Our results suggest that energy conservation nudges need to be targeted to be most effective. (JEL: Q41, D03, D72) 1. Introduction Europe, especially Scandinavia, has high taxes on electricity and gasoline to encourage conservation and counter global warming. Taxes in Denmark represent more than half of the cost of electricity to consumers.1 In contrast, the United States has low taxes and little political will to sacrifice for the sake of conservation. Congressional voting patterns highlight that conservative Representatives are highly unlikely to vote for carbon mitigation legislation (Cragg et al. 2013). Facing political gridlock in the Congress and concerned about the challenge of climate change, an ongoing policy agenda is seeking out alternative strategies for encouraging conservation. Recent psychology research suggests an alternative tool for changing household behavior is to focus on well crafted messages offering peer comparisons (see Griskevicius, Cialdini, and Goldstein 2008). Robert Cialdini and his coauthors have conducted a series of field experiments that have demonstrated that lowcost persuasion strategies or “nudges” can change an individual’s behavior by making The editor in charge of this paper was Stefano DellaVigna. Acknowledgments: We thank Maximilian Auffhammer, the participants at the 2010 POWER Conference, and seminar participants at Princeton and the University of Illinois for comments. We thank the UCLA Ziman Real Estate Center for funding. We thank the editor and five reviewers for their comments. Costa and Kahn are Research Associates at NBER. E-mail: costa@econ.ucla.edu (Costa); mkahn@ioe.ucla.edu (Kahn) 1. See Eurostat News Release, 75/2010, 28 May 2010. http://epp.eurostat.ec.europa.eu/cache/ITY_ PUBLIC/8-28052010-AP/EN/8-28052010-AP-EN.PDF Journal of the European Economic Association June 2013 c 2013 by the European Economic Association  11(3):680–702 DOI: 10.1111/jeea.12011 Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019 ENERGY CONSERVATION “NUDGES” AND ENVIRONMENTALIST IDEOLOGY: EVIDENCE FROM A RANDOMIZED RESIDENTIAL ELECTRICITY FIELD EXPERIMENT Costa and Kahn Energy Conservation “Nudges” and Environmentalist Ideology 681 2. In the United Kingdom, where electricity taxes are low, David Cameron has touted the behavioral transformations of putting “the typical electricity bill for a house like theirs in a neighborhood like theirs” in front of households. Speech of 13 June, 2008. http://www.aletmanski.com/files/davidcameron-powerofsocialinnovation.doc Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019 him aware of the actions of others who have been in a similar situation (Goldstein et al. 2008; Schultz et al. 2007). Nudges may be a low cost strategy for encouraging energy conservation (Allcott and Mullainathan 2010; Thaler and Sunstein 2008).2 We posit that liberal/environmentalists are more likely to respond to energy conservation nudges. A series of recent empirical papers has documented that environmentalists are more likely to engage in “voluntary restraint” than the average person (Kotchen and Moore 2008). Those who vote in favor of “green policies” and register for liberal/environmentalist political parties are more likely to have a smaller carbon footprint and to purchase green products such as the Toyota Prius (Kahn 2007, Kahn and Morris 2009). Such environmentalists consciously avoid free riding and voluntarily restrain their consumption of goods and services that generate a negative externality. Our evidence on the role of ideology in energy conservation “nudges” comes from a randomized field experiment carried out by a western utility district in which we can observe a household’s ideology (an unobservable in prior studies), its socioeconomic and demographic characteristics, and its behavioral responses. Starting in Spring 2008, this utility has been sending households in the treatment group a Home Energy Report (HER). The report provides household specific information on own monthly electricity usage over time and relative to neighbors’ usage over the same time period. The report provides energy saving tips. To examine the role that political ideology and environmentalism play in determining how randomly selected households respond to these reports, we have collected data on the customer’s political party of registration, household donations to environmental organizations and household participation in renewable energy programs, and data on the characteristics of the local residential communities where the households live. Households who are registered in liberal political parties and who live in residential communities with a large liberal share and who have previously signed up for energy from renewable resources and donate to environmental causes are arguably environmentalists. Our focus on ideology, an unobservable in previous studies, distinguishes our work from other research (e.g. Allcott 2011; Ayres, Raseman, and Shih 2009). We find that the effectiveness of energy conservation “nudges” depends on an individual’s ideology. In the United States, Democrat, Peace and Freedom, and Green party members (liberals in the US terminology) are more likely to vote for environmentalist causes than Republican, American Party, or Libertarian party members (conservatives in the US terminology). We measure ideology not just with registered political party, but also with indicators of living in a liberal or conservative community and willingness to pay for energy generated from renewable resources and to donate to environmental organizations. Although liberals and environmentalists are more energy efficient than conservatives (Costa and Kahn 2010), thus making it harder 682 Journal of the European Economic Association 2. The Energy Conservation “Nudge” The “nudge” that the electric utility company sends to treatment households in an ongoing randomized experiment to encourage reductions in electricity consumption is a two-page HER (see the Appendix for a sample). Similar reports have been used by other utilities in the United States. The front page compares the electricity consumption of the household with all neighbors with similar size homes and heat type and with neighbors who are in the bottom 20th percentile of electricity usage. The back page compares the household’s electricity usage in the current month relative to the same time month in the prior year and awards green stars in every month the household consumed less relative to the same month in the past year (panel not shown in the Appendix because it is not publicly available). It also provides three tips for saving energy, such as turning down the thermostat when using an electric blanket or purchasing an Energy Star durable, and indicates the dollar amount in energy savings per year (shown in the Appendix). Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019 for them to reduce consumption further, we find that liberals and environmentalists are more responsive to these nudges than conservatives. We find that among political liberals who purchase electricity from renewable resources, who donate to environmental causes, and who live in a census block group where the share of liberals is in the top 75th percentile, receiving a HER led to reductions in electricity usage of 3.6%. In contrast, among political conservatives who do not pay for renewable electricity, who do not donate to environmental groups, and who live in a census block group where the share of liberals is in the bottom 25th percentile, receiving a HER led to reductions in electricity usage of 1.1%. Liberals are more likely to turn down the air-conditioning in the summer in response to the HER report. Political liberals were 15% less likely to opt out of receiving the report and, in a survey, political liberals are also less likely than conservatives to state that the reports were useless and to report disliking them. By documenting the role that ideology plays in determining the effectiveness of a specific “nudge”, this paper contributes to the growing literature on the consequences of political ideology. Much of this work has focused on the role of political ideology in shaping preferences for redistribution (e.g. Piketty 1995). Our work focuses on the role of political ideology in shaping responses to non-market mechanisms designed to reduce consumption. Recent work on the determinants of political ideology has examined the causal role of the media (DellaVigna and Kaplan 2007), property rights (Di Tella, Galiani, and Schargrodsky 2007), and historical circumstances (Alesina and Fuchs-Schundeln 2007; Giuliano and Spilimbergo 2009) in shaping a person’s ideological outlook. Both social psychologists and economists have argued that beliefs on how society and the economy work predominately are formed at ages 18–25 (see Giuliano and Spilimbergo 2009). In this study, we will take as given that a household either is or is not a liberal/environmentalist and we will study how these political and social views influence household response to the same randomized treatment. Costa and Kahn Energy Conservation “Nudges” and Environmentalist Ideology 683 2.1. The HER Experiment Between March 14 and May 9 2008, the electric utility sent the first Home Electricity Reports to a treatment group of approximately 35,000 households. By April 1, 43% of all treatment households had received the report and by April 15 the figure was 62%. Households are still receiving the report, either on a quarterly or monthly basis. A control group of roughly 49,000 households have never received a HER. The HER experiment selected households from 85 census tracts with a high density of single-family homes (see ADM Associates 2009). Both treatment and control households had to have a current account with the electric utility that had been active for at least one year, could not be living in apartment buildings, and had to be living in a house with square footage between 250 and 99,998 square feet. Groups of contiguous census blocks were randomly assigned to either the treatment or control group. A “block batch” of five contiguous census blocks was randomly assigned to the treatment group and then a contiguous census block batch was assigned to the control group. The process continued until roughly 35,000 households were assigned to both the treatment and control groups. The remaining census blocks (about 14,000 homes) were assigned to the control group. Contiguous block groups were used because the implementation contractor, Positive Energy (now OPOWER), believed that increased communication among people receiving the HERs in the same community would lead to greater energy savings.4 Allcott (2011), Ayers, Raseman, and Shih (2009), and Schultz et al. (2007) found that providing feedback to customers on home electricity and natural gas usage with a focus on peer comparisons decreased consumption by 1% to 2%, potentially saving 110 million kWh per year if feedback were provided to all of the utility’s customers (Ayers, Raseman, and Shih 2009). Additional evidence that social incentives can make a difference comes from California’s 2001 media campaign to promote voluntary conservation after rolling blackouts in 2000 and early 2001. Consumption in San Diego 3. We thank a reviewer for suggesting this research strategy. 4. In a 2009 Home Energy Use Survey conducted by the electric utility, households in the control group were more likely to report talking to friends and neighbors about their electricity bill than households in the treatment group, suggesting that receiving the HER did not inspire discussion and that any positive peer effects operate through implicit social pressure. Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019 Each report contains two pieces of information: the household’s absolute level of consumption and how its consumption compares with that of 100 neighbors living in similar-sized homes. We also know whether a household in the treatment group received a message of “great”, “good”, or “room for improvement” in the first report. As we discuss in the Online Appendix, we use this information to implement a regression discontinuity design to test whether the change in electricity consumption for the treatment group differs depending on the normative message that the household receives in the first report.3 684 Journal of the European Economic Association declined by 7% during the initial two phases of the campaign, before rebounding (Reiss and White 2008). Within a household production framework, a household values electricity as an input in producing comfort (e.g. indoor temperature) and leisure and household production activities. Total household electricity consumption in any given period is the sum of electricity used in each of these activities. A household’s total electricity consumption depends on choices over (1) the attributes of the house, such as size; (2) the attributes of appliances; and (3) the intensity of utilization of appliances for leisure and household activities, indoor temperature control and illumination. These choices, in turn, depend on climate, prices and personal attributes, including ideology. We view environmental ideology as a set of prior beliefs including those about the importance of energy conservation The ideological divide on environmental issues between Democrats and Republicans could affect how a household responds to an energy conservation “nudge”. In the United States, conservatives consistently oppose environmental regulation and energy policies intended to further environmental aims, as seen in polling data on the belief in climate change (Dunlap and McCright 2008) and Congressional voting patterns (Cragg et al. 2013). Dunlap and McCright (2008) report that in 2008 there was 34 percentage point gap between Democrats and Republicans in their agreement with a statement that the effects of global warming have already begun, up from a four percentage point gap in 1997. The 2008 National Environmental Scorecard of the League of Conservation Voters gives the House Democratic leadership a score of 95 (out of a best score of 100) and the Republican leadership a score of 3.5 A 2009 Pew survey found a 23 percentage gap between Democrat and Republican agreement with the statement that people should be willing to pay higher prices to protect the environment. Republicans and Democrats respond differently to “carbon offsets” versus “carbon tax” (Hardistry, Johnson, and Weber 2010), suggesting that ideology moderates how individuals think of key words. European studies have highlighted the role that environmental ideology plays in determining the willingness to take voluntary actions to mitigate one’s carbon externality and the willingness to pay to purchase a “green product”. Thalmann (2004) and Halbheer, Niggli, and Schmutzler (2006) document that voters who are left-ofcenter or who are environmentalists were more likely to vote for Swiss environmental referenda. Brounen and Kok (2011) found that the price premium for residential homes that are certified as highly energy efficient in Holland is higher in “green” communities, that is communities where the Green Party and the Party for the Animals had received a larger fraction of the vote. Given this background, there are two main hypotheses that can be tested. Many households will read the report and respond to it for non-ideological reasons, such 5. See http://www.people-press.org/2009/05/21/section-9-the-environment-and-the-economy/ Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019 3. Why Could Ideology Mediate the Response to this Nudge? Costa and Kahn Energy Conservation “Nudges” and Environmentalist Ideology 685 4. Data Our primary data set consists of residential billing data from January 2007 to October 2009. These data provide us with information on kilowatt hours purchased per billing cycle, the length of the billing cycle (measured in days), whether the house uses electric heat, and whether the household is enrolled in the electric utility’s program to purchase energy from renewable sources. We link each billing cycle to the mean temperature in that billing cycle.6 We link the billing data to the treatment and control data which contain information on when the household began to receive the HERs, as well as information on square footage of the house, information on whether the home heats with electricity or natural gas, and the age of the house. We cannot match 1,976 observations in the pilot and control data to the residential billing data. In our final data set, the treatment and control data therefore contain 81,722, with 48,058 households in the control group. Among the households in the treatment group, 24,028 received a monthly report and 9,636 received a quarterly report. We merge individual voter registration and marketing data for March 2009 to our data set.7 For registered voters we know party affiliation, and whether the individual donates to environmental organizations. We were able to link half of our sample to the voter registration data. We linked either the person whose name was on the utility bill or the first person on the utility bill.8 The individuals we could not link were living in smaller households and in census block groups with a low proportion of the college-educated, were more likely to receive a subsidy for electricity because of their 6. Two different households in the same calendar year and same month who are on different billing cycles will face different climate conditions. 7. We purchased the data from www.aristotle.com. 8. Only 5% of households were “mixed” between conservatives and liberals. Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019 as wanting to lower their bill. These households, regardless of ideology, may reduce their consumption. The response of liberals is ambiguous. Liberals may reduce their consumption by more than conservatives because of their ideology and have been observed to consume less electricity (Kotchen and Moore 2008; Costa and Kahn 2010). However, because they have already invested more time and effort in monitoring their electricity bills and in engaging in voluntary restraint (i.e. lowering the air-conditioner in the summer), their response could be lower than that of conservatives. Secondly, and more controversially, it is possible that anti-environmentalist conservatives who receive a green looking report and learn that they consume more than their peers may refuse to decrease their consumption or even increase their consumption in an act of defiance. People find information more reliable when it conforms to their strong prior beliefs (e.g. Lord, Ross, and Leper 1979; Miller et al. 1993; Munro and Ditto 1997; Gentzkow and Shapiro 2006, 2010) and are influenced mainly by those in their network (Murphy and Shleifer 2004). 686 Journal of the European Economic Association 9. Relative to all homeowners in the same county these individuals were also more likely to be of Asian or other ancestry rather than of European ancestry, but were less likely to be Spanish speaking. They were also lower income. 10. The collected revenue is used by the electric utility to purchase and produce power from wind, water, and Sun. 11. Household income is available from credit bureau data. Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019 low income, and were more likely to have a household head above age 60.9 We also merge to these data, by the census block group, the share of registered voters who were liberal (Democrat, Green, or Peace and Freedom) in 2000 and the share of the college-educated in the block group. We expect that environmentalists are more likely to live in liberal, educated communities (Kahn 2007). We have access to two revealed preference measures of a household’s environmentalism. From the data base with voter registration information, we know whether a household has donated money to an environmental group. We also know whether the household has signed up for the company’s renewable power program prior to the treatment. This is the electric utility’s major program to increase the share of its customers who have signed up for renewable energy. Each household decides whether to opt in and pay a fixed cost of $3 a month to have 50% of its power generated by renewables or $6 a month to have 100% of its power generated by renewables.10 We also have access to an ancillary data set which we use to examine household attitudes about the HER by ideology. In 2009 the electric utility company surveyed 1,375 households who received the HER, asking them questions about the HER report. We restrict this sample to households for whom we have information on age and the fraction of liberals in the block group and to households who were not in minor parties we could not classify as liberal or conservatives. This leaves us with 1,061 observations in this ancillary data set. Table 1 shows that the treatment and control groups are roughly representative of all homeowners in the county in terms of household and neighborhood characteristics. But, there are some clear differences. The treatment and control groups consume roughly 10% more electricity than the average county homeowner as of 2007 (before the experiment). Relative to the average homeowner, the experiment homes are older and more likely to be electric homes. The households in the experiment group are roughly 10% richer than the average county homeowner.11 The geographical areas included in the experiment have a higher share of college graduates than the average county home owner’s community. The randomization of the HER across blocks was effective. Ayers, Raseman, and Shih (2009) reported that controlling for house characteristics, household demographics, and the number of cooling degree days and heating degree days, there was no systematic difference in energy usage between treatment and control groups prior to the treatment (also see Table 1 where we report no systematic differences between treatment and control groups adjusting for block batch group correlation). 17.710 1.159 14.967 43,252.950 682.611 20.619 0.162 0.105 0.492 0.498 0.330 0.045 0.499 0.275 0.371 0.283 27.930 2.111 56.582 66,484.710 1,709.447 1,976.764 0.283 0.460 0.412 0.461 0.124 0.002 0.474 0.082 0.165 0.088 285,717 S.D. 0.447 0.439 0.112 0.002 0.425 0.099 0.246 0.104 48,058 31.051 2.111 56.941 74,826.920 1,720.876 1,971.176 0.364 0.436 Mean S.D. 0.497 0.496 0.316 0.046 0.494 0.299 0.431 0.305 15.473 1.136 14.952 41,364.120 602.081 18.377 0.158 0.098 Control Group 0.438 0.449 0.112 0.002 0.430 0.097 0.264 0.103 33,664 30.801 2.103 56.594 74,312.590 1706.109 1972.618 0.363 0.438 Mean 0.496 0.497 0.315 0.043 0.495 0.296 0.441 0.304 14.727 1.137 15.085 41,546.370 578.287 18.547 0.162 0.097 S.D. Treatment Group Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019 Note: All variables listed after the block group variables are dummy variables. The treatment and controls are not statistically significant after adjusting for clustering at the block batch level. Data on all homeowners comes from the records of the utility company and includes households in both the treatment and control groups. Avg. Daily Electricity (kWh) in 2007 Household Size Age of Head Household Income Home Square Footage Home Year Built Block Group% College Block Group% Liberal Registered as Republican, American, Libertarian Democrat, Green, Peace and Freedom No party Other Not registered Donates to Environmental Causes Electric Heat Home Pays for Renewable Energy Observations Mean All Home Owners TABLE 1. Summary statistics, all homeowners, control and treatment group. Costa and Kahn Energy Conservation “Nudges” and Environmentalist Ideology 687 688 Journal of the European Economic Association TABLE 2. Summary statistics by political party registration. Conservatives Mean S.D. Mean S.D. 0.406 33.952 2.352 58.490 84279.550 1828.034 1973.234 0.380 0.408 0.087 0.247 0.070 0.491 16.236 1.192 14.104 43711.440 632.129 16.881 0.157 0.086 0.282 0.431 0.256 0.416 29.551*** 2.096*** 59.199*** 74806.960*** 1672.423*** 1968.815*** 0.375*** 0.454*** 0.114*** 0.237*** 0.141*** 0.493 14.248 1.099 13.413 40377.220 548.087 19.375 0.156 0.101 0.318 0.425 0.348 0.222 0.412 0.304*** 0.457 0.138 21,193 0.341 0.395*** 21,172 0.487 Note: A conservative is defined as Republican, American Party, or Libertarian. A liberal is defined as Democrat, Green, or Peace and Freedom. A “like-minded” community is defined for a conservative as a census block in the bottom quartile of fraction liberal. For a liberal, a “like-minded” community is defined as a census block in the top quartile of fraction liberal. The symbols ** and *** indicate that the differences between conservatives and liberals are statistically, significantly different at the 5 and 1 percent level (adjusting for block batch group), respectively. Households living in electric homes were more likely to receive a monthly rather than a quarterly report.12 Table 2 shows that registered conservatives are slightly younger, have higher household incomes, larger households, and larger homes than registered liberals. While both conservatives and liberals donate to environmental causes and pay for renewable energy, liberals are more likely to do so. We could explain only 2%–4% of the variance in registered conservative status with age, household income, house value, house size, electric heat, and renter status (see Online Appendix Table A.1 for details). Among the treated, liberals are more likely to receive the quarterly report, indicating that they use less electricity than conservatives. Liberals living in a liberal community (defined as a community in which the liberal share is in the top quartile) are almost three times as likely to receive the quarterly report as conservatives living in a conservative community (defined as a community in which the liberal share is in the bottom quartile). If political ideology does not affect the response to the HER, conservatives should respond more because they are treated more intensely. 12. Within a treated “block batch” households received either a monthly or quarterly report. Roughly 71% of households received the monthly report. Households with a low baseline electricity consumption received the quarterly report while households with a high baseline electricity consumption received the monthly report. Conditional that a household’s daily average electricity consumption was less than 20 kWh per day it had a 2.5% chance of receiving the monthly report. Households whose 2006 electricity consumption was greater than 23 kWh per day had a 99% chance of receiving the monthly report. Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019 Fraction in Treatment Group Avg. Daily Electricity (kWh) Household Size Age of Head Household Income Home Square Footage Home Year Built Block Group% College Block Group% Liberal Donates to Environmental Causes Electric Heat Home Pays for Renewable Energy Treatment Group Receives Quarterly Report Receives Quarterly Report and in Like-minded community Observations Full Sample Liberals Costa and Kahn Energy Conservation “Nudges” and Environmentalist Ideology 689 5. Econometric Framework ln(kW h) = β0 + β1 (Household FE) + β2 (Month/Year FE) + β3 (Temp, Electric) + β4 TREAT + β6 (TREAT×Party Registration) + β7 (TREAT × Green Indicators) + β8 (TREAT × Individual Characteristics) + β9 (TREAT × Block Characteristics) + β10 (TREAT × House Characteristics) + β11 (Post TreatmentPeriod Dummy × All Characteristics) + ε, (1) where the unit of analysis is the household in a year and month and where TREAT is a dummy equal to one if the household received the Home Energy Report and where the different specifications use different subsets of the variables. TREAT thus is equal to 0 either if the household is never treated or is not yet treated. In all regressions we control for household and month/year fixed effects, a cubic in mean daily temperature within the billing cycle, and an interaction of the cubic mean daily temperature with a dummy indicator if the house is an electric house (Temp, Electric). We also interact party registration, green indicators, individual characteristics, block characteristics, and house characteristics with an indicator for whether the treatment period has started. Our political party indicators are liberal (Democrat, Green, or Peace and Freedom), other party (Reform, Conservative, Natural Law or Other), no party affiliation, and not registered, with conservative (Republican, American Party, and Libertarian) as the omitted dummy variable. Our green indicators are whether the household purchases energy from renewable sources and whether the household donates money to environmental causes. We cluster the standard errors on the block batch group to account for correlation within the block batch group. Our specifications differ in the number of included treatment interaction effects. We examine who accepts treatment by estimating, for the treatment group, a probit regression of the form OptOut = β0 + β1 High + β2 ln(Usage) + β3 Age + β4 Liberal +β5 Unregistered + ε, (2) where OptOut is a dummy variable equal to one if the household opts out of receiving the treatment, High is a dummy variable equal to one if the household’s consumption Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019 Previous studies have examined the HER report’s average treatment effect and have also examined how its effect varies by standard characteristics such as attributes of the home and socio-economic characteristics of the owner. The distinguishing feature of this study is our emphasis on household environmental ideology as an important determinant for how households respond to well-meaning new information. We estimate intent-to-treat effects (which we will simply refer to as treatment effects) of receiving the HER by seven specifications of the form 690 Journal of the European Economic Association Report Reaction = β0 + β1 High + β2 ln(Usage) + β3 Age + β4 Liberal + β5 Unregistered + ε, (3) where Report Reaction is either a dummy variable equal to one if the household found the reports of no value (responses of not at all or not very valuable) or a dummy variable equal to one if the household disliked the reports (responses of did not like or indifferent). 6. Results Own ideology, whether measured by political party affiliation, donations to environmental organizations, or the purchase of green energy, is associated with differential treatment effects (see regression 2 in Table 3).13 Although the mean overall treatment effect is –0.021 (see regression 1 in Table 3) when we do not allow for heterogeneous treatment effects, a registered conservative will decrease mean daily kWh by 1.7% in response to the treatment but a registered liberal will reduce consumption by 2.4%, all else equal (see regression 2 in Table 3). Those purchasing energy from renewable resources reduce their consumption by 0.9% in response to the treatment relative to those not purchasing green energy. Those donating to environmental organizations reduce their consumption by 1.1% more than households who do not donate to environmental organizations. The fraction of liberals and the fraction of college-educated in the census block group affects treatment response, independent of own characteristics (see regression 3 in Table 3). An increase of 0.1 in the fraction of liberals in the census block group reduces consumption by 0.6% in response to the treatment. Controlling for the fraction of liberals in the block group leads to statistically insignificant effects of own party affiliation but leaves the effects of ideology as measured by donations unchanged. Own ideology, donating to environmental groups, and paying for renewable energy remain jointly statistically significant. The higher the fraction of college graduates in a census block group, the lower consumption. 13. We also have information on household monthly expenditure on electricity. In the presence of an increasing block tariff structure, some recipients of the HER may reduce their expenditure by more than their electricity consumption. We have estimated regressions similar to equation (1) in which we use the log of household monthly electricity expenditure as the dependent variable. We obtained very similar results. Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019 is above the neighborhood average, Usage is the household’s electricity usage in 2006, Age is the age of the head of the household, Liberal is a dummy equal to one if the household head was registered as either a Democrat, Green, or Peace and Freedom party member, and Unregistered is a dummy equal to one of if the household was not registered. Using a small surveyed sample, we also examine who, in the treatment group, found the reports of no value or disliked the reports by estimating probit regressions of the form Costa and Kahn Energy Conservation “Nudges” and Environmentalist Ideology 691 TABLE 3. Treatment effects by ideology, education, and home structure. Dependent Variable: Log (Mean Daily kWh) (2) − 0.021*** − 0.017*** (0.003) (0.003) (Registered liberal) − 0.007** (0.003) (Registered other party) 0.031 (0.032) (No registered party) 0.004 (0.005) (Not in voter registration − 0.003 data) (0.004) (Donates to environmental − 0.011** (0.004) organizations) (Pays for renewable − 0.009* (0.005) energy) (Liberal share within block group) (College graduate share within block group) (Logarithm of age of house) (Logarithm of square footage of house) (Electric House) Treated (3) (4) (5) (6) 0.026** (0.011) − 0.004 (0.003) 0.028 (0.032) 0.004 (0.005) − 0.002 (0.004) − 0.011** (0.004) − 0.006 (0.005) − 0.062*** (0.023) − 0.045*** (0.017) 0.069 (0.063) − 0.004 (0.003) 0.028 (0.032) 0.004 (0.005) − 0.002 (0.004) − 0.010** (0.004) − 0.006 (0.005) − 0.038* (0.021) − 0.043** (0.018) − 0.009** (0.004) − 0.003 (0.007) − 0.014*** (0.005) 0.016 (0.073) − 0.004 (0.003) 0.030 (0.031) 0.003 (0.005) − 0.003 (0.004) − 0.009** (0.004) − 0.006 (0.005) − 0.036* (0.022) − 0.047** (0.019) − 0.007 (0.005) − 0.008 (0.007) − 0.014*** (0.005) − 0.001 (0.004) 0.008*** (0.003) − 0.008 (0.018) 0.008 (0.008) − 0.005 (0.003) 0.028 (0.032) 0.004 (0.005) − 0.002 (0.004) − 0.011** (0.004) − 0.007 (0.005) (Logarithm of household income) (Logarithm of home value) (Dummy = 1 if renter) (Second quintile of liberal share in block group) (Third quintile of liberal share in block group) (Fourth quintile of liberal share in block group) (Fifth quintile of liberal share in block group) Joint significance of registered liberal, donates to environmental organizations, and pays for renewable energy, F(3,956) Household fixed effects Month-Year fixed effects Y Y − 0.046*** (0.018) 5.68*** 3.81*** 3.30** 3.23** − 0.011 (0.007) − 0.018*** (0.007) − 0.010* (0.005) − 0.018* (0.010) 3.98*** Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019 (1) 692 Journal of the European Economic Association TABLE 3. Continued Dependent Variable: Log (Mean Daily kWh) Observations R-squared (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) 2,760,175 2,760,175 2,760,175 2,760,141 2,754,232 2,760,175 0.804 0.804 0.804 0.805 0.805 0.804 Note: Each observation is a household-billing cycle. Standard errors, clustered on the block batch group, are in parentheses. * indicates significance at the 10% level, ** at the 5% level, and *** at the 1% level. Additional control variables are a cubic in mean daily (24 hr.) temperature, the cubic in daily temperature interacted with a dummy indicating whether the home is an electric home, household fixed effects, year-month fixed effects, and interactions between characteristics and a time dummy indicating the experiment has started. Mean daily kWh are 31.69. Conservative is the omitted category and is defined as Republican, American Party, or Libertarian. Liberal is defined as Democrat, Green Party, or Peace and Freedom. The fourth regression in Table 3 controls for the effect of house characteristics on treatment response. Those in older houses, in bigger homes, and in electric homes reduce their consumption more. Housing characteristics may reflect occupant characteristics. Liberals are more likely to be in older houses (but less likely to be in bigger homes). Controlling for housing characteristics, each increase of 0.1 in the fraction of liberals in the census block group reduces consumption by 0.4% in response to the treatment. The fifth regression in Table 3 adds controls for the effect of household income, home value, and renter status on treatment response. There is no statistically significant differential treatment effect of renter status. The treatment effect is increasing in home value. The addition of these and the previous control variables reduces the impact of liberal share within the block group but does not affect how liberals respond to the treatment. The sixth regression in Table 4 examines the linearity assumption on liberal share within census block group by substituting quintiles of the liberal share for liberal share within blocks. It shows that a greater liberal share relative to the lowest quintile is associated with households’ reducing their electricity consumption in response to the treatment. The quintiles are jointly significantly different from 0 at the 10% level (F(4,956) = 2.29). We observe a similar pattern when we examined quartiles instead of quintiles (see regression 1 in Online Appendix Table A.2). Our seventh and final regression (predicted values are graphed in Figure 1 and the regression is given in the last column of Online Appendix Table A.2) tests for persistence over time by adding indicators for whether the time period is during the first or second half of the experiment. It shows that registered conservatives who do not purchase renewable energy and who do not donate to environmental organizations reduce their consumption in response to the treatment by 0.01 in the first half of the experiment and by 0.02 in the second half of the experiment. Registered liberals who purchase renewable energy and who donate to environmental organizations reduce their consumption in response to the treatment by 0.04 in the first half of the experiment and by 0.05 in the second half of the experiment. Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019 (1) Costa and Kahn Energy Conservation “Nudges” and Environmentalist Ideology 693 TABLE 4. Predicted Treatment Effects by Ideology Std. Err. −0.036*** 0.006 −0.021*** −0.011*** 0.003 0.003 −0.048*** 0.010 −0.031*** −0.008*** 0.009 0.003 Note: Predicted treatment effects are estimated from Regressions 4 and 6 in Table 3. *** indicates statistical significance at the 1% level. Everyone in the treatment group is assigned the given characteristics while all other characteristics are kept at their median values. Conservative is defined as Republican, American Party, or Libertarian. Liberal is defined as Democrats, Green Party, and Peace and Freedom. When we restricted the sample to households whose electricity usage was above the median in 2006 (precisely those households a utility would want to target to reduce total electricity demand), we obtained a similar response for liberals but a larger response for households who pay for renewable energy (see Online Appendix Table A.3). The effects of being in an electric home are no longer as large. When we examined households with baseline electricity usage below the median we found a smaller treatment effect and a statistically insignificant effect of paying for renewable energy and for donating to environmental groups (see Online Appendix Table A.4). Table 4 examines the role that ideology plays in responding to receiving the HER. We use the regression results from Table 3’s columns (4) and (6). Evaluating all characteristics at the median and using the regression in column (4), the treatment effect for liberals who purchase energy from renewable resources, who donate to environmental causes, and who live in a block group where the share of liberals is at least in the 75th percentile (less than 1% of our sample) is –0.036. The treatment effect for registered liberal who live in a block group where the share of liberals is at least in the 75th percentile (26% of our sample) is –0.021. The treatment effect for a conservative who does not pay for renewable energy, does not donate to environmental groups, and is in bottom 25th percentile liberal block group (22% of our sample) is –0.011. When we use the regression results from Table 3’s column (6) we obtain a treatment effect of –0.008 for a conservative who does not pay for renewable energy, does not donate to environmental groups, and is in the bottom quintile liberal block group. The treatment effect for a liberal who pays for renewable energy, donates to environmental groups, and is in the top quintile liberal block group is –0.048. Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019 Using Regression 4 (Table 3) Registered liberal, pays for renewable energy, donates to environmental groups, and in top 75th percentile liberal block group Registered liberals and in top 75th percentile liberal block group Registered conservative, does not pay for renewable energy, does not donate to environmental groups, and in bottom 25th percentile liberal block group Using Regression 6 (Table 3) Registered liberal, pays for renewable energy, donates to environmental groups, and in top 75th percentile liberal block group Registered liberals and in top quintile liberal block group Registered conservative, does not pay for renewable energy, does not donate to environmental groups, and in bottom quintile liberal block group Treatment Effect Journal of the European Economic Association 0 694 -.02 -.04 -.06 Registed liberal, pays for renewable energy, donates to environmental groups First Half Experiment Second Half Experiment Time F IGURE 1. Predicted treatment effects over time. Predicted treatment effects are estimated from Regression 4 in Web Appendix Table 2. Upper and lower bounds of a 95% confidence interval are given as dashed lines. Everyone in the treatment group is assigned the given characteristics while all other characteristics are kept at their median values. Conservative is defined as Republican, American Party, or Libertarian. Liberal is defined as Democrats, Green Party, and Peace and Freedom. “First Half Experiment” refers to all observations in 2008 and thus includes roughly 8 months of treatment. “Second Half Experiment” refers to households in 2009 and thus includes about 10 months of treatment. We further probed the robustness of our results by estimating equation (1) using quantile regressions (see Table 5). Estimating the impact of the HERs at the 10th, 25th, 50th, 75th, and 90th quantiles, our predicted results show that at the lower quantiles liberals who pay for renewable energy and donate to environmental organizations are three to four times as likely to reduce their consumption in response to the treatment as conservatives who do not pay for renewable energy and do not donate to environmental organizations. At the 90th quantile, differences between conservatives and liberals are smaller. 6.1. Persistence of the Report Effects Our examination of seasonal patterns of response to the treatment leads us to conclude that liberals are more likely to turn down the air-conditioning in the summer in response to the treatment. When we added to equation (4) in Table 3 an interaction Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019 Registered conservative, pays for renewable energy, donates to environmental groups Costa and Kahn Energy Conservation “Nudges” and Environmentalist Ideology 695 TABLE 5. Treatment effects by ideology, quantile regressions. Dependent Variable: Log (Mean Daily kWh) Quantiles Treated x (Registered liberal) (Registered other party) (No registered party) (Not in voter registration data) (Donates to environmental organizations) (Pays for renewable energy) Household fixed effects Month-Year fixed effects Observations Pseudo R-squared Predicted Treatment Effect Conservatives who do not donate to environmental organization or pay for renewable energy Liberals who donate to environmental organizations and pay for renewable energy 0.25 0.50 0.75 0.90 − 0.018*** (0.002) − 0.017*** (0.002) − 0.017 (0.021) − 0.017 (0.031) − 0.020*** (0.006) − 0.007*** (0.003) − 0.009 (0.034) 0.003 (0.005) − 0.003 (0.004) − 0.019*** (0.006) − 0.007*** (0.002) 0.013 (0.024) 0.000 (0.003) 0.000 (0.002) − 0.014*** (0.003) − 0.007 (0.039) 0.014 (0.101) 0.001 (0.061) 0.002 (0.011) − 0.009 (0.074) − 0.006 (0.043) 0.027 (0.048) − 0.001 (0.055) 0.004 (0.034) − 0.006 (0.104) − 0.005** (0.003) 0.073** (0.035) 0.005 (0.009) 0.007 (0.008) − 0.000 (0.008) − 0.002 (0.003) Y Y 2,760,175 0.112 − 0.010*** (0.003) Y Y 2,760,175 0.175 − 0.01 (0.066) Y Y 2,760,175 0.245 − 0.009 (0.033) Y Y 2,760,175 0.284 − 0.007 (0.006) Y Y 2,760,175 0.267 − 0.018 − 0.017 − 0.017 − 0.017 − 0.020 − 0.046 − 0.048 − 0.043 − 0.038 − 0.032 Note: Each observation is a household-billing cycle. Bootstrap standard errors, clustered on the block batch group, are in parentheses. * indicates significance at the 10% level, ** at the 5% level, and *** at the 1% level. Additional control variables are a cubic in mean daily (24 hr.) temperature, the cubic in daily temperature interacted with a dummy indicating whether the home is an electric home, household fixed effects, year-month fixed effects, and interactions between characteristics and a time dummy indicating the experiment has started . Mean daily kWh are 31.69. Conservative is the omitted category and is defined as Republican, American Party, or Libertarian. Liberal is defined as Democrat, Green Party, or Peace and Freedom. between treatment and summer months (May 1–October 31) and an interaction between treatment, summer months, and liberal, we obtained a coefficient on the interaction between treatment and summer months of –0.002 (σ = 0.006) and a coefficient on the interaction between treatment and summer months and liberal of –0.012 ( σ = 0.004) (see Online Appendix Table A.2, column 3). It is theoretically ambiguous whether the HERs will have a larger impact in the short run or the medium term. When a household first receives such a report this may be a salient event whose “new news” shocks the household and subsequent reports Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019 Treated 0.10 696 Journal of the European Economic Association 6.2. Opting out of the Treatment The decision to quit the HER treatment and consumer survey reactions to the HER provide additional evidence on which subgroups of the population disliked the treatment that are consistent with our identity story. Households could opt out of receiving the HER either by emailing, phoning, or mailing the utility. Although the information is free, 2% of households took action not to receive it and 36% of the survey group reported disliking the report. Our results have implications not just for our identity hypothesis but also for the long-term success of the HER program among different types of households. Households that opted out of the treatment were more likely to be high electricity consumers, both relative to their neighbors and in absolute levels, and they were less likely to be liberals than conservatives (see Table 6). At the mean opt out rate of 0.020, a liberal was 15% less likely to opt out. High electricity users relative to their neighbors were 65% more likely to opt out. In a subsample of 1,061 consumers interviewed about the home energy reports, high electricity users, both relative to their neighbors and in absolute levels, were more likely to claim that the reports were useless or that they disliked them. Liberals were less likely than conservatives to state that the reports were useless or that they disliked them. Being liberal decreased the probability of finding a report useless by 0.131, a decrease of 44% from the sample mean of 0.301. Being a liberal decreased the probability of disliking the report by 0.102, a decrease of 28% from the sample mean of 0.363. High electricity users relative to their neighbors were 27% more likely to find the report useless and were 38% more likely to report disliking the report. Liberals and conservatives did not report differential rates of spending less than 2 minutes reading the report (see the last column in Online Appendix Table A.5). High users were statistically, significantly more likely to spend less time reading the report. Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019 reinforce the original news. In this case, we might observe a large drop in consumption followed by a constant level (climate adjusted). Alternatively, in the medium term a household is more likely to adjust more of its durables stock and may make more energy efficient investments when it makes new investments in such durables. The evidence suggests that this strategy is being pursued. We found that households in the treatment group were more likely to obtain a rebate from the utility for purchasing an energy efficient durable. In a probit regression (see Online Appendix Table A.5) of the probability of obtaining a rebate on whether the household was in the treatment group, the household’s political affiliation, the age of the household head, and the household’s baseline electricity usage, we found that the derivative of the coefficient on the treatment dummy was a statistically significant 0.006 (σ = 0.002). At the sample mean of 0.056, this represents an 11% increase in the probability of obtaining a rebate. Costa and Kahn Energy Conservation “Nudges” and Environmentalist Ideology 697 TABLE 6. Decision to opt out of treatment and view of reports by ideology. Dependent Variable = 1 if Reports of Dislike Dummy = 1 if Above community mean electricity use Logarithm of 2006 electricity consumption Age of household head Dummy = 1 if registered Republican, American Party, Libertarian Democrat, Green, Peace and Freedom Dummy = 1 if not registered Pseudo R-squared Observations 0.013*** (0.002) 0.008*** (0.002) 0.000*** (0.000) − 0.003** (0.001) − 0.004** (0.001) 0.062 32,667 No Value Reports 0.082** (0.035) 0.159*** (0.038) 0.003*** (0.001) 0.138*** (0.036) 0.103*** (0.040) 0.001 (0.001) − 0.131*** (0.032) − 0.078** (0.032) 0.055 1,061 − 0.102*** (0.035) − 0.031 (0.036) 0.04 1,061 Note: The opt out decision is estimated for all treated households. Registered voters with no party affiliation or with an affiliation other than Republican, American Party, Libertarian, Democrat, Green, Peace, and Freedom are not included in the regressions. The mean opt out rate is 0.020. A subsample of the treatment group was interviewed about the home energy reports. 30.1% of the sample found the reports to be not at all or not very valuable. 36.3% of the sample reported not liking or being indifferent about receiving the reports. Standard errors are in parentheses. 6.3 A Regression Discontinuity Test of Differential Responses to Normative Messages This paper’s main focus has been to estimate the differential response of environmentalists and non-environmentalists to receiving a HER report. We have also examined whether, among those who received a HER report, there is a differential response between political liberals and conservatives to the normative message included with the first report. Households received one of three normative messages: “great”, “good”, or “room for improvement”. These normative messages were based on the household’s consumption compared to that of 100 neighbors living in similar-sized houses. For the first report only we observe both the normative message and the ratio of the household’s electricity consumption in the last month divided by the average consumption of 100 nearby neighbors. As discussed in the Online Appendix, we implemented a regression discontinuity design (see Lee and Lemieux 2010) to test whether political ideology influences how households respond to receiving sharp normative feedback. Although our point estimates suggest that a political conservative increases consumption by 5.7% in response to receiving a normative message of “good” versus “room for improvement” whereas a political liberal increases consumption by only 0.9%, the 95% point wise confidence intervals are large and we fail to reject the hypothesis that there is no “normative message” for either conservatives or liberals. Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019 Opt Out 698 Journal of the European Economic Association 7. Conclusion Appendix. Sample Home Electricity Report 14. Allcott (2011) provides an excellent cost–benefit analysis of electricity nudges. Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019 “Nudges” can make us healthier, richer in our retirement (through opt out defaults), and better environmental citizens. Given that such information treatments are cheap to produce and distribute, these could be cost-effective policies especially for those subsets of households who are most responsive to these treatments.14 This paper exploited a unique data merge of information from an electricity information provision field experiment to study how liberal/environmental ideology mediates responses to peer comparison information. Liberal households are less likely to drop out of the experiment and more likely to report that they like receiving the report than political conservatives. In response to receiving the report, liberals reduce their electricity consumption by a larger percentage than conservatives. Our results suggest that environmental nudges are most effective in relatively liberal communities. What works in California may not work in Lubbock, Texas. And even in California, targeted messaging may be more cost-effective than random assignment of home energy reports. Future research should continue to test for what might be effective conservation messages with political conservatives. Costa and Kahn Energy Conservation “Nudges” and Environmentalist Ideology 699 Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019 Source: Residential Energy Use Behavior Change Pilot, OPOWER white paper, http://www.opower.com/LinkClick.aspx?fileticket=cLLj7p8LwGU%3d&tabid=7 700 Journal of the European Economic Association References Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019 ADM Associates, Inc. (2009). The Impact of Home Electricity Reports. September. Alesina, Alberto and Nicola Fuchs-Schundeln (2007). “Good-Bye Lenin (or Not?): The Effect of Communism on People’s Preferences.” American Economic Review, 97, 1507–1528. Allcott, Hunt (2011). “Social Norms and Energy Conservation.” Journal of Public Economics, 95, 1082–1095. Allcott, Hunt and Sendhil Mullainathan (2010). “Behavior and Energy Policy.” Science, 327, 1204– 1205. Ayers, Ian, Sophie Raseman, and Alice Shih (2009). “Evidence from Two Large Field Experiments that Peer Comparison Feedback Can Reduce Residential Energy Usage.” National Bureau of Economic Research Working Paper 15386. Bertrand, Marianne, Esther Duflo, and Sendhil Mullainathan (2004). “How Much Should We Trust Differences-in-Differences Estimates?” Quarterly Journal of Economics, 119, 249–275. Brounen, Dirk and Nils Kok (2011). “On the Economics of Energy Labels in the Housing Market.” Journal of Environmental Economics and Management, 62, 166–179. Cialdini, Robert G. et. al. (2006), Managing Social Norms for Persuasive Impact, Social Influence, 1, 3–15. Costa, Dora L. and Matthew E. Kahn (2010). “Why Has California’s Residential Electricty Consumption Been So Flat Since the 1980s? A Microeconometric Approach.” National Bureau of Economic Research Working Paper 15978. Cragg, Michael, YuYu Zhou, Kevin Gurney, and Matthew E. Kahn (2013). “Carbon Geography: The Political Economy of Congressional Support for Legislation Intended to Mitigate Greenhouse Gas Production.” Economic Inquiry, 51, 1640–1650. DellaVigna, Stefano and Ethan Kaplan (2007). “The Fox News Effect: Media Bias and Voting.” Quarterly Journal of Economics, 122, 1187–1234. Di Tella, Rafael, Sebastian Galiani, and Ernesto Schargrodsky (2007). “The Formation of Beliefs: Evidence from the Allocation of Land Titles to Squatters.” Quarterly Journal of Economics, 122, 209–241. Dunlap, Riley E. and Aaron M. McCright (2008). “A Widening Gap: Republican and Democratic Views on Climate Change.” Environment, 50, 26–35. Gentzkow, Matthew and Jesse M. Shapiro (2006). “Media Bias and Reputation.” Journal of Political Economy, 114, 280–316. Gentzkow, Matthew and Jesse M. Shapiro (2010). “What Drives Media Slant? Evidence from U.S. Daily Newspapers.” Econometrica, 78, 35–71. Giuliano, Paola and Antonio Spilimbergo (2009). “Growing Up in a Recession: Beliefs and the Macroeconomy.” National Bureau of Economic Research Working Paper No. 15321. Goldstein, Noah, Robert B. Cialdini, and Vladas Griskevicius (2008). “A Room With a Viewpoint: Using Social Norms to Motivate Environmental Conservation in Hotels.” Journal of Consumer Research, 35, 472–482. Griskevicius, Vladas, Robert B. Cialdini, and Noah J. Goldstein (2008). “Social Norms: An Underestimated and Underemployed Lever for Managing Climate Change.” IJSC, 3, 5–13. Halbheer, Daniel, Sarah Niggli, and Armin Schmutzler (2006). “What Does it Take to Sell Environmental Policy? An Empirical Analysis of Referendum Data.” Environmental and Resource Economics, 33, 441–462. Hardistry, David, Eric Johnson, and Elke Weber (2010). “A Dirty Word or a Dirty World? Attribute Framing, Political Affiliation, and Query Theory.” Psychological Science, 2, 86–92. Kahn, Matthew E (2007). “Do Greens Drive Hummers or Hybrids? Environmental Ideology as a Determinant of Consumer Choice.” Journal of Environmental Economics and Management, 54, 129–145. Kahn, Matthew E. and Eric Morris (2009). “Walking the Walk: The Association Between Environmentalism and Green Transit Behavior.” Journal of the American Planning Association, 75, 389–405. Costa and Kahn Energy Conservation “Nudges” and Environmentalist Ideology 701 Supporting Information Additional Supporting Information may be found in the online version of this article at the publisher’s website: Table A.1. Correlates of being a registered conservative and of being a registered conservative who does not donate to environmental groups, does not pay for renewable energy, and lives in a conservative area. Table A.2. Treatment effects by ideology. Table A.3. Treatment effects by ideology, education, and structure for households above the median baseline usage. Table A.4. Treatment effects by ideology, education, and structure for households below the median baseline usage. Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019 Kahn, Matthew E. and Ryan K. Vaughn (2009). “Green Market Geography: The Spatial Clustering of Hybrid Vehicles and LEED Registered Buildings.” Contributions in Economic Policy, A Berkeley Electronic Press Journal. Kotchen, Matthew and Michael Moore (2007). “Private Provision of Environmental Public Goods: Household Participation in Green-Electricity Programs.” Journal of Environmental Economics and Management, 53, 1–16. Kotchen, Matthew and Michael Moore (2008). “Conservation: From Voluntary Restraint to a Voluntary Price Premium.” Environmental and Resource Economics, 40, 195–215. Lee, David S. and Thomas Lemieux (2010). “Regression Discontinuity Designs in Economics.” Journal of Economic Literature, 48, 281–355. Lee, David S., Enrico Moretti, and Matthew J. Butler (2004). “Do Voters Affect Or Elect Policies? Evidence from the U.S. House.” The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 119, 807–859. Lord, Charles G., Lee Ross, and Mark R. Lepper (1979). “Biased Assimilation and Attitude Polarization: The Effects of Prior Theories on Subsequently Considered Evidence.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 2098–2109. Miller, Arthur G., John W. McHoskey, Cynthia M. Bane, and Timothy G. Dowd (1993). “The Attitude Polarization Phenomenon: Role of Response Measure, Attitude Extremity, and Behavioral Consequences of Reported Attitude Change.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 561–574. Munro, Geoffrey D. and Peter H. Ditto (1997). “Biased Assimilation, Attitude Polarization, and Affect in Reactions to Stereotype-Relevant Scientific Information.” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 636–653. Murphy, Kevin and Andrei Shleifer (2004). “Persuasion in Politics.” American Economic Review, 94, 435–439. Piketty, Thomas (1995). “Social Mobility and Redistributive Policies.” Quarterly Journal of Economics, 110, 551–584. Reiss, Peter and Matthew White (2008). “What Changes Energy Consumption? Prices and Public Pressure.” RAND Journal of Economics, 39, 636–663. Schultz, Wesley, Jessica M. Nolan, Robert B. Cialdini, Noah J. Goldstein, and Vladas Griskevicius (2007). “The Constructive, Destructive, and Reconstructive Power of Social Norms.” Psychological Science, 18, 429–434. Thaler, Richard H. and Cass R. Sunstein (2008). Nudge: Improving Decisions about Health, Wealth, and Happiness. Yale University Press. Thalmann, Philippe (2004). “The Public Acceptance of Green Taxes: 2 Million Voters Express Their Opinion.” Public Choice, 119, 179–217. 702 Journal of the European Economic Association Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019 Table A.5. Correlates of probability of obtaining a rebate to purchase an energy efficient durable and of spending less than 2 minutes reading the home energy report. Table A.6. Regression discontinuity test of differential responses to a normative message. Figure A.1. Density of household/neighbor kWh, conservatives. Density of the ratio of average daily household kWh to average daily kWh of 100 nearest neighbors living in a similar sized house in the first HER report received. Conservative is defined as Republican, American Party, or Libertarian. Figure A.2. Density of household/neighbor kWh, liberals. Density of the ratio of average daily household kWh to average daily kWh of 100 nearest neighbors living in a similar sized house in the first HER report received. Liberal is defined as Democrat, Green Party, or Peace and Freedom. Figure A.3. Change in electricity consumption for conservatives who receive a her and are at the “Good” versus “Room for Improvement” Cutoff. Figure A.4. Change in electricity consumption for liberals who receive a her and are at the “Good” versus “Room for Improvement” Cutoff. Referee Report The report is due before each presentation. The report should be less than two pages (single space, 12 fonts size, Times New Roman, Normal margin). In the report, you should include the following sections: Background and motivation (2 point) • Research question (2 points) • Data and study design (2 points) • Findings and policy implication (2 points) • Your comments and critiques (2 points) Use your own understanding to write the report, copy and paste is not allowed.
Purchase answer to see full attachment
User generated content is uploaded by users for the purposes of learning and should be used following Studypool's honor code & terms of service.

Explanation & Answer

I appreciate working with you! In case of any further edits, please do not hesitate to let me know! See you soon! Remember me as always! Would love and appreciate to work with you in the future! Goodbye

Energy Conservation Outline
❖ Background and Motivation
❖ Research Questions
❖ Data and Supply design
❖ Findings and Policy Implication
❖ Comments and critiques


Running head: ENERGY CONSERVATION REPORT

Energy Conservation Report
Student’s Name
Institutional Affiliation

1

ENERGY CONSERVATION REPORT

2

Energy Conservation Report
Background and motivation
The issue of energy conservation in households has increasingly become a major
component to energy policy in most parts of the world. Nudges are therefore promoted to
influence the behavior and decision making of groups and individuals in regards to energy
conservation. In continents such as Europe, high taxes are imposed on energy sources to
encourage conservation and minimize global climate change and global warming. In Denmark
taxes take more than half proportion of the total cost of electricity. On the other hand, the U.S
records low taxes and lack of political readiness to sacrifice for electricity conservation. This
report seeks to show that the most common nudge of feedback provision to households is two to
four times more effective with liberals as opposed to conservatives.
Research Question
The increased political gridlock in the congress and concerns about climate issue have
triggered a continuous policy agenda, whose objective is to identify an alternative strategy for
encouraging conservation. Studies indicate that focusing on well-crafted messages that offer peer
comparisons could be an effective alternative tool for influencing households’ behavior. Further,
other researchers argue that nudges can influence households’ behaviors by informing them of
actions taken by others who have found themselves in similar circumstances. Promoting Nudges
in this case are considered to be a cost effective strategy for encouraging energy conservation.
According to Costa & Kahn (2013), there are high chances that the liberal environmentalists will
respond to energy conservation nudges than the average person. Further, those who support
green policies are more likely to have a smaller carbon footprint.in the article, the researchers
seek to analyze how social and political views affect household response to similar randomized
treatment.
Data and study design
Their research involved energy conservation nudges obtained from a random field
experiment conducted by a western utility district where each household’s ideology was
observed on characteristics including demographic, socioeconomic as well as behavioral
responses. The data set selected consisted of billing information from January 2007 to October
2009 (Costa & Kahn, 2013). The data was recorded in kilowatt hours bought in each billing
cycle, where the billing length was measured in days. This was irrespective of whether the
household’s uses electricity and whether they are part of the electric utility’s program to obtain
energy from nonrenewable resources. The investigation focused on specific household
information in regards to their monthly electricity usage over a given period and comparison
done with their neighbor’s usage over the same period. The report further stated energy saving
tips. To determine the role of political party ideology in assessing how the randomly selected
households respond, data related to customer political party of registration, their contributions to
environmental organizations as well as their involvement in renewable energy programs was
collected.
Findings and policy implications
Findings from their research indicated that the effectiveness of energy conservation
nudges is based on a person’s own ideology. For instance, democrats and green party members
in the United Sta...


Anonymous
I was stuck on this subject and a friend recommended Studypool. I'm so glad I checked it out!

Studypool
4.7
Indeed
4.5
Sitejabber
4.4

Related Tags