Dora L. Costa
Matthew E. Kahn
University of California, Los Angeles
University of California, Los Angeles
Abstract
“Nudges” are being widely promoted to encourage energy conservation. We show that the popular
electricity conservation “nudge” of providing feedback to households on own and peers’ home
electricity usage in a home electricity report is two to four times more effective with political
liberals than with conservatives. Political conservatives are more likely than liberals to opt out of
receiving the home electricity report and to report disliking the report. Our results suggest that energy
conservation nudges need to be targeted to be most effective. (JEL: Q41, D03, D72)
1. Introduction
Europe, especially Scandinavia, has high taxes on electricity and gasoline to encourage
conservation and counter global warming. Taxes in Denmark represent more than half
of the cost of electricity to consumers.1 In contrast, the United States has low taxes
and little political will to sacrifice for the sake of conservation. Congressional voting
patterns highlight that conservative Representatives are highly unlikely to vote for
carbon mitigation legislation (Cragg et al. 2013).
Facing political gridlock in the Congress and concerned about the challenge of
climate change, an ongoing policy agenda is seeking out alternative strategies for
encouraging conservation. Recent psychology research suggests an alternative tool
for changing household behavior is to focus on well crafted messages offering peer
comparisons (see Griskevicius, Cialdini, and Goldstein 2008). Robert Cialdini and his
coauthors have conducted a series of field experiments that have demonstrated that lowcost persuasion strategies or “nudges” can change an individual’s behavior by making
The editor in charge of this paper was Stefano DellaVigna.
Acknowledgments: We thank Maximilian Auffhammer, the participants at the 2010 POWER Conference,
and seminar participants at Princeton and the University of Illinois for comments. We thank the UCLA
Ziman Real Estate Center for funding. We thank the editor and five reviewers for their comments. Costa
and Kahn are Research Associates at NBER.
E-mail: costa@econ.ucla.edu (Costa); mkahn@ioe.ucla.edu (Kahn)
1. See Eurostat News Release, 75/2010, 28 May 2010. http://epp.eurostat.ec.europa.eu/cache/ITY_
PUBLIC/8-28052010-AP/EN/8-28052010-AP-EN.PDF
Journal of the European Economic Association June 2013
c 2013 by the European Economic Association
11(3):680–702
DOI: 10.1111/jeea.12011
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019
ENERGY CONSERVATION “NUDGES” AND
ENVIRONMENTALIST IDEOLOGY: EVIDENCE
FROM A RANDOMIZED RESIDENTIAL
ELECTRICITY FIELD EXPERIMENT
Costa and Kahn Energy Conservation “Nudges” and Environmentalist Ideology
681
2. In the United Kingdom, where electricity taxes are low, David Cameron has touted the behavioral transformations of putting “the typical electricity bill for a house like theirs in a neighborhood like theirs” in front
of households. Speech of 13 June, 2008. http://www.aletmanski.com/files/davidcameron-powerofsocialinnovation.doc
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019
him aware of the actions of others who have been in a similar situation (Goldstein et al.
2008; Schultz et al. 2007). Nudges may be a low cost strategy for encouraging energy
conservation (Allcott and Mullainathan 2010; Thaler and Sunstein 2008).2
We posit that liberal/environmentalists are more likely to respond to energy
conservation nudges. A series of recent empirical papers has documented that
environmentalists are more likely to engage in “voluntary restraint” than the average
person (Kotchen and Moore 2008). Those who vote in favor of “green policies” and
register for liberal/environmentalist political parties are more likely to have a smaller
carbon footprint and to purchase green products such as the Toyota Prius (Kahn 2007,
Kahn and Morris 2009). Such environmentalists consciously avoid free riding and
voluntarily restrain their consumption of goods and services that generate a negative
externality.
Our evidence on the role of ideology in energy conservation “nudges” comes from
a randomized field experiment carried out by a western utility district in which we can
observe a household’s ideology (an unobservable in prior studies), its socioeconomic
and demographic characteristics, and its behavioral responses. Starting in Spring 2008,
this utility has been sending households in the treatment group a Home Energy Report
(HER). The report provides household specific information on own monthly electricity
usage over time and relative to neighbors’ usage over the same time period. The
report provides energy saving tips. To examine the role that political ideology and
environmentalism play in determining how randomly selected households respond to
these reports, we have collected data on the customer’s political party of registration,
household donations to environmental organizations and household participation in
renewable energy programs, and data on the characteristics of the local residential
communities where the households live. Households who are registered in liberal
political parties and who live in residential communities with a large liberal share
and who have previously signed up for energy from renewable resources and donate
to environmental causes are arguably environmentalists. Our focus on ideology, an
unobservable in previous studies, distinguishes our work from other research (e.g.
Allcott 2011; Ayres, Raseman, and Shih 2009).
We find that the effectiveness of energy conservation “nudges” depends on an
individual’s ideology. In the United States, Democrat, Peace and Freedom, and
Green party members (liberals in the US terminology) are more likely to vote
for environmentalist causes than Republican, American Party, or Libertarian party
members (conservatives in the US terminology). We measure ideology not just with
registered political party, but also with indicators of living in a liberal or conservative
community and willingness to pay for energy generated from renewable resources and
to donate to environmental organizations. Although liberals and environmentalists are
more energy efficient than conservatives (Costa and Kahn 2010), thus making it harder
682
Journal of the European Economic Association
2. The Energy Conservation “Nudge”
The “nudge” that the electric utility company sends to treatment households in an ongoing randomized experiment to encourage reductions in electricity consumption is a
two-page HER (see the Appendix for a sample). Similar reports have been used by other
utilities in the United States. The front page compares the electricity consumption of the
household with all neighbors with similar size homes and heat type and with neighbors
who are in the bottom 20th percentile of electricity usage. The back page compares
the household’s electricity usage in the current month relative to the same time month
in the prior year and awards green stars in every month the household consumed less
relative to the same month in the past year (panel not shown in the Appendix because it
is not publicly available). It also provides three tips for saving energy, such as turning
down the thermostat when using an electric blanket or purchasing an Energy Star
durable, and indicates the dollar amount in energy savings per year (shown in the
Appendix).
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019
for them to reduce consumption further, we find that liberals and environmentalists are
more responsive to these nudges than conservatives.
We find that among political liberals who purchase electricity from renewable
resources, who donate to environmental causes, and who live in a census block group
where the share of liberals is in the top 75th percentile, receiving a HER led to
reductions in electricity usage of 3.6%. In contrast, among political conservatives who
do not pay for renewable electricity, who do not donate to environmental groups, and
who live in a census block group where the share of liberals is in the bottom 25th
percentile, receiving a HER led to reductions in electricity usage of 1.1%. Liberals are
more likely to turn down the air-conditioning in the summer in response to the HER
report. Political liberals were 15% less likely to opt out of receiving the report and, in a
survey, political liberals are also less likely than conservatives to state that the reports
were useless and to report disliking them.
By documenting the role that ideology plays in determining the effectiveness of a
specific “nudge”, this paper contributes to the growing literature on the consequences
of political ideology. Much of this work has focused on the role of political ideology
in shaping preferences for redistribution (e.g. Piketty 1995). Our work focuses on the
role of political ideology in shaping responses to non-market mechanisms designed
to reduce consumption. Recent work on the determinants of political ideology has
examined the causal role of the media (DellaVigna and Kaplan 2007), property rights
(Di Tella, Galiani, and Schargrodsky 2007), and historical circumstances (Alesina
and Fuchs-Schundeln 2007; Giuliano and Spilimbergo 2009) in shaping a person’s
ideological outlook. Both social psychologists and economists have argued that beliefs
on how society and the economy work predominately are formed at ages 18–25 (see
Giuliano and Spilimbergo 2009). In this study, we will take as given that a household
either is or is not a liberal/environmentalist and we will study how these political and
social views influence household response to the same randomized treatment.
Costa and Kahn Energy Conservation “Nudges” and Environmentalist Ideology
683
2.1. The HER Experiment
Between March 14 and May 9 2008, the electric utility sent the first Home Electricity
Reports to a treatment group of approximately 35,000 households. By April 1, 43%
of all treatment households had received the report and by April 15 the figure was
62%. Households are still receiving the report, either on a quarterly or monthly basis.
A control group of roughly 49,000 households have never received a HER.
The HER experiment selected households from 85 census tracts with a high
density of single-family homes (see ADM Associates 2009). Both treatment and
control households had to have a current account with the electric utility that had
been active for at least one year, could not be living in apartment buildings, and had to
be living in a house with square footage between 250 and 99,998 square feet. Groups
of contiguous census blocks were randomly assigned to either the treatment or control
group. A “block batch” of five contiguous census blocks was randomly assigned to the
treatment group and then a contiguous census block batch was assigned to the control
group. The process continued until roughly 35,000 households were assigned to both
the treatment and control groups. The remaining census blocks (about 14,000 homes)
were assigned to the control group. Contiguous block groups were used because the
implementation contractor, Positive Energy (now OPOWER), believed that increased
communication among people receiving the HERs in the same community would lead
to greater energy savings.4
Allcott (2011), Ayers, Raseman, and Shih (2009), and Schultz et al. (2007) found
that providing feedback to customers on home electricity and natural gas usage with
a focus on peer comparisons decreased consumption by 1% to 2%, potentially saving
110 million kWh per year if feedback were provided to all of the utility’s customers
(Ayers, Raseman, and Shih 2009). Additional evidence that social incentives can make
a difference comes from California’s 2001 media campaign to promote voluntary
conservation after rolling blackouts in 2000 and early 2001. Consumption in San Diego
3. We thank a reviewer for suggesting this research strategy.
4. In a 2009 Home Energy Use Survey conducted by the electric utility, households in the control group
were more likely to report talking to friends and neighbors about their electricity bill than households in
the treatment group, suggesting that receiving the HER did not inspire discussion and that any positive
peer effects operate through implicit social pressure.
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019
Each report contains two pieces of information: the household’s absolute level
of consumption and how its consumption compares with that of 100 neighbors living
in similar-sized homes. We also know whether a household in the treatment group
received a message of “great”, “good”, or “room for improvement” in the first report.
As we discuss in the Online Appendix, we use this information to implement a
regression discontinuity design to test whether the change in electricity consumption
for the treatment group differs depending on the normative message that the household
receives in the first report.3
684
Journal of the European Economic Association
declined by 7% during the initial two phases of the campaign, before rebounding (Reiss
and White 2008).
Within a household production framework, a household values electricity as an input
in producing comfort (e.g. indoor temperature) and leisure and household production
activities. Total household electricity consumption in any given period is the sum of
electricity used in each of these activities. A household’s total electricity consumption
depends on choices over (1) the attributes of the house, such as size; (2) the attributes of
appliances; and (3) the intensity of utilization of appliances for leisure and household
activities, indoor temperature control and illumination. These choices, in turn, depend
on climate, prices and personal attributes, including ideology.
We view environmental ideology as a set of prior beliefs including those about
the importance of energy conservation The ideological divide on environmental issues
between Democrats and Republicans could affect how a household responds to an
energy conservation “nudge”. In the United States, conservatives consistently oppose
environmental regulation and energy policies intended to further environmental aims,
as seen in polling data on the belief in climate change (Dunlap and McCright 2008) and
Congressional voting patterns (Cragg et al. 2013). Dunlap and McCright (2008) report
that in 2008 there was 34 percentage point gap between Democrats and Republicans
in their agreement with a statement that the effects of global warming have already
begun, up from a four percentage point gap in 1997. The 2008 National Environmental
Scorecard of the League of Conservation Voters gives the House Democratic leadership
a score of 95 (out of a best score of 100) and the Republican leadership a score of 3.5
A 2009 Pew survey found a 23 percentage gap between Democrat and Republican
agreement with the statement that people should be willing to pay higher prices to
protect the environment. Republicans and Democrats respond differently to “carbon
offsets” versus “carbon tax” (Hardistry, Johnson, and Weber 2010), suggesting that
ideology moderates how individuals think of key words.
European studies have highlighted the role that environmental ideology plays
in determining the willingness to take voluntary actions to mitigate one’s carbon
externality and the willingness to pay to purchase a “green product”. Thalmann (2004)
and Halbheer, Niggli, and Schmutzler (2006) document that voters who are left-ofcenter or who are environmentalists were more likely to vote for Swiss environmental
referenda. Brounen and Kok (2011) found that the price premium for residential homes
that are certified as highly energy efficient in Holland is higher in “green” communities,
that is communities where the Green Party and the Party for the Animals had received
a larger fraction of the vote.
Given this background, there are two main hypotheses that can be tested. Many
households will read the report and respond to it for non-ideological reasons, such
5.
See http://www.people-press.org/2009/05/21/section-9-the-environment-and-the-economy/
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019
3. Why Could Ideology Mediate the Response to this Nudge?
Costa and Kahn Energy Conservation “Nudges” and Environmentalist Ideology
685
4. Data
Our primary data set consists of residential billing data from January 2007 to October
2009. These data provide us with information on kilowatt hours purchased per billing
cycle, the length of the billing cycle (measured in days), whether the house uses electric
heat, and whether the household is enrolled in the electric utility’s program to purchase
energy from renewable sources. We link each billing cycle to the mean temperature in
that billing cycle.6
We link the billing data to the treatment and control data which contain information
on when the household began to receive the HERs, as well as information on square
footage of the house, information on whether the home heats with electricity or natural
gas, and the age of the house. We cannot match 1,976 observations in the pilot and
control data to the residential billing data. In our final data set, the treatment and control
data therefore contain 81,722, with 48,058 households in the control group. Among
the households in the treatment group, 24,028 received a monthly report and 9,636
received a quarterly report.
We merge individual voter registration and marketing data for March 2009 to our
data set.7 For registered voters we know party affiliation, and whether the individual
donates to environmental organizations. We were able to link half of our sample to
the voter registration data. We linked either the person whose name was on the utility
bill or the first person on the utility bill.8 The individuals we could not link were
living in smaller households and in census block groups with a low proportion of the
college-educated, were more likely to receive a subsidy for electricity because of their
6. Two different households in the same calendar year and same month who are on different billing
cycles will face different climate conditions.
7. We purchased the data from www.aristotle.com.
8. Only 5% of households were “mixed” between conservatives and liberals.
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019
as wanting to lower their bill. These households, regardless of ideology, may reduce
their consumption. The response of liberals is ambiguous. Liberals may reduce their
consumption by more than conservatives because of their ideology and have been
observed to consume less electricity (Kotchen and Moore 2008; Costa and Kahn 2010).
However, because they have already invested more time and effort in monitoring their
electricity bills and in engaging in voluntary restraint (i.e. lowering the air-conditioner
in the summer), their response could be lower than that of conservatives. Secondly,
and more controversially, it is possible that anti-environmentalist conservatives who
receive a green looking report and learn that they consume more than their peers may
refuse to decrease their consumption or even increase their consumption in an act
of defiance. People find information more reliable when it conforms to their strong
prior beliefs (e.g. Lord, Ross, and Leper 1979; Miller et al. 1993; Munro and Ditto
1997; Gentzkow and Shapiro 2006, 2010) and are influenced mainly by those in their
network (Murphy and Shleifer 2004).
686
Journal of the European Economic Association
9. Relative to all homeowners in the same county these individuals were also more likely to be of Asian
or other ancestry rather than of European ancestry, but were less likely to be Spanish speaking. They were
also lower income.
10. The collected revenue is used by the electric utility to purchase and produce power from wind, water,
and Sun.
11. Household income is available from credit bureau data.
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019
low income, and were more likely to have a household head above age 60.9 We also
merge to these data, by the census block group, the share of registered voters who
were liberal (Democrat, Green, or Peace and Freedom) in 2000 and the share of the
college-educated in the block group. We expect that environmentalists are more likely
to live in liberal, educated communities (Kahn 2007).
We have access to two revealed preference measures of a household’s environmentalism. From the data base with voter registration information, we know whether
a household has donated money to an environmental group. We also know whether
the household has signed up for the company’s renewable power program prior to
the treatment. This is the electric utility’s major program to increase the share of
its customers who have signed up for renewable energy. Each household decides
whether to opt in and pay a fixed cost of $3 a month to have 50% of its power
generated by renewables or $6 a month to have 100% of its power generated by
renewables.10
We also have access to an ancillary data set which we use to examine household
attitudes about the HER by ideology. In 2009 the electric utility company surveyed
1,375 households who received the HER, asking them questions about the HER report.
We restrict this sample to households for whom we have information on age and the
fraction of liberals in the block group and to households who were not in minor parties
we could not classify as liberal or conservatives. This leaves us with 1,061 observations
in this ancillary data set.
Table 1 shows that the treatment and control groups are roughly representative of
all homeowners in the county in terms of household and neighborhood characteristics.
But, there are some clear differences. The treatment and control groups consume
roughly 10% more electricity than the average county homeowner as of 2007 (before
the experiment). Relative to the average homeowner, the experiment homes are older
and more likely to be electric homes. The households in the experiment group are
roughly 10% richer than the average county homeowner.11 The geographical areas
included in the experiment have a higher share of college graduates than the average
county home owner’s community.
The randomization of the HER across blocks was effective. Ayers, Raseman,
and Shih (2009) reported that controlling for house characteristics, household
demographics, and the number of cooling degree days and heating degree days, there
was no systematic difference in energy usage between treatment and control groups
prior to the treatment (also see Table 1 where we report no systematic differences
between treatment and control groups adjusting for block batch group correlation).
17.710
1.159
14.967
43,252.950
682.611
20.619
0.162
0.105
0.492
0.498
0.330
0.045
0.499
0.275
0.371
0.283
27.930
2.111
56.582
66,484.710
1,709.447
1,976.764
0.283
0.460
0.412
0.461
0.124
0.002
0.474
0.082
0.165
0.088
285,717
S.D.
0.447
0.439
0.112
0.002
0.425
0.099
0.246
0.104
48,058
31.051
2.111
56.941
74,826.920
1,720.876
1,971.176
0.364
0.436
Mean
S.D.
0.497
0.496
0.316
0.046
0.494
0.299
0.431
0.305
15.473
1.136
14.952
41,364.120
602.081
18.377
0.158
0.098
Control Group
0.438
0.449
0.112
0.002
0.430
0.097
0.264
0.103
33,664
30.801
2.103
56.594
74,312.590
1706.109
1972.618
0.363
0.438
Mean
0.496
0.497
0.315
0.043
0.495
0.296
0.441
0.304
14.727
1.137
15.085
41,546.370
578.287
18.547
0.162
0.097
S.D.
Treatment Group
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019
Note: All variables listed after the block group variables are dummy variables. The treatment and controls are not statistically significant after adjusting for clustering at the block
batch level. Data on all homeowners comes from the records of the utility company and includes households in both the treatment and control groups.
Avg. Daily Electricity (kWh) in 2007
Household Size
Age of Head
Household Income
Home Square Footage
Home Year Built
Block Group% College
Block Group% Liberal
Registered as
Republican, American, Libertarian
Democrat, Green, Peace and Freedom
No party
Other
Not registered
Donates to Environmental Causes
Electric Heat Home
Pays for Renewable Energy
Observations
Mean
All Home Owners
TABLE 1. Summary statistics, all homeowners, control and treatment group.
Costa and Kahn Energy Conservation “Nudges” and Environmentalist Ideology
687
688
Journal of the European Economic Association
TABLE 2. Summary statistics by political party registration.
Conservatives
Mean
S.D.
Mean
S.D.
0.406
33.952
2.352
58.490
84279.550
1828.034
1973.234
0.380
0.408
0.087
0.247
0.070
0.491
16.236
1.192
14.104
43711.440
632.129
16.881
0.157
0.086
0.282
0.431
0.256
0.416
29.551***
2.096***
59.199***
74806.960***
1672.423***
1968.815***
0.375***
0.454***
0.114***
0.237***
0.141***
0.493
14.248
1.099
13.413
40377.220
548.087
19.375
0.156
0.101
0.318
0.425
0.348
0.222
0.412
0.304***
0.457
0.138
21,193
0.341
0.395***
21,172
0.487
Note: A conservative is defined as Republican, American Party, or Libertarian. A liberal is defined as Democrat,
Green, or Peace and Freedom. A “like-minded” community is defined for a conservative as a census block in
the bottom quartile of fraction liberal. For a liberal, a “like-minded” community is defined as a census block in
the top quartile of fraction liberal. The symbols ** and *** indicate that the differences between conservatives
and liberals are statistically, significantly different at the 5 and 1 percent level (adjusting for block batch group),
respectively.
Households living in electric homes were more likely to receive a monthly rather than
a quarterly report.12
Table 2 shows that registered conservatives are slightly younger, have higher
household incomes, larger households, and larger homes than registered liberals. While
both conservatives and liberals donate to environmental causes and pay for renewable
energy, liberals are more likely to do so. We could explain only 2%–4% of the variance
in registered conservative status with age, household income, house value, house size,
electric heat, and renter status (see Online Appendix Table A.1 for details).
Among the treated, liberals are more likely to receive the quarterly report,
indicating that they use less electricity than conservatives. Liberals living in a liberal
community (defined as a community in which the liberal share is in the top quartile)
are almost three times as likely to receive the quarterly report as conservatives living
in a conservative community (defined as a community in which the liberal share is
in the bottom quartile). If political ideology does not affect the response to the HER,
conservatives should respond more because they are treated more intensely.
12. Within a treated “block batch” households received either a monthly or quarterly report. Roughly
71% of households received the monthly report. Households with a low baseline electricity consumption
received the quarterly report while households with a high baseline electricity consumption received the
monthly report. Conditional that a household’s daily average electricity consumption was less than 20
kWh per day it had a 2.5% chance of receiving the monthly report. Households whose 2006 electricity
consumption was greater than 23 kWh per day had a 99% chance of receiving the monthly report.
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019
Fraction in Treatment Group
Avg. Daily Electricity (kWh)
Household Size
Age of Head
Household Income
Home Square Footage
Home Year Built
Block Group% College
Block Group% Liberal
Donates to Environmental Causes
Electric Heat Home
Pays for Renewable Energy
Treatment Group
Receives Quarterly Report
Receives Quarterly Report and in
Like-minded community
Observations Full Sample
Liberals
Costa and Kahn Energy Conservation “Nudges” and Environmentalist Ideology
689
5. Econometric Framework
ln(kW h) = β0 + β1 (Household FE) + β2 (Month/Year FE) + β3 (Temp, Electric)
+ β4 TREAT + β6 (TREAT×Party Registration) + β7 (TREAT
× Green Indicators) + β8 (TREAT × Individual Characteristics)
+ β9 (TREAT × Block Characteristics)
+ β10 (TREAT × House Characteristics)
+ β11 (Post TreatmentPeriod Dummy × All Characteristics) + ε,
(1)
where the unit of analysis is the household in a year and month and where TREAT
is a dummy equal to one if the household received the Home Energy Report and
where the different specifications use different subsets of the variables. TREAT thus
is equal to 0 either if the household is never treated or is not yet treated. In all
regressions we control for household and month/year fixed effects, a cubic in mean
daily temperature within the billing cycle, and an interaction of the cubic mean daily
temperature with a dummy indicator if the house is an electric house (Temp, Electric).
We also interact party registration, green indicators, individual characteristics, block
characteristics, and house characteristics with an indicator for whether the treatment
period has started. Our political party indicators are liberal (Democrat, Green, or
Peace and Freedom), other party (Reform, Conservative, Natural Law or Other), no
party affiliation, and not registered, with conservative (Republican, American Party,
and Libertarian) as the omitted dummy variable. Our green indicators are whether
the household purchases energy from renewable sources and whether the household
donates money to environmental causes. We cluster the standard errors on the block
batch group to account for correlation within the block batch group. Our specifications
differ in the number of included treatment interaction effects.
We examine who accepts treatment by estimating, for the treatment group, a probit
regression of the form
OptOut = β0 + β1 High + β2 ln(Usage) + β3 Age + β4 Liberal
+β5 Unregistered + ε,
(2)
where OptOut is a dummy variable equal to one if the household opts out of receiving
the treatment, High is a dummy variable equal to one if the household’s consumption
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019
Previous studies have examined the HER report’s average treatment effect and have
also examined how its effect varies by standard characteristics such as attributes of
the home and socio-economic characteristics of the owner. The distinguishing feature
of this study is our emphasis on household environmental ideology as an important
determinant for how households respond to well-meaning new information.
We estimate intent-to-treat effects (which we will simply refer to as treatment
effects) of receiving the HER by seven specifications of the form
690
Journal of the European Economic Association
Report Reaction = β0 + β1 High + β2 ln(Usage) + β3 Age + β4 Liberal
+ β5 Unregistered + ε,
(3)
where Report Reaction is either a dummy variable equal to one if the household found
the reports of no value (responses of not at all or not very valuable) or a dummy
variable equal to one if the household disliked the reports (responses of did not like or
indifferent).
6. Results
Own ideology, whether measured by political party affiliation, donations to
environmental organizations, or the purchase of green energy, is associated with
differential treatment effects (see regression 2 in Table 3).13 Although the mean
overall treatment effect is –0.021 (see regression 1 in Table 3) when we do not allow
for heterogeneous treatment effects, a registered conservative will decrease mean
daily kWh by 1.7% in response to the treatment but a registered liberal will reduce
consumption by 2.4%, all else equal (see regression 2 in Table 3). Those purchasing
energy from renewable resources reduce their consumption by 0.9% in response
to the treatment relative to those not purchasing green energy. Those donating to
environmental organizations reduce their consumption by 1.1% more than households
who do not donate to environmental organizations.
The fraction of liberals and the fraction of college-educated in the census block
group affects treatment response, independent of own characteristics (see regression
3 in Table 3). An increase of 0.1 in the fraction of liberals in the census block group
reduces consumption by 0.6% in response to the treatment. Controlling for the fraction
of liberals in the block group leads to statistically insignificant effects of own party
affiliation but leaves the effects of ideology as measured by donations unchanged. Own
ideology, donating to environmental groups, and paying for renewable energy remain
jointly statistically significant. The higher the fraction of college graduates in a census
block group, the lower consumption.
13. We also have information on household monthly expenditure on electricity. In the presence of an
increasing block tariff structure, some recipients of the HER may reduce their expenditure by more than
their electricity consumption. We have estimated regressions similar to equation (1) in which we use the log
of household monthly electricity expenditure as the dependent variable. We obtained very similar results.
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019
is above the neighborhood average, Usage is the household’s electricity usage in 2006,
Age is the age of the head of the household, Liberal is a dummy equal to one if the
household head was registered as either a Democrat, Green, or Peace and Freedom
party member, and Unregistered is a dummy equal to one of if the household was not
registered.
Using a small surveyed sample, we also examine who, in the treatment group,
found the reports of no value or disliked the reports by estimating probit regressions
of the form
Costa and Kahn Energy Conservation “Nudges” and Environmentalist Ideology
691
TABLE 3. Treatment effects by ideology, education, and home structure.
Dependent Variable: Log (Mean Daily kWh)
(2)
− 0.021*** − 0.017***
(0.003)
(0.003)
(Registered liberal)
− 0.007**
(0.003)
(Registered other party)
0.031
(0.032)
(No registered party)
0.004
(0.005)
(Not in voter registration
− 0.003
data)
(0.004)
(Donates to environmental
− 0.011**
(0.004)
organizations)
(Pays for renewable
− 0.009*
(0.005)
energy)
(Liberal share within
block group)
(College graduate share
within block group)
(Logarithm of age of
house)
(Logarithm of square
footage of house)
(Electric House)
Treated
(3)
(4)
(5)
(6)
0.026**
(0.011)
− 0.004
(0.003)
0.028
(0.032)
0.004
(0.005)
− 0.002
(0.004)
− 0.011**
(0.004)
− 0.006
(0.005)
− 0.062***
(0.023)
− 0.045***
(0.017)
0.069
(0.063)
− 0.004
(0.003)
0.028
(0.032)
0.004
(0.005)
− 0.002
(0.004)
− 0.010**
(0.004)
− 0.006
(0.005)
− 0.038*
(0.021)
− 0.043**
(0.018)
− 0.009**
(0.004)
− 0.003
(0.007)
− 0.014***
(0.005)
0.016
(0.073)
− 0.004
(0.003)
0.030
(0.031)
0.003
(0.005)
− 0.003
(0.004)
− 0.009**
(0.004)
− 0.006
(0.005)
− 0.036*
(0.022)
− 0.047**
(0.019)
− 0.007
(0.005)
− 0.008
(0.007)
− 0.014***
(0.005)
− 0.001
(0.004)
0.008***
(0.003)
− 0.008
(0.018)
0.008
(0.008)
− 0.005
(0.003)
0.028
(0.032)
0.004
(0.005)
− 0.002
(0.004)
− 0.011**
(0.004)
− 0.007
(0.005)
(Logarithm of household
income)
(Logarithm of home
value)
(Dummy = 1 if renter)
(Second quintile of liberal
share in block group)
(Third quintile of liberal
share in block group)
(Fourth quintile of liberal
share in block group)
(Fifth quintile of liberal
share in block group)
Joint significance of
registered liberal,
donates to
environmental
organizations, and pays
for renewable energy,
F(3,956)
Household fixed effects
Month-Year fixed effects
Y
Y
− 0.046***
(0.018)
5.68***
3.81***
3.30**
3.23**
− 0.011
(0.007)
− 0.018***
(0.007)
− 0.010*
(0.005)
− 0.018*
(0.010)
3.98***
Y
Y
Y
Y
Y
Y
Y
Y
Y
Y
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019
(1)
692
Journal of the European Economic Association
TABLE 3. Continued
Dependent Variable: Log (Mean Daily kWh)
Observations
R-squared
(2)
(3)
(4)
(5)
(6)
2,760,175 2,760,175 2,760,175 2,760,141 2,754,232 2,760,175
0.804
0.804
0.804
0.805
0.805
0.804
Note: Each observation is a household-billing cycle. Standard errors, clustered on the block batch group, are in
parentheses. * indicates significance at the 10% level, ** at the 5% level, and *** at the 1% level. Additional
control variables are a cubic in mean daily (24 hr.) temperature, the cubic in daily temperature interacted with a
dummy indicating whether the home is an electric home, household fixed effects, year-month fixed effects, and
interactions between characteristics and a time dummy indicating the experiment has started. Mean daily kWh
are 31.69. Conservative is the omitted category and is defined as Republican, American Party, or Libertarian.
Liberal is defined as Democrat, Green Party, or Peace and Freedom.
The fourth regression in Table 3 controls for the effect of house characteristics
on treatment response. Those in older houses, in bigger homes, and in electric
homes reduce their consumption more. Housing characteristics may reflect occupant
characteristics. Liberals are more likely to be in older houses (but less likely to be
in bigger homes). Controlling for housing characteristics, each increase of 0.1 in the
fraction of liberals in the census block group reduces consumption by 0.4% in response
to the treatment.
The fifth regression in Table 3 adds controls for the effect of household income,
home value, and renter status on treatment response. There is no statistically significant
differential treatment effect of renter status. The treatment effect is increasing in home
value. The addition of these and the previous control variables reduces the impact of
liberal share within the block group but does not affect how liberals respond to the
treatment.
The sixth regression in Table 4 examines the linearity assumption on liberal share
within census block group by substituting quintiles of the liberal share for liberal
share within blocks. It shows that a greater liberal share relative to the lowest quintile
is associated with households’ reducing their electricity consumption in response to
the treatment. The quintiles are jointly significantly different from 0 at the 10% level
(F(4,956) = 2.29). We observe a similar pattern when we examined quartiles instead
of quintiles (see regression 1 in Online Appendix Table A.2).
Our seventh and final regression (predicted values are graphed in Figure 1 and
the regression is given in the last column of Online Appendix Table A.2) tests for
persistence over time by adding indicators for whether the time period is during the
first or second half of the experiment. It shows that registered conservatives who do
not purchase renewable energy and who do not donate to environmental organizations
reduce their consumption in response to the treatment by 0.01 in the first half of the
experiment and by 0.02 in the second half of the experiment. Registered liberals who
purchase renewable energy and who donate to environmental organizations reduce their
consumption in response to the treatment by 0.04 in the first half of the experiment
and by 0.05 in the second half of the experiment.
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019
(1)
Costa and Kahn Energy Conservation “Nudges” and Environmentalist Ideology
693
TABLE 4. Predicted Treatment Effects by Ideology
Std. Err.
−0.036***
0.006
−0.021***
−0.011***
0.003
0.003
−0.048***
0.010
−0.031***
−0.008***
0.009
0.003
Note: Predicted treatment effects are estimated from Regressions 4 and 6 in Table 3. *** indicates statistical
significance at the 1% level. Everyone in the treatment group is assigned the given characteristics while all
other characteristics are kept at their median values. Conservative is defined as Republican, American Party, or
Libertarian. Liberal is defined as Democrats, Green Party, and Peace and Freedom.
When we restricted the sample to households whose electricity usage was above
the median in 2006 (precisely those households a utility would want to target to
reduce total electricity demand), we obtained a similar response for liberals but a
larger response for households who pay for renewable energy (see Online Appendix
Table A.3). The effects of being in an electric home are no longer as large. When we
examined households with baseline electricity usage below the median we found a
smaller treatment effect and a statistically insignificant effect of paying for renewable
energy and for donating to environmental groups (see Online Appendix Table A.4).
Table 4 examines the role that ideology plays in responding to receiving the HER.
We use the regression results from Table 3’s columns (4) and (6). Evaluating all
characteristics at the median and using the regression in column (4), the treatment
effect for liberals who purchase energy from renewable resources, who donate to
environmental causes, and who live in a block group where the share of liberals is at
least in the 75th percentile (less than 1% of our sample) is –0.036. The treatment effect
for registered liberal who live in a block group where the share of liberals is at least in the
75th percentile (26% of our sample) is –0.021. The treatment effect for a conservative
who does not pay for renewable energy, does not donate to environmental groups,
and is in bottom 25th percentile liberal block group (22% of our sample) is –0.011.
When we use the regression results from Table 3’s column (6) we obtain a treatment
effect of –0.008 for a conservative who does not pay for renewable energy, does not
donate to environmental groups, and is in the bottom quintile liberal block group. The
treatment effect for a liberal who pays for renewable energy, donates to environmental
groups, and is in the top quintile liberal block group is –0.048.
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019
Using Regression 4 (Table 3)
Registered liberal, pays for renewable energy, donates to
environmental groups, and in top 75th percentile liberal block
group
Registered liberals and in top 75th percentile liberal block group
Registered conservative, does not pay for renewable energy, does
not donate to environmental groups, and in bottom 25th
percentile liberal block group
Using Regression 6 (Table 3)
Registered liberal, pays for renewable energy, donates to
environmental groups, and in top 75th percentile liberal block
group
Registered liberals and in top quintile liberal block group
Registered conservative, does not pay for renewable energy, does
not donate to environmental groups, and in bottom quintile
liberal block group
Treatment Effect
Journal of the European Economic Association
0
694
-.02
-.04
-.06
Registed liberal, pays for renewable energy, donates to environmental groups
First Half Experiment
Second Half Experiment
Time
F IGURE 1. Predicted treatment effects over time. Predicted treatment effects are estimated from
Regression 4 in Web Appendix Table 2. Upper and lower bounds of a 95% confidence interval are
given as dashed lines. Everyone in the treatment group is assigned the given characteristics while all
other characteristics are kept at their median values. Conservative is defined as Republican, American
Party, or Libertarian. Liberal is defined as Democrats, Green Party, and Peace and Freedom. “First
Half Experiment” refers to all observations in 2008 and thus includes roughly 8 months of treatment.
“Second Half Experiment” refers to households in 2009 and thus includes about 10 months of
treatment.
We further probed the robustness of our results by estimating equation (1) using
quantile regressions (see Table 5). Estimating the impact of the HERs at the 10th, 25th,
50th, 75th, and 90th quantiles, our predicted results show that at the lower quantiles
liberals who pay for renewable energy and donate to environmental organizations are
three to four times as likely to reduce their consumption in response to the treatment as
conservatives who do not pay for renewable energy and do not donate to environmental
organizations. At the 90th quantile, differences between conservatives and liberals are
smaller.
6.1. Persistence of the Report Effects
Our examination of seasonal patterns of response to the treatment leads us to conclude
that liberals are more likely to turn down the air-conditioning in the summer in
response to the treatment. When we added to equation (4) in Table 3 an interaction
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019
Registered conservative, pays for renewable
energy, donates to environmental groups
Costa and Kahn Energy Conservation “Nudges” and Environmentalist Ideology
695
TABLE 5. Treatment effects by ideology, quantile regressions.
Dependent Variable: Log (Mean Daily kWh) Quantiles
Treated x
(Registered liberal)
(Registered other party)
(No registered party)
(Not in voter
registration data)
(Donates to
environmental
organizations)
(Pays for renewable
energy)
Household fixed effects
Month-Year fixed effects
Observations
Pseudo R-squared
Predicted Treatment
Effect
Conservatives who do
not donate to
environmental
organization or pay
for renewable energy
Liberals who donate to
environmental
organizations and pay
for renewable energy
0.25
0.50
0.75
0.90
− 0.018***
(0.002)
− 0.017***
(0.002)
− 0.017
(0.021)
− 0.017
(0.031)
− 0.020***
(0.006)
− 0.007***
(0.003)
− 0.009
(0.034)
0.003
(0.005)
− 0.003
(0.004)
− 0.019***
(0.006)
− 0.007***
(0.002)
0.013
(0.024)
0.000
(0.003)
0.000
(0.002)
− 0.014***
(0.003)
− 0.007
(0.039)
0.014
(0.101)
0.001
(0.061)
0.002
(0.011)
− 0.009
(0.074)
− 0.006
(0.043)
0.027
(0.048)
− 0.001
(0.055)
0.004
(0.034)
− 0.006
(0.104)
− 0.005**
(0.003)
0.073**
(0.035)
0.005
(0.009)
0.007
(0.008)
− 0.000
(0.008)
− 0.002
(0.003)
Y
Y
2,760,175
0.112
− 0.010***
(0.003)
Y
Y
2,760,175
0.175
− 0.01
(0.066)
Y
Y
2,760,175
0.245
− 0.009
(0.033)
Y
Y
2,760,175
0.284
− 0.007
(0.006)
Y
Y
2,760,175
0.267
− 0.018
− 0.017
− 0.017
− 0.017
− 0.020
− 0.046
− 0.048
− 0.043
− 0.038
− 0.032
Note: Each observation is a household-billing cycle. Bootstrap standard errors, clustered on the block batch group,
are in parentheses. * indicates significance at the 10% level, ** at the 5% level, and *** at the 1% level. Additional
control variables are a cubic in mean daily (24 hr.) temperature, the cubic in daily temperature interacted with a
dummy indicating whether the home is an electric home, household fixed effects, year-month fixed effects, and
interactions between characteristics and a time dummy indicating the experiment has started . Mean daily kWh
are 31.69. Conservative is the omitted category and is defined as Republican, American Party, or Libertarian.
Liberal is defined as Democrat, Green Party, or Peace and Freedom.
between treatment and summer months (May 1–October 31) and an interaction between
treatment, summer months, and liberal, we obtained a coefficient on the interaction
between treatment and summer months of –0.002 (σ = 0.006) and a coefficient on the
interaction between treatment and summer months and liberal of –0.012 (
σ = 0.004)
(see Online Appendix Table A.2, column 3).
It is theoretically ambiguous whether the HERs will have a larger impact in the
short run or the medium term. When a household first receives such a report this may
be a salient event whose “new news” shocks the household and subsequent reports
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019
Treated
0.10
696
Journal of the European Economic Association
6.2. Opting out of the Treatment
The decision to quit the HER treatment and consumer survey reactions to the HER
provide additional evidence on which subgroups of the population disliked the
treatment that are consistent with our identity story. Households could opt out of
receiving the HER either by emailing, phoning, or mailing the utility. Although the
information is free, 2% of households took action not to receive it and 36% of the
survey group reported disliking the report. Our results have implications not just for
our identity hypothesis but also for the long-term success of the HER program among
different types of households.
Households that opted out of the treatment were more likely to be high electricity
consumers, both relative to their neighbors and in absolute levels, and they were less
likely to be liberals than conservatives (see Table 6). At the mean opt out rate of 0.020,
a liberal was 15% less likely to opt out. High electricity users relative to their neighbors
were 65% more likely to opt out.
In a subsample of 1,061 consumers interviewed about the home energy reports,
high electricity users, both relative to their neighbors and in absolute levels, were
more likely to claim that the reports were useless or that they disliked them. Liberals
were less likely than conservatives to state that the reports were useless or that they
disliked them. Being liberal decreased the probability of finding a report useless by
0.131, a decrease of 44% from the sample mean of 0.301. Being a liberal decreased
the probability of disliking the report by 0.102, a decrease of 28% from the sample
mean of 0.363. High electricity users relative to their neighbors were 27% more likely
to find the report useless and were 38% more likely to report disliking the report.
Liberals and conservatives did not report differential rates of spending less than
2 minutes reading the report (see the last column in Online Appendix Table A.5).
High users were statistically, significantly more likely to spend less time reading the
report.
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019
reinforce the original news. In this case, we might observe a large drop in consumption
followed by a constant level (climate adjusted).
Alternatively, in the medium term a household is more likely to adjust more of
its durables stock and may make more energy efficient investments when it makes
new investments in such durables. The evidence suggests that this strategy is being
pursued. We found that households in the treatment group were more likely to obtain a
rebate from the utility for purchasing an energy efficient durable. In a probit regression
(see Online Appendix Table A.5) of the probability of obtaining a rebate on whether
the household was in the treatment group, the household’s political affiliation, the age
of the household head, and the household’s baseline electricity usage, we found that
the derivative of the coefficient on the treatment dummy was a statistically significant
0.006 (σ = 0.002). At the sample mean of 0.056, this represents an 11% increase in
the probability of obtaining a rebate.
Costa and Kahn Energy Conservation “Nudges” and Environmentalist Ideology
697
TABLE 6. Decision to opt out of treatment and view of reports by ideology.
Dependent Variable = 1 if Reports of Dislike
Dummy = 1 if
Above community mean electricity use
Logarithm of 2006 electricity consumption
Age of household head
Dummy = 1 if registered
Republican, American Party, Libertarian
Democrat, Green, Peace and Freedom
Dummy = 1 if not registered
Pseudo R-squared
Observations
0.013***
(0.002)
0.008***
(0.002)
0.000***
(0.000)
− 0.003**
(0.001)
− 0.004**
(0.001)
0.062
32,667
No Value
Reports
0.082**
(0.035)
0.159***
(0.038)
0.003***
(0.001)
0.138***
(0.036)
0.103***
(0.040)
0.001
(0.001)
− 0.131***
(0.032)
− 0.078**
(0.032)
0.055
1,061
− 0.102***
(0.035)
− 0.031
(0.036)
0.04
1,061
Note: The opt out decision is estimated for all treated households. Registered voters with no party affiliation or
with an affiliation other than Republican, American Party, Libertarian, Democrat, Green, Peace, and Freedom
are not included in the regressions. The mean opt out rate is 0.020. A subsample of the treatment group was
interviewed about the home energy reports. 30.1% of the sample found the reports to be not at all or not very
valuable. 36.3% of the sample reported not liking or being indifferent about receiving the reports. Standard errors
are in parentheses.
6.3 A Regression Discontinuity Test of Differential Responses to Normative
Messages
This paper’s main focus has been to estimate the differential response of
environmentalists and non-environmentalists to receiving a HER report. We have also
examined whether, among those who received a HER report, there is a differential
response between political liberals and conservatives to the normative message
included with the first report.
Households received one of three normative messages: “great”, “good”, or
“room for improvement”. These normative messages were based on the household’s
consumption compared to that of 100 neighbors living in similar-sized houses. For the
first report only we observe both the normative message and the ratio of the household’s
electricity consumption in the last month divided by the average consumption of 100
nearby neighbors.
As discussed in the Online Appendix, we implemented a regression discontinuity
design (see Lee and Lemieux 2010) to test whether political ideology influences
how households respond to receiving sharp normative feedback. Although our point
estimates suggest that a political conservative increases consumption by 5.7% in
response to receiving a normative message of “good” versus “room for improvement”
whereas a political liberal increases consumption by only 0.9%, the 95% point wise
confidence intervals are large and we fail to reject the hypothesis that there is no
“normative message” for either conservatives or liberals.
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019
Opt Out
698
Journal of the European Economic Association
7. Conclusion
Appendix. Sample Home Electricity Report
14.
Allcott (2011) provides an excellent cost–benefit analysis of electricity nudges.
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019
“Nudges” can make us healthier, richer in our retirement (through opt out defaults),
and better environmental citizens. Given that such information treatments are cheap
to produce and distribute, these could be cost-effective policies especially for those
subsets of households who are most responsive to these treatments.14
This paper exploited a unique data merge of information from an electricity
information provision field experiment to study how liberal/environmental ideology
mediates responses to peer comparison information. Liberal households are less likely
to drop out of the experiment and more likely to report that they like receiving the
report than political conservatives. In response to receiving the report, liberals reduce
their electricity consumption by a larger percentage than conservatives.
Our results suggest that environmental nudges are most effective in relatively
liberal communities. What works in California may not work in Lubbock, Texas.
And even in California, targeted messaging may be more cost-effective than random
assignment of home energy reports. Future research should continue to test for what
might be effective conservation messages with political conservatives.
Costa and Kahn Energy Conservation “Nudges” and Environmentalist Ideology
699
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019
Source: Residential Energy Use Behavior Change Pilot, OPOWER white paper,
http://www.opower.com/LinkClick.aspx?fileticket=cLLj7p8LwGU%3d&tabid=7
700
Journal of the European Economic Association
References
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019
ADM Associates, Inc. (2009). The Impact of Home Electricity Reports. September.
Alesina, Alberto and Nicola Fuchs-Schundeln (2007). “Good-Bye Lenin (or Not?): The Effect of
Communism on People’s Preferences.” American Economic Review, 97, 1507–1528.
Allcott, Hunt (2011). “Social Norms and Energy Conservation.” Journal of Public Economics, 95,
1082–1095.
Allcott, Hunt and Sendhil Mullainathan (2010). “Behavior and Energy Policy.” Science, 327, 1204–
1205.
Ayers, Ian, Sophie Raseman, and Alice Shih (2009). “Evidence from Two Large Field Experiments
that Peer Comparison Feedback Can Reduce Residential Energy Usage.” National Bureau of
Economic Research Working Paper 15386.
Bertrand, Marianne, Esther Duflo, and Sendhil Mullainathan (2004). “How Much Should We Trust
Differences-in-Differences Estimates?” Quarterly Journal of Economics, 119, 249–275.
Brounen, Dirk and Nils Kok (2011). “On the Economics of Energy Labels in the Housing Market.”
Journal of Environmental Economics and Management, 62, 166–179.
Cialdini, Robert G. et. al. (2006), Managing Social Norms for Persuasive Impact, Social Influence,
1, 3–15.
Costa, Dora L. and Matthew E. Kahn (2010). “Why Has California’s Residential Electricty
Consumption Been So Flat Since the 1980s? A Microeconometric Approach.” National Bureau
of Economic Research Working Paper 15978.
Cragg, Michael, YuYu Zhou, Kevin Gurney, and Matthew E. Kahn (2013). “Carbon Geography: The
Political Economy of Congressional Support for Legislation Intended to Mitigate Greenhouse
Gas Production.” Economic Inquiry, 51, 1640–1650.
DellaVigna, Stefano and Ethan Kaplan (2007). “The Fox News Effect: Media Bias and Voting.”
Quarterly Journal of Economics, 122, 1187–1234.
Di Tella, Rafael, Sebastian Galiani, and Ernesto Schargrodsky (2007). “The Formation of Beliefs:
Evidence from the Allocation of Land Titles to Squatters.” Quarterly Journal of Economics,
122, 209–241.
Dunlap, Riley E. and Aaron M. McCright (2008). “A Widening Gap: Republican and Democratic
Views on Climate Change.” Environment, 50, 26–35.
Gentzkow, Matthew and Jesse M. Shapiro (2006). “Media Bias and Reputation.” Journal of Political
Economy, 114, 280–316.
Gentzkow, Matthew and Jesse M. Shapiro (2010). “What Drives Media Slant? Evidence from U.S.
Daily Newspapers.” Econometrica, 78, 35–71.
Giuliano, Paola and Antonio Spilimbergo (2009). “Growing Up in a Recession: Beliefs and the
Macroeconomy.” National Bureau of Economic Research Working Paper No. 15321.
Goldstein, Noah, Robert B. Cialdini, and Vladas Griskevicius (2008). “A Room With a Viewpoint:
Using Social Norms to Motivate Environmental Conservation in Hotels.” Journal of Consumer
Research, 35, 472–482.
Griskevicius, Vladas, Robert B. Cialdini, and Noah J. Goldstein (2008). “Social Norms: An
Underestimated and Underemployed Lever for Managing Climate Change.” IJSC, 3, 5–13.
Halbheer, Daniel, Sarah Niggli, and Armin Schmutzler (2006). “What Does it Take to Sell
Environmental Policy? An Empirical Analysis of Referendum Data.” Environmental and
Resource Economics, 33, 441–462.
Hardistry, David, Eric Johnson, and Elke Weber (2010). “A Dirty Word or a Dirty World? Attribute
Framing, Political Affiliation, and Query Theory.” Psychological Science, 2, 86–92.
Kahn, Matthew E (2007). “Do Greens Drive Hummers or Hybrids? Environmental Ideology as a
Determinant of Consumer Choice.” Journal of Environmental Economics and Management, 54,
129–145.
Kahn, Matthew E. and Eric Morris (2009). “Walking the Walk: The Association Between
Environmentalism and Green Transit Behavior.” Journal of the American Planning Association,
75, 389–405.
Costa and Kahn Energy Conservation “Nudges” and Environmentalist Ideology
701
Supporting Information
Additional Supporting Information may be found in the online version of this article
at the publisher’s website:
Table A.1. Correlates of being a registered conservative and of being a registered
conservative who does not donate to environmental groups, does not pay for renewable
energy, and lives in a conservative area.
Table A.2. Treatment effects by ideology.
Table A.3. Treatment effects by ideology, education, and structure for households
above the median baseline usage.
Table A.4. Treatment effects by ideology, education, and structure for households
below the median baseline usage.
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019
Kahn, Matthew E. and Ryan K. Vaughn (2009). “Green Market Geography: The Spatial Clustering
of Hybrid Vehicles and LEED Registered Buildings.” Contributions in Economic Policy, A
Berkeley Electronic Press Journal.
Kotchen, Matthew and Michael Moore (2007). “Private Provision of Environmental Public Goods:
Household Participation in Green-Electricity Programs.” Journal of Environmental Economics
and Management, 53, 1–16.
Kotchen, Matthew and Michael Moore (2008). “Conservation: From Voluntary Restraint to a
Voluntary Price Premium.” Environmental and Resource Economics, 40, 195–215.
Lee, David S. and Thomas Lemieux (2010). “Regression Discontinuity Designs in Economics.”
Journal of Economic Literature, 48, 281–355.
Lee, David S., Enrico Moretti, and Matthew J. Butler (2004). “Do Voters Affect Or Elect Policies?
Evidence from the U.S. House.” The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 119, 807–859.
Lord, Charles G., Lee Ross, and Mark R. Lepper (1979). “Biased Assimilation and Attitude
Polarization: The Effects of Prior Theories on Subsequently Considered Evidence.” Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 2098–2109.
Miller, Arthur G., John W. McHoskey, Cynthia M. Bane, and Timothy G. Dowd (1993). “The Attitude
Polarization Phenomenon: Role of Response Measure, Attitude Extremity, and Behavioral
Consequences of Reported Attitude Change.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
64, 561–574.
Munro, Geoffrey D. and Peter H. Ditto (1997). “Biased Assimilation, Attitude Polarization, and
Affect in Reactions to Stereotype-Relevant Scientific Information.” Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 23, 636–653.
Murphy, Kevin and Andrei Shleifer (2004). “Persuasion in Politics.” American Economic Review,
94, 435–439.
Piketty, Thomas (1995). “Social Mobility and Redistributive Policies.” Quarterly Journal of
Economics, 110, 551–584.
Reiss, Peter and Matthew White (2008). “What Changes Energy Consumption? Prices and Public
Pressure.” RAND Journal of Economics, 39, 636–663.
Schultz, Wesley, Jessica M. Nolan, Robert B. Cialdini, Noah J. Goldstein, and Vladas Griskevicius
(2007). “The Constructive, Destructive, and Reconstructive Power of Social Norms.”
Psychological Science, 18, 429–434.
Thaler, Richard H. and Cass R. Sunstein (2008). Nudge: Improving Decisions about Health, Wealth,
and Happiness. Yale University Press.
Thalmann, Philippe (2004). “The Public Acceptance of Green Taxes: 2 Million Voters Express Their
Opinion.” Public Choice, 119, 179–217.
702
Journal of the European Economic Association
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jeea/article-abstract/11/3/680/2300535 by State Univ of NY - Binghamton user on 11 February 2019
Table A.5. Correlates of probability of obtaining a rebate to purchase an energy
efficient durable and of spending less than 2 minutes reading the home energy report.
Table A.6. Regression discontinuity test of differential responses to a normative
message.
Figure A.1. Density of household/neighbor kWh, conservatives. Density of the ratio
of average daily household kWh to average daily kWh of 100 nearest neighbors living
in a similar sized house in the first HER report received. Conservative is defined as
Republican, American Party, or Libertarian.
Figure A.2. Density of household/neighbor kWh, liberals. Density of the ratio of
average daily household kWh to average daily kWh of 100 nearest neighbors living in
a similar sized house in the first HER report received. Liberal is defined as Democrat,
Green Party, or Peace and Freedom.
Figure A.3. Change in electricity consumption for conservatives who receive a her
and are at the “Good” versus “Room for Improvement” Cutoff.
Figure A.4. Change in electricity consumption for liberals who receive a her and are
at the “Good” versus “Room for Improvement” Cutoff.
Referee Report The report is due before each presentation. The report should be less than
two pages (single space, 12 fonts size, Times New Roman, Normal margin). In the report, you
should include the following sections:
Background and motivation (2 point)
• Research question (2 points)
• Data and study design (2 points)
• Findings and policy implication (2 points)
• Your comments and critiques (2 points)
Use your own understanding to write the report, copy and paste is not allowed.
Purchase answer to see full
attachment