Discussion Board Questions:
Special Topics: Conspiracy theories, Fake News, Social Media and Disaster
Listen to “The Daily” podcast episode from May 24,2018 (23 min) https://tinyurl.com/yb5o7uyl
Next, read these two articles, which can be found in Wk 11 Module’s “Additional
Readings” folder.
•
•
Psychological Outcomes in Reaction to Media Exposure to Disasters and Large-Scale Violence: A MetaAnalysis
Social Media in Disaster Risk Reduction and Crisis Management
Social Media and the Internet undoubtedly give us helpful tools for crisis response and
management. That said, there are negatives that can sometimes outweigh the
positives.
1. Discuss the ways that the changing media landscape is affecting the psychosocial
consequences of disaster, for better or for worse.
2. Reflect on the ways that media (social or otherwise) positively and negatively impact
crisis management. How can we quell the rumor mill? How do we combat further
traumatization?
Please comment on at least 3 posts of your peers.
My post:
The use of social media in today’s society has significantly brought a lot of positives in disaster
mitigation and management. Social media has also been at the forefront of crisis elevation and
other social vises related to disaster elevation, such as undermining authority, spreading rumors,
and terrorist attacks (Alexander, 2013). Such vices in disaster development have been propagated
by the fast-changing media landscape. The psychological consequences of the disaster are
basically based on the way society interacts and views an instance of disaster psychologically.
Due to the lack of control of who posts what and when in social media platforms, gory images of
victims of disaster and mass suffering are being posted in such platforms which can severely
impact the psychological aspect of a lot of people. Such images and unedited news of death and
suffering as a result of a disaster posted on social media are bringing about depression, anxiety,
extreme stress, and other traumatic experiences. As such, the changing social media landscape is
affecting the psychological aspect of disaster for worse.
The general media is positively changing the way cases of disaster, and related crises are
managed. It has positively changed crisis management by promoting social cohesion, extending
cases of emergency responses, and monitoring crisis situations. Social and general media has
also negatively impact crisis management by spreading fake news, disseminating rumors, and
promoting acts of terrorism. This has also affected individuals that are already going through
psychological issues and increase their traumatic experience (Hopwood, and Schutte, 2017). It is
important for the government to intervene and come up with policies that shall see the regulation
of the use of social media so as to ensure instances of fake news and rumor mongering are
punished. This is the only way that the society shall be able to quell the rumor mill. With the
reduction of fake news, rumor mongering and introduction of policies on what to post and what
not to, traumatization shall reduce as individuals will not be exposed to images and news that
affect them psychologically.
References
Alexander, D. E. (2013). Social Media in Disaster Risk Reduction and Crisis
Management. Science and Engineering Ethics, 20(3), 717-733. doi:10.1007/s11948-013-9502-z
Hopwood, T. L., & Schutte, N. S. (2017). Psychological outcomes in reaction to media exposure
to disasters and large-scale violence: A meta-analysis. Psychology of Violence, 7(2), 316-327.
doi:10.1037/vio0000056
Student 1 post:
Technology is highly diverse. Digital media is a big part of the technology that is growing
up in this generation and became part of the granted social and cultural fabric of
learning, playing, and social communication. The development of smartphones and
computers made the media commonplace and universal. Users of social media can
share links with a broader network of people than the limited neighborhoods and
schools. Users of social media can connect with others who are far away and share
their interests, opinion, and experiences. Social media can be a tool for the community
to communicate. According to Xiao, Huang, & Wu (2015), In disaster management,
social media is increasingly being used for information dissemination and the
establishment of situational awareness. However, you can not control the posts and
comments on social media. Some posts can impact the psychological aspects of
victims. News of disasters can cause psychological harm.
It is essential to understand the psychological outcomes associated with media. Due to
disaster news in media, people may suffer from increased anxiety, depression, a
heightened sense of threat, and posttraumatic stress symptomology. The information
should be correct and up to date to minimize the rumors. Public information officer has
direct communication with the news, media, and public. In a time of crisis, public
information officers may face many issues that make the work more challenging. The
first issue is the overload of Information. Overload of Information can affect personal
well-being, decision making, innovation, and productivity. Moreover, the sensitivity and
the difficult information that have to share with their audiences. Some information can
cause psychological harm to the audiences.
The second issue is social media problems. social media are increasingly important,
and a very fast form of communication. However, during a crisis, the message that
posts it may translate into a different way and make issues like responsibility and blame.
the third issue happened when the report published, the public will ask questions to get
a more clear picture. What usually happens is the reporters decide what to tell their
viewers about what is occurring which may cause inaccurate information.
Xiao, Y., Huang, Q., & Wu, K. (2015). Understanding social media data for disaster
management. Natural Hazards, 79(3), 1663-1679. doi:10.1007/s11069-015-1918-0
Alexander, D. E. (2013). Social Media in Disaster Risk Reduction and Crisis
Management. Science and Engineering Ethics, 20(3), 717-733. doi:10.1007/s11948013-9502-z
Hopwood, T. L., & Schutte, N. S. (2017). Psychological outcomes in reaction to media
exposure to disasters and large-scale violence: A meta-analysis. Psychology of
Violence, 7(2), 316-327. doi:10.1037/vio0000056
Student 2 post:
1. The Psychosocial Consequences of Disaster
Contemporary use of media in disaster responses, emergencies and crisis is an area that has
attracted intense interest. Alexander (2014) burgeoned a variety of literature to create a robust
presentation of media in crisis response. Facebook, wiki pages, blogs, etc. have potential contact
psychological outcomes, both positively and negatively. Let us consider highly televised or
‘listened to’ news such as putting ‘fake news’ on trial or the 9/11 event, there is a significant
positive association between television viewing and psychiatric symptoms. Spending time
watching or reading about a disaster is highly associated with PTSD ceaseless and PTS.
Moreover, lifetime traumatic events such as impacts of death of a family member or a friend can
be controlled by frequently viewing the televised images. On the other hand, a meta-data analysis
conducted by Hopwood & Schulte (2017) showed a strong association of the same media
coverage to anxiety reactions. The study concludes exposure to violent events and disasters
causes negative psychological outcomes. Coverage of collective tragedies exacerbates distress
and a full cycle of depression. Further, this is associated with dreaming, complicated grief,
trigger cigarette smoking, and alcohol drinking.
2. The Media Impact of Crisis Management
Alexander (2014) appreciates seven positive different ways in which media coverage enhances
disaster, emergency and crisis situations. First, wide coverage exposes the disaster to a public
debate. Media makes it easy for concerned parties to monitor situations while extending
emergency responses and management to experts and other parties. Again, crowdsourcing and
collaborative development have become easy with the changing scope of media. Moreover,
social media is the best forum to extend causes and enhance social cohesion such as creating a
charitable fund donation. Someone that has effectively worked (examples include the great east
disaster in Japan and the Nepalese community in Australia). Lastly, social media jerks the
heterogeneity of sources to enhance further research.
References
Alexander, D. E. (2014). Social media in disaster risk reduction and crisis management. Science
and engineering ethics, 20(3), 717-733.
Barbaro M., (2018). Listen to ‘The Daily’: Putting ‘Fake News’ on Trial. Retrieved from
https://www.nytimes.com/2018/05/24/podcasts/the-daily/sandy-hook-alex-jones-infowarslawsuit.html?rref=collection%2Fcolumn%2Fthedaily&action=click&contentCollection=podcasts®ion=stream&module=stream_unit&version=la
test&contentPlacement=5&pgtype=collection
Hopwood, T. L., & Schutte, N. S. (2017). Psychological outcomes in reaction to media exposure
to disasters and large-scale violence: A meta-analysis. Psychology of violence, 7(2), 316.
Student 3 post:
Media over the last two decades has grown at an alarming rate. About twenty years ago,
before social media took off all we had were tv shows and the radio to give people a voice. While
print media was a thing, and still is today it doesn’t have the same effect as social media. These
social media platforms give people a voice to their own versions of stories and opinions. While
this idea is not necessarily wrong or bad, it is difficult when a disaster has occurred. The changing
of the media landscape has harbored both positive and negative effects on our psychosocial minds.
For one, since media, specifically social media cannot be silenced or controlled fully, fake news
sources spread like wildfires. When there are huge disasters, such as the Sandy Hook Massacre,
some people report conspiracies and de-value the victims and their parents’ experiences. By calling
these horrible situations ‘fake’, it brings distrust to news sites and pain for the families
experiencing these harsh moments. While there are negatives like distrust and false information,
there are some positives. With the development of media and technology, we can have the news
read or listened to only moments after its happened. With options on both television, radio, and
mobile devices to go ‘live,’ we are always in the loop. This can also be taken as a negative in a
disastrous moment because what we are watching ‘live’ may not be the full story which can spark
panic and outrage.
Crisis management has certain risks when using media, regardless if it is social, print, or
broadcasted. While there are risks, there are also blatant positives. Since crisis management works
with disaster relief, fact-checking, and listening to the public, social media can be used to properly
broadcast this information as needed. When there is a crisis and extra help is needed, a simple
tweet, document, or even announcement can make all the difference in selecting individuals to
help aid in disaster relief. Emergency warnings can also be pushed out to the public using media
when crisis management is involved. Although there are positives, there are also negatives. The
negatives deal with an ethical risk issue. If the information put out there is not correct, this leaves
room for more harm as people may panic. Ethical issues dealing with people and their right to
privacy but are often overpowered by the positive need for the news to be heard. Rumors can also
be spread when there are not enough people. This is a difficult task because humans tend to be
mistrusting. Crisis management can, however, produce and physically show evidence that falsifies
rumors and fear-filled claims. Building public trust and company or individual reputation is one
way to continue expanding the world of crisis management. As traumatizations occur, crisis
management should continue to spread true information with proper physical evidence that stays
ethical for those individuals involved.
Psychology of Violence
2017, Vol. 7, No. 2, 316 –327
© 2016 American Psychological Association
2152-0828/17/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/vio0000056
Psychological Outcomes in Reaction to Media Exposure to Disasters and
Large-Scale Violence: A Meta-Analysis
Tanya L. Hopwood and Nicola S. Schutte
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
University of New England
Objective: A quantitative meta-analysis set out to consolidate the effect of experimental studies of media
exposure to disasters and large-scale violence on negative psychological outcomes. Method: The
meta-analysis included 18 experimental studies with an overall sample size of 1,634 to obtain an overall
effect size and information regarding moderators of the effect size. Results: An overall significant and
large effect size of Hedges’ g of 1.61 showed that, across studies, media exposure to disasters and
large-scale violence was followed by negative psychological outcomes. Outcome type was a significant
moderator, with anxiety reactions showing an especially strong effect. Community sensitization was a
significant moderator, with studies conducted in a region that had recently been exposed to the type of
disaster or violence portrayed in the media showing especially large effect sizes. Conclusion: The results
indicate that media exposure to disasters and large-scale violence can cause negative psychological
outcomes, at least transiently. Limitations included a lack of statistical power in some moderator analyses
and the inability to draw inferences about the duration of effects. There is a need for further research
aimed at identifying the possible cumulative effects of media exposure and identification of groups at
greatest risk for harmful outcomes.
Keywords: media exposure, disaster, violence, meta-analysis
antecedent of posttraumatic symptoms (Houston, 2009; Pfefferbaum et al., 2014), and Murray (2008) claimed that the extant
research has supported three types of effects of televised violence—increased aggression, desensitization, and fear. Further,
Wilson (2008) concluded that children’s extensive use of screen
media (including violent material) may affect their socialization,
though these effects may be mediated by factors such as age,
gender, the extent to which they identify with the characters
depicted, and how real they believe the media content to be.
Although some studies have found either no effect or beneficial
effects of disaster-related media exposure (Linley, Joseph, Cooper,
Harris, & Meyer, 2003; Williams & Khan, 2011), and other
researchers have asserted that media exposure may only exacerbate preexisting symptoms of trauma (Ahern et al., 2002), the
majority of studies suggest that media consumption of disaster and
large-scale violence-related material may evoke psychological reactions similar to those experienced by direct victims of trauma
(Houston, 2009; Pfefferbaum et al., 2014; Slone & Shoshani,
2010). Longitudinal and survey studies found that people distally
located from the September 11 terrorist attacks of 2001 and exposed to the events primarily via mass media experienced significant personal threat and posttraumatic stress reactions (Callahan,
Hilsenroth, Yonai, & Waehler, 2005; Dougall, Hayward, & Baum,
2005; Schuster et al., 2001; Silver, Holman, McIntosh, Poulin, &
Gil-Rivas, 2002). Some studies found a positive association between hours of September 11 TV coverage consumed and stress
reactions (Blanchard et al., 2004; Schlenger et al., 2002), and other
researchers identified a link between disaster-focused distress and
perceived similarity to the victims as depicted by media (Wayment, 2004).
Although substantial research has been conducted in the area of
media exposure to disasters and large-scale violence, most of this
Advances in technology are affording people unprecedented
second-hand exposure to disasters and large-scale violence (Kaplan, 2008; Slone & Shoshani, 2010). Given the vast number of
people consuming modern media and the growing propensity
of news outlets to employ techniques such as rolling coverage of
disasters and large-scale violence as they occur (Jain, 2010; Kaplan, 2008), it is increasingly important for research to explore the
potential impact of threat-related content. The purpose of the
present study was to consolidate the effect of experimental studies
of media exposure to disasters and large-scale violence on negative
psychological outcomes.
There has been a long-standing academic debate as to whether
or not media with violent content can constitute exposure to
violence. Some scholars claim that much of the research linking
violent media to aggressive behaviors has drawn unfounded inferences of causation from largely correlational research (Grimes &
Bergen, 2008). In addition, the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual
of Mental Disorders (5th ed.; DSM–5; American Psychiatric Association, 2013) has amended the previous version (4th ed., text.
rev.; DSM–IV–TR; American Psychiatric Association, 2000) to
explicitly state that media consumption cannot constitute exposure
to trauma for a diagnosis of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD).
Bolstering the other side of the debate, numerous studies have
found evidence to suggest that media exposure may act as an
This article was published Online First May 5, 2016.
Tanya L. Hopwood and Nicola S. Schutte, School of Behavioural,
Cognitive, and Social Sciences, University of New England.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Nicola S.
Schutte, Psychology, University of New England, Psychology Lane, Armidale, Australia. E-mail: nschutte@une.edu.au
316
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
MEDIA EXPOSURE TO LARGE-SCALE VIOLENCE
research has employed self-report survey methods. These studies
provide valuable details and are rich in ecological validity, collecting information on how individuals have chosen to consume
media and retrospectively recording subjective reactions. Researchers have examined media forms such as TV, newspaper,
radio, and the Internet, and have looked at coverage of both
terrorist events and natural disasters. Many such studies have
found strong links between disaster-related media consumption
and negative psychological outcomes, including increased anxiety
(Schuster et al., 2001), fear and depression (Lachlan, Spence, &
Seeger, 2009), a heightened sense of threat (Maeseele, Verleye,
Stevens, & Speckhard, 2008), aggression (Argyrides & Downey,
2004), and posttraumatic stress symptomology (Pfefferbaum, Pfefferbaum, North, & Neas, 2002; Pfefferbaum et al., 2000; Schlenger
et al., 2002).
A recent descriptive research synthesis by Pfefferbaum et al.
(2014), which examined correlational studies of the relationship
between disaster-related media consumption and psychological
outcomes, found evidence of an association between TV viewing
of disaster news and negative outcomes such as posttraumatic
stress (PTS) symptoms, stress reactions, depression, and fear.
Similarly, a meta-analysis by Houston (2009) found a significant
overall effect size (r ⫽ .162) for the relationship between
terrorism-related media consumption and PTS. However, as with
the individual studies, the correlational nature of the data comprising these meta-analyses does not allow for inferences regarding
causality; people who watch traumatic events on the news may
consequently experience fear. Alternatively, people experiencing
fear may watch traumatic news, perhaps for information-seeking,
surveillance purposes, or reassurance.
Longitudinal studies that measured psychological dimensions
pre- and postdisasters (Cohen et al., 2006; Kennedy, Charlesworth,
& Chen, 2004; Otto et al., 2007; van Zelst, de Beurs, & Smit,
2003) have helped support the theory of media exposure to disasters and large-scale violence effecting negative psychological outcomes. These studies, although again high in ecological validity,
cannot control for a range of potentially confounding exposure
variables, and so are unable to isolate the effects of media.
Although many researchers have used experimental methods to
explore individual psychological reactions to media exposure to
disasters and large-scale threats, no meta-analysis of these studies
exists. By conducting a meta-analysis of experimental studies of
media exposure to disasters or large-scale violence, we aimed to
identify an overall effect size for psychological outcomes and also
consolidate information regarding the main types of reactions to
such media exposure. A review of the literature indicated that
anxiety (or stress) and anger are commonly measured outcomes
(Lerner, Gonzalez, Small, & Fischhoff, 2003; Pfefferbaum et al.,
2014). As pointed out by Slone and Shoshani (2010), the experience of these emotions is predicted by the theory of protection
motivation (Rogers, 1983). This theory asserts that when a person
interprets a situation as threatening, anxiety will often result. This
anxiety may promote a need to defend the self and others, which
may in turn lead to anger. We believe that this theory may help
provide a useful scaffold for understanding how media exposure to
disasters and large-scale violence may communicate a sense of
personal or community threat, which may in turn provoke reactions such as anxiety, anger, and other forms of negative affect.
317
Another theory that may add to the conceptual framework for
understanding people’s reactions to this type of media content is
the conservation of resources theory (Hobfoll & Lilly, 1993). The
conservation of resources theory claims that a person’s ability to
cope with challenges depends on his or her perceived inventory of
practical, social, and emotional resources. Maguen, Papa, and Litz
(2008) posited that large-scale threats (such as terrorism) intensify
perceptions of resource loss in areas such as self-esteem, selfefficacy, and internal locus of control. The loss of these safeguarding resources may increase levels of negative affect and
diminish adaptive coping (Moos & Holahan, 2003). Brewin, Andrews, and Valentine (2000) showed that many studies have supported a cascading effect of perceived resource loss, with multiple
stressors leading to increased vulnerability to further stress. If it
can be demonstrated that one-time media exposure to disasters or
large-scale violence plays a causal role in negative psychological
outcomes— even transiently—this may suggest the presence of
maladaptive and more enduring effects in some individuals as the
result of cumulative long-term exposure.
In the current meta-analysis, we predicted that across studies,
there would be a significant effect size for the impact of media
exposure to disasters or large-scale violence on negative psychological outcomes. There are a number of variables that we believed
might moderate outcomes across studies: intentionality of the
event portrayed, media format, whether the study was conducted
before or after September 11, community sensitization to trauma in
relation to the sample, and differences in participant sample gender
and age. The background for selection of these moderator variables
follows.
Intentionality of Event
Research has indicated that a disaster designed with human
intent (e.g., a terrorist attack) may be associated with a higher risk
of subsequent psychopathology than a disaster of accidental or
natural origin (DiMaggio & Galea, 2006). In addition, research
results have suggested that different forms of emotions may be
more common in the wake of accidental versus intentional trauma
(e.g., anger for intentional events, fear for random events; Rosoff,
John, & Prager, 2012). We examined whether portrayals of intentionally created disasters would result in stronger negative psychological outcomes.
Media Format
Communications research has provided evidence for the efficacy of video footage, compared with more traditional forms of
media such as newsprint or radio, in creating a more emotionally
arousing experience—a sense of realism that has been referred to
as presence (Lombard & Ditton, 1997). Graphic footage of disasters may evoke a sense of immediacy and engagement, and perhaps even a potent communication of threat (Callahan et al., 2005;
Cho et al., 2003). Meta-analytic studies and research syntheses of
correlational research (Houston, 2009; Pfefferbaum et al., 2014)
indicated that consumption of disaster or large-scale threat news
via TV is significantly associated with numerous negative psychological outcomes, including PTSD, PTS, depression, anxiety, and
anger. We examined whether video portrayal of disasters would be
associated with larger effect sizes.
HOPWOOD AND SCHUTTE
318
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
The Impact of September 11
Much research on media consumption of disasters and largescale violence occurred in the wake of the terrorist attacks in the
United States on September 11, 2001 (Neria & Sullivan, 2011).
The rolling coverage of these events in the media continued for
days, and then sporadically during the weeks and months that
followed, giving millions of people across the globe access to
details of the disaster (Pfefferbaum et al., 2002). The extensive
coverage and large scale of the mass casualties that occurred may
have evoked changes in belief systems related to safety and security for many individuals (Linley et al., 2003). We examined
whether post-September 11 studies would show stronger effect
sizes.
Community Sensitization to Trauma
Although some theorists contend that previous exposure to
trauma may serve as a form of inoculation against adversity
(Eysenck, 1983, as cited in Shrira, Palgi, Hamama-Raz, Goodwin,
& Ben-Ezra, 2014), many studies have indicated that a history of
previous trauma increases susceptibility to adverse psychological
outcomes, such as PTSD (Brewin et al., 2000; Chatard et al., 2012;
Shrira et al., 2014). Some global locations have had more recent
direct experience of disasters (e.g., war, terrorism, disease, and
natural disasters) than others. In the current meta-analysis, the
global region in which each experiment was conducted was coded
as a possible moderator variable. Also, an additional variable
called community sensitization was created to identify whether or
not the region in question had recently (within the previous 5
years) experienced a disaster of the type portrayed in the stimulus
material. We examined whether studies conducted in areas that
have recently experienced large-scale disaster or violence would
show larger effect sizes.
contended that children may be a particularly vulnerable population in terms of adverse PTS reactions (Comer & Kendall, 2007;
Dirkzwager, Kerssens, & Yzermans, 2006; Pfefferbaum, 1997;
Saylor, Cowart, Lipovsky, Jackson, & Finch, 2003). We examined
whether studies of adults with older participants would show
stronger effect sizes and whether studies with child participants
would show stronger effect sizes.
Current Study
The current study aimed to consolidate findings of experimental
research providing information regarding the causal impact of
disaster-related media exposure on psychological reactions through a
meta-analysis. Our predictions are as follows.
Hypothesis 1: Across studies, there is a significant effect size
for the impact of media exposure to disasters or large-scale
violence on negative psychological outcomes.
Hypothesis 2: Media portrayals involving intentionality result
in stronger negative psychological outcomes.
Hypothesis 3: Video portrayal results in larger effect sizes.
Hypothesis 4: Post-September 11 studies show larger effect
sizes.
Hypothesis 5: Studies conducted in areas recently experiencing large-scale disaster or violence show larger effect sizes.
Hypothesis 6: Studies with a higher percentage of females
show larger effect sizes.
Hypothesis 7: Studies with older participants or child participants show stronger effect sizes.
Method
Individual Differences
In the correlational research of media consumption of disasters
and large-scale violence and psychological outcomes, the most
widely replicated moderating variable is gender, with females
consistently demonstrating greater susceptibility to negative outcomes than males (Baum, Rahav, & Sharon, 2014). It has been
suggested that this gender effect may be partially accounted for by
women’s higher levels of fear for others (altruistic fear; Nellis &
Savage, 2012), heightened empathy (Nellis, 2009), increased levels of perceived vulnerability (Baum et al., 2014; Nellis, 2009), or
greater propensity to acknowledge distress (Lachlan, Spence, &
Nelson, 2010). We examined whether studies with a higher percentage of females would show stronger effect sizes.
Eysenck’s (1983) inoculation theory (as cited in Shrira et al.,
2014) suggests that older people have experienced more challenges across their lifetimes and have thus developed greater
resilience—a quality that may help protect them from the adverse
effects of further trauma. Some correlational studies have found
evidence to support these inoculation and maturation effects (e.g.,
Schlenger et al., 2002; Shrira et al., 2014). However, other studies
have found that elderly people may be more susceptible than
younger people to PTSD and other detrimental outcomes (e.g.,
Kun, Han, Chen, & Yao, 2009). Further, many researchers have
Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria
Studies were deemed eligible if they used an experimental
methodology in a controlled environment to measure individuals’
psychological outcomes in response to media coverage of disasters
or large-scale violence. The key terms were operationalized as
follows: media coverage (the independent variable [IV]) included
factual reports from TV, Internet, radio, newspaper articles, or
realistic simulations of any of these. Disasters or large-scale violence included major accidents (e.g., plane crashes or multiple road
accidents), natural disasters (e.g., hurricanes), acts of terrorism,
war or combat, climate change, economic crises, and crime with
the potential for casualties. Psychological outcomes (the dependent
variable [DV]) included state anxiety, negative affect, fear, or
perceived threat from the type of trauma in the exposure condition.
The DV could also include other negative outcomes— either emotional or cognitive—such as anger or blame, or positive outcomes
such as trust and empathy. One might expect outcomes such as
trust to decrease after some media exposure, such as coverage of
a terrorist attack, and other outcomes, such as empathy, to increase
after other media exposure, such as coverage of a natural disaster.
Eligible studies needed to either assign groups across the IV
(e.g., media exposure to disaster or large-scale threat content vs.
MEDIA EXPOSURE TO LARGE-SCALE VIOLENCE
neutral content) or use repeated measures, with pre- and postexposure measures on the DV. Given the meta-analysis aimed to
assess effect size variability across ages, genders, and locations,
eligible populations included the general public in any geographical region.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Search Strategies and Data Extraction
In June and July 2015, the PsycARTICLES, ProQuest, and
Summon bibliographic databases were used to carry out a systematic search for experimental studies measuring psychological outcomes in the context of media exposure to disasters and large-scale
violence. Keywords included “media coverage,” “media exposure,”
“television news,” “disaster media coverage,” “newspaper,” “internet
news,” “terrorism,” “war,” “natural disaster,” “virus,” “pandemic,”
“crime,” “anxiety,” “negative affect,” “fear,” “threat,” “anger,” “positive affect,” and “experiment.” A series of search expressions were
created for each disaster type to cover all relevant possibilities (for
example, terrorism AND [“media coverage” OR “television
news”] AND [anxiety OR “negative affect” OR fear OR threat OR
anger OR “positive affect”] AND experiment).
To ensure that the search was methodical and met recommended
guidelines for meta-analysis research, the PRISMA search protocol (Moher, Liberati, Tetzlaff, Altman, & PRISMA Group, 2009)
was used to record all articles identified, screened, and assessed for
eligibility. The reference list of each eligible article was reviewed
and assessed for additional relevant studies, and Google Scholar
was used to search for relevant research by identified experts in the
field. Separate eligible reports by the same research teams were
examined to ensure the use of independent data sets.
Finally, each study was assessed to determine whether adequate
information was provided to ensure suitability of the stimulus
material (exposure group media content), and whether the methodology was appropriate for the purpose of determining a causal
relationship between the IV and DV. Studies were excluded if
there were insufficient data to allow for the calculation of an effect
size of at least one relevant DV.
In total, 4,364 reports were identified through the database
searches, with an additional 137 reports obtained through other
sources, such as a search of literature cited in pertinent articles.
After removal of duplicate items, 2,565 records remained. Screening via title and abstract (and full text when necessary) resulted in
the exclusion of 2,523 reports that either did not employ an
experimental methodology, did not manipulate an appropriate
form of media exposure, or did not measure a relevant outcome. Of
the remaining 42 eligible reports, three were excluded because of
insufficient information regarding the stimulus media material, 20
were excluded because of methodologies that were incompatible
with the research question (e.g., correlational studies), and four
were excluded because of insufficient reported data to allow for
calculation of an effect size of at least one relevant DV. Ultimately, the search resulted in 15 reports containing 18 studies
suitable for inclusion in the meta-analysis (see Table 1). Some
groups that were not appropriate for testing our hypotheses (such
as those using a treatment condition prior to media exposure) were
excluded; the experimental groups excluded from each study and
the rationales for these decisions are outlined in Table 2. The
overall sample size was 1,634, with 959 females (58.7%) and 675
males (41.3%).
319
Publication Status of Studies
Several eligible unpublished reports were located during the
search phase but were excluded because of incompatible methodologies or insufficient information. Thus, all studies included in
the meta-analysis were published.
Coding Procedures
Following the recommendations of Cooper (2010), all studies
were coded separately by two raters. The coding was consistent
across raters (97% agreement) and disparities were resolved
through discussion.
Outcome types. The studies included in the meta-analysis
measured a variety of psychological outcomes, most considered
negative (e.g., state anxiety, anger, perceived threat), but some
with positive valence (e.g., trust and empathy). To allow us to
investigate whether type of outcome impacted effect size, each
outcome in each study was coded in one of six broad categories—
state anxiety, negative affect, fear, perceived threat, other negative
outcome (e.g., anger), and positive outcome.
Moderator variables. Based on the findings of correlational
research and the theories espoused by previous researchers (as
mentioned in the introduction), several study variables were coded
with the aim of assessing their influence on the effect size of
psychological outcomes. Moderator variables included intentionality (intentional, unintentional, mixture); media format (video,
audio, print, static images, mixed), date of study (pre- or postSeptember 11, 2001), global location (U.S., Middle East, Europe/
GB, Asia, Australia, other), community sensitization to disaster
type (exposure or no exposure to given disaster type within the
previous 5 years), gender breakdown (percentage of females), and
mean age of participants.
Study quality. Given the potentiality for study quality to
impact results, study design and the reliability of psychometric
measures (for DVs) were coded as moderator variables. Study
design was coded with four levels of empirical design: (a) pre–post
measures with no control group; (b) experimental and control
groups with no random assignment; (c) random assignment to
experimental and control group—posttest only; and (d) random
assignment to experimental and control group—pre- and postmeasures. The reliability of psychometric measures for the outcomes
(DVs) was coded as a continuous variable, with a mean Cronbach’s alpha calculated for outcomes in each study (five of the 18
studies had missing data for this variable).
Data Analysis
All data analyses were performed using Comprehensive MetaAnalysis Version 3 (CMA; Biostat, Inc., 2013). As most studies
reported results in the form of means (and standard deviations) for
exposure and control groups, and based on the recommendation of
Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins, and Rothstein (2009), Hedges’ g was
chosen for the effect size metric. The meta-analysis provided a
weighted mean summary effect size for negative psychological
outcomes. Positive outcomes (e.g., trust and empathy) were included but were entered to reflect an opposite-effect direction
when appropriate. For the main analysis, if outcomes of different
types were reported for the same group of participants in a study,
101
58
58
120
120
60
67
67
80
22
40
40
40
40
40
40
116
248
100
100
100
237
43
43
84
84
100
100
40
78
78
78
Barlett and Anderson (2014, Study 2)
Boyle (1984)
Boyle (1984)
Comer et al. (2008)
Comer et al. (2008)
Fischer et al. (2007, Study 2)
Fischer et al. (2007, Study 4)
Fischer et al. (2007, Study 4)
Fischer et al. (2010, Study 1)
Fischer et al. (2010, Study 3)
Fischer et al. (2011, Study 1)
Fischer et al. (2011, Study 1)
Fischer et al. (2011, Study 1)
Fischer et al. (2011, Study 2)
Fischer et al. (2011, Study 2)
Fischer et al. (2011, Study 2)
Lightstone et al. (2005–2006)
Ortiz et al. (2011)
Shoshani and Slone (2008)
Shoshani and Slone (2008)
Shoshani and Slone (2008)
Slone (2000)
Slone and Shoshani (2006)
Slone and Shoshani (2006)
Slone and Shoshani (2008)
Slone and Shoshani (2008)
Slone and Shoshani (2010)
Slone and Shoshani (2010)
Williams and Khan (2011)
Zeidner et al. (2011)
Zeidner et al. (2011)
Zeidner et al. (2011)
19.24
22.60
22.60
10.80g
10.80g
30.37
24.68
24.68
27.43
41.47
25.67
25.67
25.67
26.85
26.85
26.85
NR
9.24
22.65
22.65
22.65
34.70
22.65
22.65
22.65
22.65
23.00
23.00
NR
24.44
24.44
24.44
Mean age
73
83
83
61h
61h
52
81
81
63
73
50
50
50
50
50
50
78
54
50
50
50
50
50
50
50
50
50
50
50
59
59
59
%Femalea
Other (Econ.)
Acc./Nat.
Acc./Nat.
Terrorism
Terrorism
Terrorism
Mixture
Mixture
Terrorism
Terrorism
Terrorism
Terrorism
Terrorism
Terrorism
Terrorism
Terrorism
Terrorism
Acc./Nat.
Terrorism
Terrorism
Terrorism
Terrorism
Terrorism
Terrorism
Terrorism
Terrorism
Terrorism
Terrorism
Terrorism
Terrorism
Terrorism
Terrorism
Disaster type
U
U
U
I
I
I
M
M
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
U
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
Intent.b
Video
Video
Video
Video
Video
Print
Images
Images
Mixed
Images
Images
Images
Images
Print
Print
Print
Print
Video
Video
Video
Video
Video
Video
Video
Video
Video
Video
Video
Video
Video
Video
Video
Media
3
1
1
1
1
3
3
3
3
3
3
3
3
3
3
3
4
3
4
4
4
4
4
4
4
4
4
4
3
3
3
3
Study designc
U.S.
AUS
AUS
U.S.
U.S.
EUR/GB
EUR/GB
EUR/GB
EUR/GB
EUR/GB
EUR/GB
EUR/GB
EUR/GB
EUR/GB
EUR/GB
EUR/GB
U.S.
U.S.
ME
ME
ME
ME
ME
ME
ME
ME
ME
ME
U.S.
ME
ME
ME
Location
No
Yes
Yes
No
No
No
No
No
No
Yes
No
No
No
No
No
No
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
No
Yes
Yes
Yes
Sensitizationd
Neg.
SA
Neg.
SA
PT
PT
Neg.
Pos.
PT
PT
NA
PT
Pos.
NA
PT
Pos.
SA
SA
SA
Neg.
Pos.
SA
SA
Neg.
SA
Neg.
SA
Neg.
SA
NA
PT
Pos.
Outcome
.91
NR
NR
.88
.84
NR
.86
.87
.84
NR
.86
.79
.88
.86
NR
NR
.89
.85
.95
.94
.83
.94
.92
.91
.95
.94
.95
.94
.89
.90
.90
.94
Reliability (␣)e
1.028
.759
.532
.832
.341
.520
.401
.683i
.561
1.950
1.021
.598
.183i
.780
.456
.466i
.857
.427
11.034
8.241
6.182i
1.662
5.122
4.925
6.588
.617
10.823
7.009
⫺1.429
.768
.768
.447i
ESf
1.028
.607
.607
.504
.504
.520
.542
.542
.561
1.950
.496
.496
.496
.542
.542
.542
.857
.427
7.910
7.910
7.910
1.662
5.023
5.023
3.603
3.603
8.916
8.916
⫺1.429
.661
.661
.661
Mean ES
Note. NR ⫽ not reported; U.S. ⫽ United States of America; AUS ⫽ Australia; EUR/GB ⫽ Europe or Great Britain; ME ⫽ Middle East; Acc./Nat. ⫽ large-scale accident or natural disaster; Econ. ⫽
economic crisis; SA ⫽ state anxiety; NA ⫽ negative affect; PT ⫽ perceived threat; Neg. ⫽ other negative psychological outcomes; Pos. ⫽ positive psychological outcomes.
a
The percentage of females in the sample to the nearest whole percent. Studies reporting a fairly even gender breakdown were coded as 50%. b Intent. ⫽ intentionality; I ⫽ human agency intentional
event; U ⫽ unintentional event; M ⫽ mixture of intentional and unintentional events. c 1 ⫽ pre–post with no comparison group; 2 ⫽ experimental and control groups with no random assignment;
3 ⫽ random assignment experimental and control group—posttest only; 4 ⫽ random assignment experimental and control group—pre and post. d Sensitization indicates whether the location had a
history of the disaster type portrayed in the media within 5 years prior to study publication. e Cronbach’s alpha values for reported reliability were averaged when more than one measure for that
outcome type was recorded for the study. f Raw effect size for the outcome type, assuming independence of the data for each outcome. Raw effect sizes are shown averaged when more than one
measure for the outcome type (e.g., for type other negative effect) was recorded for the study. g The mean age for child participants in the study (n ⫽ 90); a subgroup of mothers was also included
(n ⫽ 30), but no age data were reported. h The percentage of females across both child and adult participants in the study (48% for child sample; 100% for adult sample). i For positive outcomes,
a high score indicates a less positive outcome.
n
Authors (year, study no.)
Table 1
Characteristics of Included Studies and Effect Sizes
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
320
HOPWOOD AND SCHUTTE
MEDIA EXPOSURE TO LARGE-SCALE VIOLENCE
321
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Table 2
Sample Numbers Used and Excluded From Each Study and Exclusion Reason
Authors (year, study no.)
n used
n excluded
Barlett and Anderson (2014, Study 2)
Boyle (1984)
Comer et al. (2008)
Fischer et al. (2007, Study 2)
Fischer et al. (2007, Study 4)
101
58
120 (90 children,
30 mothers)
60
45
Fischer et al. (2010, Study 1)
Fischer et al. (2010, Study 3)
Fischer et al. (2011, Study 1)
80
22 parents
40
Fischer et al. (2011, Study 2)
Lightstone et al. (2005)
Ortiz et al. (2011)
Shoshani and Slone (2008)
40
116
248
100
20
0
0
200
Slone (2000)
Slone and Shoshani (2006)
237
43
0
77
Slone and Shoshani (2008)
84
84
Slone and Shoshani (2010)
100
200
Williams and Khan (2011)
Zeidner et al. (2011)
40
78
0
0
Exclusion reason
0
0
60 mothers
Excluded mothers who were given experimental
training premedia exposure.
0
22 for each comparison
test
0
22 children
20
the outcomes were averaged. Raw effect sizes (assuming independence of outcomes) for each coded outcome type were also calculated and are reported in Table 1). Subgroup analyses (for the
categorical moderator variables) and metaregressions (for the continuous variables) were based on averaged outcomes for those
studies with multiple outcomes.
A subgroup moderator analysis was conducted to find weighted
effect sizes for each type of psychological outcome represented
(e.g., state anxiety, negative affect, positive outcomes). In order to
make full use of available information regarding different types of
outcomes, in this analysis, each outcome from studies reporting
information for multiple outcomes was used.
According to Borenstein et al. (2009), a random effects model is
the appropriate computational model for most meta-analyses in the
social sciences, in which it usually cannot be assumed that the
primary studies will yield the same effect size. In contrast, a fixed
effects model is appropriate when it can be assumed that all studies
are measuring the same effect in the same population. In the case
of the current meta-analysis, given the variations in study design,
participant groups, locations, and outcomes measured, the effect
sizes were expected to vary and a random effects model was used.
Both experimental groups compared individually
against control (neutral media) group.
Children did not view media.
Meaning group excluded, as this could be
interpreted as an amelioration intervention that
would affect outcome.
As above.
Excluded participants in experimental amelioration
groups (intervention prior to media exposure).
Excluded participants in experimental amelioration
groups (intervention prior to media exposure).
Excluded participants in experimental amelioration
groups (intervention was post media exposure
but Time 2 outcomes recorded after
intervention).
Excluded participants in both pre- and
postexperimental amelioration groups.
indicates that approximately 96% of the variance across studies is
the result of difference in the true effect sizes, rather than sampling
error. The high heterogeneity found here also supports the decision
to explore potential moderator variables.
Results
Overall Mean Effect Size
In order to test the major hypothesis that media exposure to
disasters or large-scale violence results in negative psychological
outcomes, a mean effect size was calculated for all studies included in the meta-analysis (k ⫽ 18). All effect sizes are presented
as standard mean differences in the metric of Hedges’ g (see Table
1). All except one study (Williams & Khan, 2011, with
g ⫽ ⫺1.43) showed a mean effect size indicating an increase in
strength in negative psychological outcomes. The overall mean
weighted effect size for negative psychological outcomes was
large, g ⫽ 1.61 (standard error [SE] ⫽ 0.27, 95% confidence
interval [CI] [1.07, 2.14], p ⬍ .001), indicating that across studies
media exposure to disasters or large-scale violence had a significant effect on negative psychological outcomes.
Heterogeneity Analyses
Inspection of heterogeneity statistics revealed a significant Q
statistic (Q ⫽ 378.53, p ⬍ .001) and a high I2 index (p ⬍ .001,
I2 ⬎ 75%). These results indicate high heterogeneity, with the
effect sizes across studies varying significantly (Cochran, 1954;
Higgins & Thompson, 2002). This variation supports the decision
to use a random effects model, as the I2 index (I2 ⫽ 95.51%)
Moderator Analyses
Method of moments metaregression examined the association
between percentage of females in samples, reliability of measures,
and mean age of samples with effect size. The percentage of females
was significantly associated with the effect size (slope ⫽ ⫺0.059,
SE ⫽ 0.025, 95% CI [⫺0.108, ⫺0.009], Z ⫽ ⫺2.31, p ⬍ .05). These
HOPWOOD AND SCHUTTE
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
322
results indicate that the higher the percentage of female participants, the weaker the impact of media exposure on negative
psychological outcomes. However, the regression slope indicated
only a very small association. The metaregression for reliability of
measures also found a significant association (slope ⫽ ⫺0.059,
SE ⫽ 0.025, 95% CI [⫺0.108, ⫺0.009], Z ⫽ ⫺2.31, p ⬍ .05).
These results indicate that the higher the mean reliability level of
measures in each study, the stronger the effect of media exposure
on negative psychological outcomes.
Using a linear metaregression, the mean age of samples was not
significantly associated with the effect size (Z ⫽ 0.30, p ⫽ .76),
indicating that a change in mean age of participants did not
correspond with a linear change in psychological outcomes. Because we predicted a nonlinear relationship (with effects for both
children and older people predicted to be greater than young or
middle aged people), a curvilinear metaregression using a quadratic model was used to test this hypothesis. The curvilinear
model, although not significant (Z ⫽ ⫺1.60, p ⫽ .11), approached
a better fit for the data than did the linear model.
Results of the categorical moderator analyses are shown in
Table 3. Intentionality of the event portrayed did not moderate the
effect size, Q(1) ⫽ 2.73, p ⫽ .098. The effect size difference
between the pre- and post-September 11 groups also was not
significant Q(1) ⫽ 0.52, p ⫽ .47. However, this test lacked power,
with only two studies being conducted prior to September 11,
2001.
Grouping by media format did not yield a significant difference
across effect sizes, Q(3) ⫽ 5.99, p ⫽ .112. As one subcategory of
this variable was a mixture of the other formats, a repeat analysis
excluded the study that used a mixed media format and yielded a
result trending toward significance, Q(2) ⫽ 5.15, p ⫽ .076. Given
that some subgroups were underrepresented (no studies used audio, three used print, and three used images), these results should
be interpreted with caution.
Global location significantly moderated the effect size, Q(3) ⫽
36.17, p ⬍ .001, as did sensitization to disaster type, Q(1) ⫽ 18.54,
p ⬍ .001. The results showed that studies conducted in the Middle
East showed the highest effect sizes. The results also indicated that
studies conducted in communities with a recent history (within the
previous 5 years) of the type of disaster portrayed showed larger
effect sizes than did studies conducted in communities without this
type of threat salience.
Given that the studies in the meta-analysis measured different
types of psychological outcomes, we examined whether the type of
outcome moderated the effect size. As the mean effect across
outcomes for studies with multiple outcomes could not be used
here, we ran this analysis with the assumption of independence of
outcomes within each study (see Table 4). This assumption of a
zero correlation between outcomes represents a conservative approach—it inflates the p value, lowering the statistical power to
detect heterogeneity (Borenstein et al., 2009). The result of this
moderator analysis was significant, Q(4) ⫽ 24.65, p ⬍ .001,
indicating that the strength of the effect varied across the types of
outcomes measured. State anxiety, the most commonly measured
outcome type (k ⫽ 12), was associated with the strongest effect,
g ⫽ 3.11, SE ⫽ 0.38, 95% CI [2.368, 3.849], Z ⫽ 8.23, p ⬍ .001.
Publication Bias
A potential source of bias in meta-analyses is publication bias,
the potential for studies with significant results to be more abundant in the published literature and more easily located by re-
Table 3
Results of Categorical Moderator Analyses Using Mean Outcomes for Participants
Category
Intentionality, Q(1) ⫽ 2.73, p ⫽ .098
Intentional
Unintentional
Pre- or post-September 11, Q(1) ⫽ .41, p ⫽ .523
Pre-September 11
Post-September 11
Media format, Q(2) ⫽ 5.15, p ⫽ .076
Video
Print
Static images
Global location, Q(3) ⫽ 36.17, p ⫽ .000
U.S.
Middle East
Europe/Great Britain
Australia
Sensitization, Q(1) ⫽ 18.54, p ⫽ .000
Yes
No
Study design, Q(2) ⫽ 40.03, p ⫽ .000
Pre–post, no control group
Random assignment, post only
Random assignment, pre–post
g
95% CI
Z
p
k
1.939
.686
[1.289, 2.588]
[.650, 2.022]
5.85ⴱ
1.01
.000
.315
14
3
1.134
1.699
[⫺.490, 2.758]
[1.097, 2.301]
1.37
5.53ⴱ
.171
.000
2
16
2.229
.642
.965
[1.478, 2.979]
[⫺.748, 2.032]
[⫺.460, 2.390]
5.82ⴱ
.91
1.33
.000
.366
.184
11
3
3
.299
4.087
.744
.607
[⫺.676, 1.275]
[3.115, 5.060]
[⫺.167, 1.656]
[⫺1.549, 2.764]
.60
8.24ⴱ
1.60
.55
.548
.000
.109
.591
5
6
6
1
2.702
.356
[1.972, 3.432]
[⫺.424, 1.136]
7.26ⴱ
.89
.000
.371
10
8
.556
.519
4.066
[⫺.875, 1.987]
[⫺.140, 1.178]
[3.147, 4.985]
.76
1.54
8.67ⴱ
.446
.123
.000
2
10
6
Note. g ⫽ point estimate of the effect size (Hedges’ g), using the mean of outcomes reported; CI ⫽ confidence interval; the 95% lower and upper limits
of g; Z ⫽ z test for g; k ⫽ the number of studies associated with the g value; Q ⫽ test statistic that determines whether the effect varies significantly between
the subcategories of the moderator variable (random effects model used).
ⴱ
p ⬍ .05.
MEDIA EXPOSURE TO LARGE-SCALE VIOLENCE
323
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Table 4
Outcome Type Moderator Analysis With Multiple Outcomes for Some Participants
Category
g
95% CI
Z
p
k
Outcome type (total k ⫽ 45),
Q(4) ⫽ 24.65, p ⫽ .000
State anxiety
Negative affect
Perceived threat
Other negative outcome
Positive outcome
3.11
.86
.60
2.27
1.63
[2.368, 3.849]
[⫺.553, 2.263]
[⫺.162, 1.371]
[1.516, 3.032]
[.805, 2.447]
8.23ⴱ
1.19
1.55
5.88
3.88
.000
.234
.122
.000
.000
12
3
10
11
9
Note. g ⫽ point estimate of the effect size (Hedges’ g); CI ⫽ confidence interval; the 95% lower and upper
limits of g; Z ⫽ z test for g; k ⫽ the number of outcomes associated with the g value; Q ⫽ test statistic that
determines whether the effect varies significantly between the subcategories of the moderator variable (random
effects model used).
ⴱ
p ⬍ .05.
searchers than studies reporting no effects (Borenstein et al., 2009;
Cooper, 2010; Rosenthal, 1979). To check for publication bias,
Duval and Tweedie’s (2000) trim and fill procedure with funnel
plot and a classic fail-safe N test (Rosenthal, 1979) were conducted
using CMA. The funnel plot showed little asymmetry, and Duval
and Tweedie’s trim and fill found no missing studies to the left of
the mean, indicating no likelihood of publication bias. The failsafe N value (1,681) far exceeded the minimum recommended
critical value of 100 (given by 5k ⫹ 10, where k is the number of
studies in the meta-analysis; Rosenthal, 1991). This suggests that
one would need to add 1,681 studies that found no effect of media
exposure on psychological outcomes to render the effect nonsignificant. Given these findings, there is no evidence of publication
bias.
Discussion
A large meta-analytic effect size showed that, across experimental studies, media exposure to disasters and large-scale violence
resulted in increased negative psychological outcomes. This result,
supporting the main hypothesis of the meta-analysis, suggests that
such media exposure results in negative psychological outcomes.
The finding of a causal impact across experimental studies adds to
previous findings generated by correlational and longitudinal studies.
Of the categorical moderators of effect size examined, community sensitization, global region, outcome type, and study design
all had significant associations with negative psychological outcomes. For continuous moderator variables, percentage of females
and reliability of outcome measures both reached significance. No
statistically significant differences were found for the other moderator variables.
Hypothesis 2, that media portrayals involving intentionality
result in stronger negative psychological outcomes, was not supported, even though there was a weak trend toward significance,
with intentionally caused disaster or violence having a larger
effect. Hypothesis 3, that video portrayal results in larger effect
sizes, also was not significant, but trended toward significance,
with video footage evoking a stronger negative psychological
outcome than print or static images. This trend is consistent with
previous research supporting the ability of video to imbue the
consumer with a more emotionally arousing experience and per-
haps a potent communication of threat (Callahan et al., 2005; Cho
et al., 2003). Hypothesis 4, that post-September 11 studies would
show larger effect sizes, was not supported.
Hypothesis 5 focused on community sensitization, in that we
expected that studies conducted in areas recently experiencing
large-scale disaster or violence show larger effect sizes. Analysis
of the moderator variable community sensitization showed that
effect sizes were significantly larger for studies conducted in a
region that had recently been exposed to the type of disaster or
threat portrayed in the media. This finding further bolsters the
theory of cumulative adverse effects, in line with the conservation
of resources theory, and is also consistent with research on complex trauma and poly victimization. A study conducted by Turner,
Finkelhor, and Ormrod (2010) indicated that multiple incidences
of trauma for children (across varying types of victimization) was
linked to adverse psychological outcomes. The results suggested
that cumulative episodes of adversity may be more strongly related
to negative outcomes than are single events or even repeated
occurrences of the same type of trauma. If, as the current results
suggest, media coverage of disasters and traumatic events has the
potential to cause transient negative psychological changes in
individuals, then it is possible that cumulative effects over time (or
intense prolonged exposure) may generate more serious adverse
outcomes.
Related to the community sensitization hypothesis, effect sizes
varied across global regions. Studies conducted in the Middle East
showed much higher effect sizes than studies from other regions.
Given the historical incidences of war and terrorist attacks in this
region (Solomon, Gelkopf, & Bleich, 2005), these results suggest
that the cumulative effects of this type of trauma may render
people more susceptible toward negative reactions to media exposure to disaster and large-scale violence. Again, this is consistent
with previous research showing that multiple stressors over time or
a history of trauma may render people more vulnerable to mental
health disorders such as PTSD and anxiety or mood disorders
(Brewin et al., 2000; Myers et al., 2015). Similarly, a metaanalysis of studies examining the behavioral consequences of
terrorism showed that a history of diagnosed mental health problems was strongly associated with PTSD following a terrorist
incident (DiMaggio & Galea, 2006).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
324
HOPWOOD AND SCHUTTE
Hypothesis 6, predicting that studies with a higher percentage of
females show larger effect sizes, was not supported. In the current
study, the metaregression using percentage of females as a covariate provided a statistically significant model, but the association
was small and not in the expected direction. This is not consistent
with gender effects having been found frequently in previous
research (e.g., Baum et al., 2014), with females found to be more
at risk of adverse outcomes related to media exposure and to other
forms of indirect exposure to trauma. Future research might further
explore the effect of gender on reaction to media exposure.
Hypothesis 7 predicted that studies with older participants or
child participants would show stronger effect sizes. The metaregressions for mean age— using both linear and curvilinear models— did not reach significance. As a group, children were underrepresented in the meta-analysis, with only two studies using child
participants. Similarly, older adults were not well represented, with
the highest mean age for a study being 41.47. Given this lack of
variability in mean age, these findings should be interpreted with
caution. These results can be viewed in the context of previous
research that has presented mixed results regarding age effects on
outcomes relating to media and other forms of indirect exposure to
trauma (e.g., Kun et al., 2009; Shrira et al., 2014).
Exploratory moderator analyses investigated effects for different outcomes and aspects of methodology. The included studies
measured a variety of psychological outcomes. State anxiety was
the most measured and strongest outcome. This result is consistent
with correlational research reporting a link between media consumption of disaster-related content and anxiety (Cohen et al.,
2006; Schlenger et al., 2002; Schuster et al., 2001; Sugimoto et al.,
2013). The overall effect size for perceived threat was not significant, which is inconsistent with many previous correlational research findings (Fahmy & Johnson, 2007; Nabi & Sullivan, 2001;
Nellis & Savage, 2012; Ridout, Grosse, & Appleton, 2008) and
what would be predicted based on protection motivation theory.
One possible explanation for this may be the laboratory conditions
attached to the studies; perhaps the artificial environment reminded participants that the media content was not necessarily
temporally or personally relevant. Thus, although the media content may have evoked negative emotions and anxiety, a perceived
lack of personal involvement or perceived reality may have reduced estimations of threat (Roser & Thompson, 1995; Wilson,
2008).
In relation to methodology, both study design and reliability of
measures were significant moderators. Studies with random assignment to groups and pre–post measurement showed stronger
effect sizes. Studies using measures with greater reliability showed
stronger effect sizes. These results suggest that the more exact the
methodology, the larger the effect sizes, and bolster confidence in
the overall effect found in the meta-analysis.
The findings of the current study partially support the proposed
theoretical framework based on protection motivation theory and
conservation of resources theory. As predicted by protection motivation theory, the significant and strong overall effect size for
state anxiety is consistent with expectations of anxiety and stress
arising in the wake of threatening events. However, the failure of
the overall effect size for perceived threat to reach significance is
inconsistent with a model of perceived threat being a necessary
precursor to anxiety. This suggests that there are additional path-
ways between the consumption of the media report and feelings of
anxiety or stress.
An improved conceptual framework might include mechanisms
such as emotional contagion—the process whereby individuals
tend to unconsciously mimic the affect of others, producing an
authentic subjective experience of the emotion (Hatfield, Cacioppo, & Rapson, 1993). Watching the suffering of other people
may evoke feelings of empathy and identification, thus facilitating
the process of emotional contagion, evoking feelings of anxiety
and stress to mirror those of depicted victims (Nummenmaa,
Hirvonen, Parkkola, & Hietanen, 2008).
Conservation of resources theory predicts that a cumulative
burden of adversity will diminish an individual’s perceived store
of psychological resources and increase vulnerability to future
trauma. This part of the conceptual framework was supported by
the current results relating to community sensitization and is
consistent with recent findings regarding complex trauma and poly
victimization (Turner et al., 2010). These findings support the
possibility of negative outcomes amassing in individuals as the
result of repeated exposure to media reports of disasters or largescale violence.
Limitations
A large number of data sets were provided by two teams and,
although we took care to ensure that the data sets themselves were
independent, the results should be interpreted with caution. It
should be noted that the largest effect sizes appear to be impacted
by the moderator variables shown to be associated with greater
effects (e.g., Middle East location, intentional acts, video presentation, community sensitization), a pattern that is consistent with
our major findings. When considering the results from the different
teams, it may be useful to be mindful of the significant differences
between the populations studied.
The lack of studies in some subgroups meant that several
moderator analyses lacked statistical power and thus may have
failed to detect real differences. For example, in relation to the preor post-September 11 moderator, only two studies were conducted
before September 11. Most of the studies included in the metaanalysis were conducted in laboratory settings, used one-time
exposure to the stimulus material, and measured immediate psychological outcomes. Although this methodology is beneficial in
terms of minimizing the influence of confounding variables, it
reduces ecological validity. People may have different reactions in
real-world situations because of autonomy in selection of media
content, social support, and a myriad of other variables that may
affect outcomes. The included studies examined only short-term
effects of trauma-related media exposure. Longer detrimental (or
beneficial) outcomes were not assessed because of the time-limited
nature of the experimental designs used. Thus, it is unknown how
transient or enduring these effects may be.
Research Implications
Future experimental studies with a longitudinal component
could assess the longevity of psychological responses to media
exposure, particularly cumulative effects. Future studies might
also identify groups at risk for intense or longer lasting effects,
including potential effects for children and elderly adults. Finally,
MEDIA EXPOSURE TO LARGE-SCALE VIOLENCE
future studies might investigate the impact of understudied media
modalities, such as audio presentations, and emerging future modalities, such as virtual reality presentations.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Clinical and Policy Implications
In conjunction with correlational research linking media consumption with anxiety, posttraumatic stress symptoms, stress reactions, depression, anger, and fear (Houston, 2009; Pfefferbaum
et al., 2014), the results of the present meta-analysis of experimental studies suggest that media consumption can be a source of
psychological distress. Clinicians might consider discussing media
consumption with distressed clients to assess whether types of
media viewed may be a source of distress for clients. Media
professionals might consider the impact of media coverage on
consumers, especially on viewers who might be sensitized to
experience negative reactions, particularly viewers in areas experiencing disasters or violence such as that portrayed in the media
coverage.
Conclusion
People have more access than ever before to graphic media
coverage of disasters and large-scale violence. Given the ubiquitous nature of bad news in the modern world, it is important to
understand the psychological outcomes associated with exposure
to this type of media. The results of the current meta-analysis
suggest that media exposure to disasters or large-scale violence
plays a causal role in negative psychological outcomes, at least
transiently. Further, people in communities with a recent history of
disaster or large-scale violence may experience greater negative
psychological outcomes when exposed to media portrayals of
similar events. Additional research is needed to determine which
populations are most at risk for negative reactions and what factors
may attenuate these effects. Future research should also explore
the potential consequences of cumulative effects or prolonged
exposure to media coverage of disasters or large-scale violence,
particularly for vulnerable individuals.
References
References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the
meta-analysis.
Ahern, J., Galea, S., Resnick, H., Kilpatrick, D., Bucuvalas, M., Gold, J.,
& Vlahov, D. (2002). Television images and psychological symptoms
after the September 11 terrorist attacks. Psychiatry: Interpersonal and
Biological Processes, 65, 289 –300. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/psyc.65.4
.289.20240
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Argyrides, M., & Downey, J. L. (2004). September 11: Immediate and long
term effects on measures of aggression, prejudice, and person perception. North American Journal of Psychology, 6, 175–187. Retrieved
from http://mwc.sagepub.com.ezproxy.une.edu.au/content/1/1/50
ⴱ
Barlett, C. P., & Anderson, C. A. (2014). Bad news, bad times, and
violence: The link between economic distress and aggression. Psychology of Violence, 4, 309 –321. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0034479
Baum, N., Rahav, G., & Sharon, M. (2014). Heightened susceptibility to
secondary traumatization: A meta-analysis of gender differences. Amer-
325
ican Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 84, 111–122. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1037/h0099383
Biostat, Inc. (2013). Comprehensive meta-analysis (Version 3) [Computer
software]. Englewood, NJ: Author.
Blanchard, E. B., Kuhn, E., Rowell, D. L., Hickling, E. J., Wittrock, D.,
Rogers, R. L., . . . Steckler, D. C. (2004). Studies of the vicarious
traumatization of college students by the September 11th attacks: Effects
of proximity, exposure and connectedness. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 42, 191–205. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967
(03)00118-9
Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R. (2009).
Introduction to meta-analysis. Chichester, UK: Wiley. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1002/9780470743386
ⴱ
Boyle, G. J. (1984). Effects of viewing a road trauma film on emotional
and motivational factors. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 16, 383–
386. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0001-4575(84)90051-4
Brewin, C. R., Andrews, B., & Valentine, J. D. (2000). Meta-analysis of
risk factors for posttraumatic stress disorder in trauma-exposed adults.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 748 –766. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.68.5.748
Callahan, K. L., Hilsenroth, M. J., Yonai, T. A. L., & Waehler, C. A.
(2005). Longitudinal stress responses to the 9/11 terrorist attacks in a
New York metropolitan college sample. Stress, Trauma and Crisis: An
International Journal, 8, 45– 60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1543
4610590913621
Chatard, A., Pyszczynski, T., Arndt, J., Selimbegović, L., Konan, P. N. D.,
& Van der Linden, M. (2012). Extent of trauma exposure and PTSD
symptom severity as predictors of anxiety-buffer functioning. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 4, 47–55.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0021085
Cho, J., Boyle, M. P., Keum, H., Shevy, M. D., McLeod, D. M., Shah,
D. V., & Pan, Z. (2003). Media, terrorism, and emotionality: Emotional
differences in media content and public reactions to the September 11th
terrorist attacks. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 47, 309 –
327. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15506878jobem4703_1
Cochran, W. G. (1954). The combination of estimates from different
experiments. Biometrics, 10, 101–129. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/
3001666
Cohen, P., Kasen, S., Chen, H., Gordon, K., Berenson, K., Brook, J., &
White, T. (2006). Current affairs and the public psyche: American
anxiety in the post 9/11 world. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 41, 251–260. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00127-006-0033-7
ⴱ
Comer, J. S., Furr, J. M., Beidas, R. S., Weiner, C. L., & Kendall, P. C.
(2008). Children and terrorism-related news: Training parents in coping
and media literacy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76,
568 –578. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.76.4.568
Comer, J. S., & Kendall, P. C. (2007). Terrorism: The psychological
impact on youth. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 14, 179 –
212. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2850.2007.00078.x
Cooper, H. M. (2010). Research synthesis and meta-analysis: A step-bystep approach (4th ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
DiMaggio, C., & Galea, S. (2006). The behavioral consequences of terrorism: A meta-analysis. Academic Emergency Medicine, 13, 559 –566.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1553-2712.2006.tb01008.x
Dirkzwager, A. J. E., Kerssens, J. J., & Yzermans, C. J. (2006). Health
problems in children and adolescents before and after a man-made
disaster. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent
Psychiatry, 45, 94 –103. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/01.chi.0000186402
.05465.f7
Dougall, A. L., Hayward, M. C., & Baum, A. (2005). Media exposure to
bioterrorism: Stress and the anthrax attacks. Psychiatry, 68, 28 – 42.
Duval, S., & Tweedie, R. (2000). Trim and fill: A simple funnel-plot-based
method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
326
HOPWOOD AND SCHUTTE
Biometrics, 56, 455– 463. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0006-341X.2000
.00455.x
Fahmy, S., & Johnson, T. J. (2007). Mediating the anthrax attacks: Media
accuracy and agenda setting during a time of moral panic. Atlantic
Journal of Communication, 15, 19 – 40. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
15456870701212583
ⴱ
Fischer, P., Fischer, J., Frey, D., Such, M., Smyth, M., Tester, M., &
Kastenmuller, A. (2010). Causal evidence that terrorism salience increases authoritarian parenting practices. Social Psychology, 41, 246 –
254. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000033
ⴱ
Fischer, P., Greitemeyer, T., Kastenmüller, A., Frey, D., & Owald, S.
(2007). Terror salience and punishment: Does terror salience induce
threat to social order? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43,
964 –971. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2006.10.004
ⴱ
Fischer, P., Postmes, T., Koeppl, J., Conway, L., & Fredriksson, T.
(2011). The meaning of collective terrorist threat: Understanding the
subjective causes of terrorism reduces its negative psychological impact.
Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 26, 1432–1445. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1177/0886260510369137
Grimes, T., & Bergen, L. (2008). The epistemological argument against a
causal relationship between media violence and sociopathic behavior
among psychologically well viewers. American Behavioral Scientist, 51,
1137–1154. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0002764207312008
Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L. (1993). Emotional contagion.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2, 96 –99. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1111/1467-8721.ep10770953
Higgins, J. P., & Thompson, S. G. (2002). Quantifying heterogeneity in a
meta-analysis. Statistics in Medicine, 21, 1539 –1558. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1002/sim.1186
Hobfoll, S. E., & Lilly, R. S. (1993). Resource conservation as a strategy
for community psychology. Journal of Community Psychology, 21,
128 –148. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/1520-6629(199304)21:2⬍128::
AID-JCOP2290210206⬎3.0.CO;2-5
Houston, J. B. (2009). Media coverage of terrorism: A meta-analytic
assessment of media use and posttraumatic stress. Journalism & Mass
Communication Quarterly, 86, 844 – 861. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
107769900908600408
Jain, A. (2010). “Beaming it live”: 24-hour television news, the spectator
and the spectacle of the 2002 Gujarat carnage. South Asian Popular
Culture, 8, 163–179. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14746681003797989
Kaplan, E. A. (2008). Global trauma and public feelings: Viewing images
of catastrophe. Consumption Markets & Culture, 11, 3–24. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1080/10253860701799918
Kennedy, C., Charlesworth, A., & Chen, J.-L. (2004). Disaster at a distance: Impact of 9.11.01 televised news coverage on mothers’ and
children’s health. Journal of Pediatric Nursing, 19, 329 –339. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1016/j.pedn.2004.09.003
Kun, P., Han, S., Chen, X., & Yao, L. (2009). Prevalence and risk factors
for posttraumatic stress disorder: A cross-sectional study among survivors of the Wenchuan 2008 earthquake in China. Depression and Anxiety, 26, 1134 –1140. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/da.20612
Lachlan, K. A., Spence, P. R., & Nelson, L. D. (2010). Gender differences
in negative psychological responses to crisis news: The case of the
I-35W collapse. Communication Research Reports, 27, 38 – 48. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1080/08824090903293601
Lachlan, K. A., Spence, P. R., & Seeger, M. (2009). Terrorist attacks and
uncertainty reduction: Media use after September 11. Behavioral Sciences of Terrorism and Political Aggression, 1, 101–110. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1080/19434470902771683
Lerner, J. S., Gonzalez, R. M., Small, D. A., & Fischhoff, B. (2003).
Effects of fear and anger on perceived risks of terrorism: A national field
experiment. Psychological Science, 14, 144 –150. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1111/1467-9280.01433
ⴱ
Lightstone, S. N., Swencionis, C., & Cohen, H. W. (2005–2006). The
effect of bioterrorism messages on anxiety levels. International Quarterly of Community Health Education, 24, 111–122. http://dx.doi.org/10
.2190/1YWV-JEDN-BQFP-NEXH
Linley, P. A., Joseph, S., Cooper, R., Harris, S., & Meyer, C. (2003).
Positive and negative changes following vicarious exposure to the September 11 terrorist attacks. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 16, 481– 485.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1025710528209
Lombard, M., & Ditton, T. (1997). At the heart of it all: The concept of
presence. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 3. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1111/j.1083-6101.1997.tb00072.x
Maeseele, P. A., Verleye, G., Stevens, I., & Speckhard, A. (2008). Psychosocial resilience in the face of a mediated terrorist threat. Media, War
& Conflict, 1, 50 – 69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1750635207087625
Maguen, S., Papa, A., & Litz, B. T. (2008). Coping with the threat of
terrorism: A review. Anxiety, Stress & Coping: An International Journal, 21, 15–35. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10615800701652777
Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., & Altman, D. G., & PRISMA Group.
(2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and metaanalyses: The PRISMA statement. Annals of Internal Medicine, 151,
264 –269, W64. http://dx.doi.org/10.7326/0003-4819-151-4-20090818000135
Moos, R. H., & Holahan, C. J. (2003). Dispositional and contextual
perspectives on coping: Toward an integrative framework. Journal of
Clinical Psychology, 59, 1387–1403. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jclp
.10229
Murray, J. P. (2008). Media violence: The effects are both real and strong.
American Behavioral Scientist, 51, 1212–1230. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1177/0002764207312018
Myers, H. F., Wyatt, G. E., Ullman, J. B., Loeb, T. B., Chin, D., Prause, N.,
. . . Liu, H. (2015). Cumulative burden of lifetime adversities: Trauma
and mental health in low-SES African Americans and Latino/as. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice and Policy, 7, 243–251.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0039077
Nabi, R., & Sullivan, J. L. (2001). Does television viewing relate to
engagement in protective action against crime? A cultivation analysis
from a theory of reasoned action perspective. Communication Research,
28, 802– 825. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/009365001028006004
Nellis, A. M. (2009). Gender differences in fear of terrorism. Journal of
Contemporary Criminal Justice, 25, 322–340. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
1043986209335012
Nellis, A. M., & Savage, J. (2012). Does watching the news affect fear of
terrorism? The importance of media exposure on terrorism fear. Crime
& Delinquency, 58, 748 –768. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0011
128712452961
Neria, Y., & Sullivan, G. M. (2011). Understanding the mental health
effects of indirect exposure to mass trauma through the media. JAMA:
Journal of the American Medical Association, 306, 1374 –1375. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.2011.1358
Nummenmaa, L., Hirvonen, J., Parkkola, R., & Hietanen, J. K. (2008). Is
emotional contagion special? An fMRI study on neural systems for
affective and cognitive empathy. NeuroImage, 43, 571–580. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.014
ⴱ
Ortiz, C. D., Silverman, W. K., Jaccard, J., & La Greca, A. M. (2011).
Children’s state anxiety in reaction to disaster media cues: A preliminary
test of a multivariate model. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research,
Practice, and Policy, 3, 157–164. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0020098
Otto, M. W., Henin, A., Hirshfeld-Becker, D. R., Pollack, M. H., Biederman, J., & Rosenbaum, J. F. (2007). Posttraumatic stress disorder
symptoms following media exposure to tragic events: Impact of 9/11 on
children at risk for anxiety disorders. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21,
888 –902. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.10.008
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
MEDIA EXPOSURE TO LARGE-SCALE VIOLENCE
Pfefferbaum, B. (1997). Posttraumatic stress disorder in children: A review
of the past 10 years. Journal of the American Academy of Child &
Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 1503–1511.
Pfefferbaum, B., Newman, E., Nelson, S. D., Nitiéma, P., Pfefferbaum,
R. L., & Rahman, A. (2014). Disaster media coverage and psychological
outcomes: Descriptive findings in the extant research. Current Psychiatry Reports, 16, 464. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11920-014-0464-x
Pfefferbaum, B., Pfefferbaum, R. L., North, C. S., & Neas, B. R. (2002).
Does television viewing satisfy criteria for exposure in posttraumatic
stress disorder? Psychiatry, 65, 306 –309.
Pfefferbaum, B., Seale, T. W., McDonald, N. B., Brandt, E. N., Jr.,
Rainwater, S. M., Maynard, B. T., . . . Miller, P. D. (2000). Posttraumatic
stress two years after the Oklahoma City bombing in youths geographically distant from the explosion. Psychiatry: Interpersonal and Biological Processes, 63, 358 –370.
Ridout, T. N., Grosse, A. C., & Appleton, A. M. (2008). News media use
and Americans’ perceptions of global threat. British Journal of Political
Science, 38, 575–593. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S000712340800029X
Rogers, R. W. (1983). Cognitive and physiological processes in fear
appeals and attitude change: A revised theory of protection motivation.
In J. T. Cacioppo & R. E. Petty (Eds.), Social psychophysiology: A
sourcebook (pp. 153–176). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Rosenthal, R. (1979). The file drawer problem and tolerance for null
results. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 638 – 641. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
0033-2909.86.3.638
Rosenthal, R. (1991). Meta-analytic procedures for social research. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. http://dx.doi.org/10.4135/9781412984997
Roser, C., & Thompson, M. (1995). Fear appeals and the formation of
active publics. Journal of Communication, 45, 103–122. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.1995.tb00717.x
Rosoff, H., John, R. S., & Prager, F. (2012). Flu, risks, and videotape:
Escalating fear and avoidance. Risk Analysis, 32, 729 –743. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.2012.01769.x
Saylor, C. F., Cowart, B. L., Lipovsky, J. A., Jackson, C., & Finch, A. J.,
Jr. (2003). Media exposure to September 11: Elementary school students’ experiences and posttraumatic symptoms. American Behavioral
Scientist, 46, 1622–1642. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0002764203254619
Schlenger, W. E., Caddell, J. M., Ebert, L., Jordan, B. K., Rourke, K. M.,
Wilson, D., . . . Kulka, R. A. (2002). Psychological reactions to terrorist
attacks: Findings from the National Study of Americans’ Reactions to
September 11. JAMA: Journal of the American Medical Association,
288, 581–588. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.288.5.581
Schuster, M. A., Stein, B. D., Jaycox, L., Collins, R. L., Marshall, G. N.,
Elliott, M. N., . . . Berry, S. H. (2001). A National Survey of Stress
Reactions After the September 11, 2001, Terrorist Attacks. The New
England Journal of Medicine, 345, 1507–1512. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1056/NEJM200111153452024
ⴱ
Shoshani, A., & Slone, M. (2008). Efficacy of clinical interventions for
indirect exposure to terrorism. International Journal of Stress Management, 15, 53–75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1072-5245.15.1.53
Shrira, A., Palgi, Y., Hamama-Raz, Y., Goodwin, R., & Ben-Ezra, M.
(2014). Previous exposure to the World Trade Center terrorist attack and
posttraumatic symptoms among older adults following Hurricane Sandy.
Psychiatry: Interpersonal and Biological Processes, 77, 374 –385.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/psyc.2014.77.4.374
327
Silver, R. C., Holman, E. A., McIntosh, D. N., Poulin, M., & Gil-Rivas, V.
(2002). Nationwide longitudinal study of psychological responses to
September 11. JAMA: Journal of the American Medical Association,
288, 1235–1244. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.288.10.1235
ⴱ
Slone, M. (2000). Responses to media coverage of terrorism. Journal of
Conflict Resolution, 44, 508 –522. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
0022002700044004005
ⴱ
Slone, M., & Shoshani, A. (2006). Evaluation of preparatory measures for
coping with anxiety raised by media coverage of terrorism. Journal of
Counseling Psychology, 53, 535–542. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/00220167.53.4.535
ⴱ
Slone, M., & Shoshani, A. (2008). Indirect victimization from terrorism:
A proposed post-exposure intervention. Journal of Mental Health Counseling, 30, 255–266. http://dx.doi.org/10.17744/mehc.30.3.88r0
563124517027
ⴱ
Slone, M., & Shoshani, A. (2010). Prevention rather than cure? Primary or
secondary intervention for dealing with media exposure to terrorism.
Journal of Counseling & Development, 88, 440 – 448. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1002/j.1556-6678.2010.tb00044.x
Solomon, Z., Gelkopf, M., & Bleich, A. (2005). Is terror gender-blind?
Gender differences in reaction to terror events. Social Psychiatry and
Psychiatric Epidemiology, 40, 947–954. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/
s00127-005-0973-3
Sugimoto, A., Nomura, S., Tsubokura, M., Matsumura, T., Muto, K., Sato,
M., & Gilmour, S. (2013). The relationship between media consumption
and health-related anxieties after the Fukushima Daiichi nuclear disaster.
PLoS ONE, 8(8), e65331. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone
.0065331
Turner, H. A., Finkelhor, D., & Ormrod, R. (2010). Poly-victimization in
a national sample of children and youth. American Journal of Preventive
Medicine, 38, 323–330. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2009.11.012
van Zelst, W., de Beurs, E., & Smit, J. H. (2003). Effects of the September
11th attacks on symptoms of PTSD on community-dwelling older persons in the Netherlands. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry,
18, 190. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/gps.758
Wayment, H. A. (2004). It could have been me: Vicarious victims and
disaster-focused distress. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
30, 515–528. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167203261892
ⴱ
Williams, K., & Khan, S. (2011). College students’ stress reactions and
math ability upon the media’s coverage of terrorism. Journal of the
Mississippi Academy of Sciences, 56, 198. Retrieved from http://go
.galegroup.com/ps/i.do?id⫽GALE%7CA266845307&v⫽2.1&u⫽
dixson&it⫽r&p⫽AONE&sw⫽w&asid⫽c24dc12a2af31fa4576
a285dc4054b14
Wilson, B. J. (2008). Media and children’s aggression, fear, and altruism.
The Future of Children, 18, 87–118. http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/foc.0
.0005
ⴱ
Zeidner, M., Ben-Zur, H., & Reshef-Weil, S. (2011). Vicarious life threat:
An experimental test of conservation of resources (COR) theory. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 641– 645. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1016/j.paid.2010.11.035
Received September 28, 2015
Revision received March 18, 2016
Accepted March 18, 2016 䡲
Sci Eng Ethics (2014) 20:717–733
DOI 10.1007/s11948-013-9502-z
ORIGINAL PAPER
Social Media in Disaster Risk Reduction and Crisis
Management
David E. Alexander
Received: 24 April 2013 / Accepted: 27 November 2013 / Published online: 4 December 2013
Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2013
Abstract This paper reviews the actual and potential use of social media in
emergency, disaster and crisis situations. This is a field that has generated intense
interest. It is characterised by a burgeoning but small and very recent literature. In
the emergencies field, social media (blogs, messaging, sites such as Facebook, wikis
and so on) are used in seven different ways: listening to public debate, monitoring
situations, extending emergency response and management, crowd-sourcing and
collaborative development, creating social cohesion, furthering causes (including
charitable donation) and enhancing research. Appreciation of the positive side of
social media is balanced by their potential for negative developments, such as
disseminating rumours, undermining authority and promoting terrorist acts. This
leads to an examination of the ethics of social media usage in crisis situations.
Despite some clearly identifiable risks, for example regarding the violation of privacy, it appears that public consensus on ethics will tend to override unscrupulous
attempts to subvert the media. Moreover, social media are a robust means of
exposing corruption and malpractice. In synthesis, the widespread adoption and use
of social media by members of the public throughout the world heralds a new age in
which it is imperative that emergency managers adapt their working practices to the
challenge and potential of this development. At the same time, they must heed the
ethical warnings and ensure that social media are not abused or misused when crises
and emergencies occur.
Keywords Social media Disasters Emergency management Ethics
Twitter Facebook
D. E. Alexander (&)
Institute for Risk and Disaster Reduction, University College London, Gower Street,
London WC1E 6BT, UK
e-mail: david.alexander@ucl.ac.uk
123
718
D. E. Alexander
Introduction and Definitions
At 09:02, local time, on 29th May 2012, a damaging earthquake struck EmiliaRomagna and Lombardy regions of northern Italy. This was the second major
seismic event to affect the area in 10 days. It killed 17 people and caused extensive
damage to 40 municipalities. Within 50 minutes a clear and relatively comprehensive picture of the earthquake and some of its most important impacts was available.
It could be consulted via the Internet from almost anywhere in the world. The
information presented was essentially accurate and the speed with which it became
available was largely a result of the use of social media to communicate from the
sites affected to places where data could be collected and presented to the public.
The term ‘social media’ embraces blogs, micro-blogs, social book-marking,
social networking, forums, collaborative creation of documents (via wikis1) and the
sharing of audio, photographic and video files (Balana 2012). It is characterised by
interactive communication, in which message content is exchanged between
individuals, audiences, organisations and sectors of the general public.
Social media usage is, to some extent, negatively correlated with age and
positively with educational attainment. For example, people over the age of 55 tend
to prefer conventional sources of news. The degree of adoption of social media
varies from country to country but is generally dynamic in most environments and
hence any summary statistics are liable to become outdated rapidly. Attempts to
relate social media to personality factors have suggested that they are most
attractive to people, of both sexes, who are relatively extrovert (Correa et al. 2010),
but there is no indication of the extent to which any effort to develop profiles of
users might be culturally conditioned. Information on gender differentiation is, at
best, fragmentary (Armstrong and McAdams 2009).
In the United States, the Internet is the most important source of information
for people under the age of 30. For other Americans, it is second only to
television (Krimsky 2007). Elsewhere, the use of ‘smart’ phones and social media
resources is increasing so rapidly that they are now a force to be reckoned with
throughout the world. Social media dispense with ‘‘information gatekeepers’’,
which include doctors giving on-line medical advice and journalists relating a
news story. These figures are replaced by apomediaries, in which network filtering
or group moderation are the only processes by which the spontaneous feed of
information is regulated—a matter of apomediation or disintermediation (Eysenbach 2008).
This paper offers a review of the use of social media in disasters and major
incidents. I consider both how citizens, emergency managers and first responders
make use of social media in crisis and how researchers perceive and characterise the
phenomenon. I examine seven ways in which social media are put to use for disaster
response, recovery and risk reduction. As social media have both beneficial and
potentially malign connotations, their advantages and drawbacks are discussed.
Next, I consider the ethical implications of social media in disaster, including the
1
The term ‘wiki’ is defined by the OED as ‘‘A type of web page designed so that its content can be edited
by anyone who accesses it, using a simplified markup language.’’ It was apparently first used in 1995.
123
Social Media in Disaster Risk Reduction
719
risks and dilemmas of unregulated communication and the degree of inclusiveness
of new media. In order to end on a positive note, examples of successes with social
media in disaster are briefly discussed. Finally, some conclusions are drawn, but
these must necessarily be provisional, as the field is in the early stages of rapid
evolution in relation to both technological development and social acceptance.
The Research Literature
The research literature on social networking and social media in disasters and crises
is still quite limited. Moreover, it focuses on the short-term aspects of emergency
response and rapid recovery. It is understandable that there are as yet no studies of
the longer term, both because social media are a relatively new phenomenon and
because the research is also new. Although ‘new media’, such as the Internet, have
received attention from academics for a decade or more, very little of the research
on social networking predates 2007. However, there is a trend towards a rapid
increase in the number of papers that have been published. In this context, the
literature on ‘social media’ needs to be differentiated from that on the social aspects
of mass media, which is a much wider field that embraces more conventional and
long-standing forms of dissemination of information, such as radio and television
(Quarantelli 1989).
Studies of social media in disasters have been conducted as part of a general
tendency to examine the functioning of social interaction by means of the Internet
and mobile devices (Krimsky 2007). Both sets of literature concentrate mainly on
specific themes, which are:
•
•
•
•
how social networks function and how they are used
how to build and utilise algorithms either to enhance social networking or to
monitor it
the extent to which people use social networks, how they perceive them and
what their communication preferences are
the penetration of devices such as ‘smart’ mobile telephones and the extent to
which these provide people with access to social media.
In addition, students of risk, crisis and disaster have studied:
•
•
•
how social media are used in crises
the views and opinions of emergency managers and journalists regarding social
media and the extent to which the new media are integrated with more
traditional means of communication
how social media interact with the traditional sources of information.
There is a broad distinction between studies of the technical and social aspects of
new media. The creation of new platforms and algorithms characterises the former
(Cheong and Lee 2010; White and Plotnik 2010), while studies of the kinds of usage
and messages sent relate to the latter (Hughes and Palen 2009; Lindsay 2011). The
technical side includes by studies of the rate and modality of diffusion of messages
(Song and Yan 2012).
123
720
D. E. Alexander
While researchers work to develop software for the efficient dissemination of
messages via social networks during crisis situations (e.g. Plotnick et al. 2009),
Reuter et al. (2012) advocated a more systematic approach to the use of social
networking software in crisis situations, starting with classification of uses and
potentials.
Researchers are equivocal about the balance between the advantages and
drawbacks of social media (see below), but they are united in identifying the uses to
which the media can be put. Social media promote cross-platform accessibility and a
constant flow of information. Situational updates can be complemented by
geographical and locational data (Vieweg et al. 2010). Just-in-time information
can be provided on how to cope with developing situations. Moreover, social media
provide a framework for the work of journalists and for public discussion and debate.
Social Media in Disaster and Crisis
The following are some of the ways in which social media can be used in disaster
risk reduction and crisis response.
1.
A listening function. Social media are able to give a voice to people who do not
normally have one. They also enable a remarkably democratic form of
participation in public debate and facilitate the exchange of information and
points of view. During an emergency, through their tendency to coalesce
opinions (or stimulate monetary donations), social media are capable of
revealing some aspects of the mental and emotional state of a nation. This may
seem a rather exaggerated claim, but it should be noted that Quarantelli (1997)
argued that the advent of modern information and communications technology
involves changes that are as...
Purchase answer to see full
attachment