Name:
Reference (APA Format)
Theoretical Basis
Hypothesis
Independent Variables
Dependent Variables
Control Groups
Data Analysis & Statistical Methods
Discussion Summary
Recommendations for Future Research
Article 1
Date:
Name:
Article 1
Date:
I am relating the article to Chapter ____ and ___________________ disorder.
Relating the Article (13 points)
On this page please relate the findings of the article you reviewed to the text and classroom
discussions on the appropriate chapter (5, 6, or a combination). This part of the write-up
should be Double Spaced, Times New Roman, 12pt font, two to three pages (2-3), APA
writing style. Be sure to also place your name in the header and indicate the chapter and
disorder you are writing about. Remember to Bold terms from the textbook the first time they
are used and denote the relevant Chapter (Example Prognosis-Chapter 4).
Class Date
Week 1
Jan 13
Due
Jan 22
Intro to Normal and Abnormal Behavior in
Children and Adolescents
Ch. 1 cont.
Ch 2 Quiz (Opens Wed 4:30 – Closes Mon 2:30)
Theories and Causes
Research
Assessment, Diagnosis and Treatment
Feb 12
Feb 19
watch online video and complete Virtual Classroom – Discussion Board
Test 1 (Ch. 1-4)
Ch 5 Quiz (Opens Wed 4:30 – Closes Mon 2:30)
Intellectual Disability (Intellectual Developmental Disorder) Chapter 5
Review Test 1
Ch. 5 cont.
Ch 6 Quiz (Opens Mon 4:30 – Closes Wed 2:30)
Week 8
Mar 2
Mar 4
Week 9
Mar 8-15
Week 10
Mar 16
Mar 18
Chapter 4
Ch. 4 cont.
Review for Test 1
Week 7
Feb 24
Feb 26
Chapter 3
Ch. 3 cont.
Ch 4 Quiz (Opens Wed 4:30 – Closes Mon 2:30)
Week 5
Feb 10
Week 6
Feb 17
Chapter 2
Ch. 2 cont.
Ch 3 Quiz (Opens Wed 4:30 – Closes Mon 2:30)
Week 4
Feb 3
Feb 5
Chapter 1
MLK – No classes
Week 3
Jan 27
Jan 29
Reading Assignments
Classes begin
Introductions, Syllabus
Ch 1 Quiz (Opens Mon 4:30 – Closes Wed 2:30)
Jan 15
Week 2
Jan 20
PGS 443 Abnormal Child Psychology
M/W 3:05-4:20
Spring 2020
Topic
Autism Spectrum and Childhood Schizophrenia
Chapter 6
Article Review 1 (from Ch. 5 or 6)
Ch. 6 cont.
Ch 7 Quiz (Opens Wed 4:30 – Closes Mon 2:30)
Spring Break
No classes
Communication and Learning Disorders
Ch. 7 cont.
Ch 8 Quiz (Opens Wed 4:30 – Closes Mon 2:30)
Chapter 7
Week 11
Mar 23
Mar 25
Week 12
Mar 30
Attention-Deficit/ Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)
Interview 1
(from Ch. 5, 6 or 7)
Conduct Problems
Review Test 1
Depressive and Bipolar Disorders
Anxiety and Obsessive-Compulsive Disorders
April 29
Chapter 11
Article Review 2
Ch. 11 cont.
(from Ch. 7-11)
Ch 14 Quiz (Opens Mon 4:30 – Closes Wed 2:30)
April 22
Week 16
April 27
Chapter 10
Ch. 10 cont.
Ch 11 Quiz (Opens Mon 4:30 – Closes Wed 2:30)
April 15
Week 15
April 20
Chapter 9
Ch. 9 cont.
Ch 10 Quiz (Opens Mon 4:30 – Closes Wed 2:30)
April 8
Week 14
April 13
Ch. 8 cont.
review for Test 2
Test 2 (Ch. 5-8)
Ch 9 Quiz (Opens Mon 4:30 – Closes Wed 2:30)
April 1
Week 13
April 6
Chapter 8
Eating Disorders and Related conditions
Interview 2
Ch. 14 cont.
(from Ch. 8-11 or 14)
review for exam 3
Complete Course Evaluation (bring computer, tablet or phone)
Test 3 (Ch. 9-11 & 14)
Last class
May 1
May 2 - 3
All classes end
Study Days
Week 17
May 4 - 9
May 4
Finals Week
FINAL Online (comprehensive)
Monday, May 4th
Chapter 14
5
Intellectual Disability (Intellectual
Developmental Disorder)
Everybody is a genius. But if you judge a fsh by its ability to climb a tree, it
will live its whole life believing that it is stupid.
—Albert Einstein
CHAPTER PREVIEW
IntellIgence and
Intellectual dIsabIlIty
Genetic and Constitutional
Factors
The Eugenics Scare
Motivation
Neurobiological Infuences
Defning and Measuring
Children’s Intelligence and
Adaptive Behavior
Changes in Abilities
Social and Psychological
Dimensions
The Controversial IQ
Features oF Intellectual
dIsabIlItIes
Clinical Description
Severity Levels
Prevalence
124
developmental course and
adult outcomes
Language and Social Behavior
Emotional and Behavioral
Problems
Other Physical and Health
Disabilities
causes
Inheritance and the Role of the
Environment
preventIon, educatIon, and
treatment
Prenatal Education and
Screening
Psychosocial Treatments
dren and adults who today would be diagnosed as
having an intellectual disability were labeled “idiots”
and often were lumped together with persons suffering from mental disorders or medical conditions. They
typically were ignored or feared, even by the medical
profession, because their differences in appearance and
ability were so little understood.
Although age-old fears, resentment, and scorn continue to overshadow many important discoveries about
subnormal intelligence, the feld of intellectual disability has experienced monumental gains over the past
century in determining causes and providing services.
Advances in understanding the development of children with intellectual disability, along with research in
genetics, psychopathology, and other areas, have dramatically changed the face of this feld.
The term intellectual disability has replaced the previous term mental retardation in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th edition (DSM-5),
based on consensus among professionals, advocacy
groups, and the lay public. Intellectual disability is a neurodevelopmental disorder, a group of conditions with onset in the developmental period that produce impairments
of social, personal, academic or occupational functioning
(APA, 2013). Intellectual disability (ID) is characterized
by signifcant limitations in mental abilities (such as reasoning, planning, and judgment) that result in impairments in adaptive functioning, such as conceptual, social,
and practical skills needed to fulfll aspects of everyday
life (Tassé et al., 2012). Although the name has changed,
the three essential elements defning this condition have
remained the same for the past 50 years: intellectual limitations, defcits in adaptive skills, and early onset.
Importantly, modern defnitions of intellectual disability take into account a person’s intellectual functioning
within the context of what is typical for that individual’s peers and culture. Cultural and linguistic diversity,
as well as differences in communication, sensory,
motor, and behavioral factors, must be carefully considered in determining a defcit or disability (American
Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities [AAIDD], 2010). Understanding improvements in
knowledge about and treatment of intellectual disability, as well as understanding the prejudice and ignorance
that had to be overcome, can be gained by looking at
how the disorder has been viewed over the years.
INTELLIGENCE AND INTELLECTUAL
DISABILITY
Throughout recorded history, persons with intellectual
and other disabilities have suffered scorn and rejection
from others, largely because of fear and ignorance.
The prevailing misunderstanding and mistreatment
of children with intellectual disabilities changed very
little until the end of the eighteenth century, fueled by
the discovery of feral children such as Victor, the “wild
boy of Aveyron” (discussed in Chapter 1), and by the
expansion of humanitarian efforts to assist other oppressed or neglected groups, such as slaves, prisoners,
the mentally ill, and persons with physical disabilities.
By the mid-nineteenth century, the concept of mental retardation had spread from France and Switzerland to much of Europe and North America. During
the same period, Dr. Samuel G. Howe convinced his
contemporaries that training and educating the “feebleminded” was a public responsibility, and he opened the
frst humanitarian institution in North America for
persons with intellectual disability—the Massachusetts
School for Idiotic and Feeble-Minded Youth.
Parents of children with ID can be credited with
advancing a perspective and response to the need for
long-term care that was completely different from
the prevailing public and professional opinions. By the
1940s, parents began to meet in groups and create local
diagnostic and guidance centers to increase the availability of humane care. These groups organized in 1950
to form the National Association for Retarded Children
(now known as The Arc), which quickly established a
scientifc board made up of representatives from every
specialty possible to study, prevent, and care for persons with intellectual disability (Kanner, 1964). These
efforts gained momentum when President John F.
Kennedy, who had a sister with intellectual disability,
formed the President’s Panel on Mental Retardation
in 1962 and called for a national program to combat
intellectual disability. Soon thereafter, exposés in the
media of the use of solitary confnement and restraints
©iStockphoto.com/Moodboard_Images
U
NTIL THE MID-NINETEENTH CENTURY, chil-
Ignorance resulted in the segregation of children with
intellectual disabilities, who were subjected to inhumane
treatment. Today, most children with ID are integrated into
regular classrooms.
C h a p t e r 5 Intellectual Disability (Intellectual Developmental Disorder)
125
for persons with ID increased public awareness of and
outrage at the treatment of these individuals.
the eugenics scare
Three generations of imbeciles are enough.
—Justice Oliver Wendell Holmes Jr., commenting on the Supreme
Court’s 1927 decision to uphold eugenics sterilization laws
Evolutionary degeneracy theory, a pervasive nineteenthcentury phenomenon, attributed the intellectual and
social problems of children with intellectual disability
to regression to an earlier period in human evolution
(Bowler, 1989). In fact, mental defciency experts in the
nineteenth century believed they had found the “missing link” between humans and lower species (Gelb,
1995). J. Langdon H. Down, best known for the clinical
description of the genetic syndrome that bears his
name, interpreted the “strange anomalies” among his
medical sample of persons with intellectual disability as
an evolutionary throwback to the Mongol race (Down,
1866). Down believed that parents in one racial group
might give birth to a child with intellectual disability
who was a “retrogression” to another group. While
grounded in speculation and misinformed conclusions,
evolutionary degeneracy theory and its notion of inferiority received growing support by the late-nineteenth
century as an explanation for insanity, mental defciency,
and social deviance. A Closer Look 5.1 depicts how
this popular theory was conveniently used to explain
undesirable human characteristics.
By 1910, the eugenics movement was gaining
momentum. Eugenics was frst defned by Sir Francis
Galton (Charles Darwin’s cousin) in 1883 as “the science
which deals with all infuences that improve the inborn
qualities of a race” (cited in Kanner, 1964, p. 128). Public
and professional emphasis shifted away from the needs
of persons with intellectual disability toward a consideration of the needs of society; society was to be protected
from the presumable harm done by the presence of these
persons in the community.
Consequently, persons with intellectual disability
often were blamed for the social ills of the time, which
is a powerful example of how labeling a problem can
quickly become an explanation for it.A Closer Look 5.2,
in fact, shows a 1912 New York Times article refecting this public sentiment. The appearance, ability, and
behavior of persons with intellectual disability were
considered evidence of their lack of moral fber, a
belief that led to the diagnostic term moral imbecile, or
moron, used to describe and explain their differences.
This concept became a straightforward explanation for
acts of deviance and justifed wide-ranging attempts to
identify and control such individuals. Morons, considered the least intellectually impaired (roughly comparable to mild intellectual disability today) were seen
as a threat to society because, unlike the insane, they
could easily pass for normal (Gelb, 1995). The intellectually impaired and other “undesirables” were seen as
a public menace, to be feared and ostracized.
defning and measuring children’s
Intelligence and adaptive behavior
Around 1900, the pioneering work of two French educators, Alfred Binet and Theophile Simon, led to some
of the frst major advancements in the feld of children’s
intellectual functioning. Binet and Simon were asked to
develop a way to identify schoolchildren who might need
special help in school. They approached this monumental
task by developing the frst intelligence tests to measure
judgment and reasoning, which they believed were basic
A CLOSER LOOK 5.1
the Infamous Kallikaks
Psychologist Henry Goddard, who began one of the largest
training schools for intellectual disability in the early twentieth century, was also a proponent of the popular degeneracy
theory and eugenics movement. In his book The Kallikak Family:
A Study in the Heredity of Feeble-Mindedness (1912), Goddard
traced two lines of descendants from a Revolutionary War soldier, Martin Kallikak, who fathered a child by a “feebleminded
barmaid” during the war, which began the first line, and then
fathered other children by a “respectable girl” he married after
the war. The name “Kallikak” was invented by Goddard from a
combination of two Greek words: kalos, meaning “attractive,
pleasing,” and kakos, meaning “bad, evil.”
126
P a r t 2 Neurodevelopmental Disorders
Goddard reported that many descendants of the first union
were feebleminded, delinquent, poor, and alcoholic, whereas
those of the second union were of good reputation. He claimed
this outcome was evidence for the inheritance of intelligence,
although he overlooked the two families’ obvious environmental differences (Achenbach, 1982). A closer look at the disclaimer from the preface to the book is telling: “It is true that we
have made rather dogmatic statements and have drawn conclusions that do not seem scientifically warranted by the data.”
(Based on authors’ case material.)
A CLOSER LOOK 5.2
early-twentieth-century perspectives on
mental retardation
WEAK-MINDED FILL RANKS OF CRIMINALS
DR. HENRY GODDARD SAYS SOCIAL PROBLEMS
CAN BE SOLVED BY SEGREGATING THEM
WOULD NOT LET THEM MARRY
THIS POLICY WOULD IN TIME LARGELY
REDUCE CRIMES, DISEASE, AND DRUNKENNESS,
HE BELIEVES
From the army of 300,000 feeble-minded persons in the
United States come the recruits that swell the ranks of the
drunkards, criminals, paupers, and other social outcasts.
Twenty-five per cent of the girls and boys in our reformatories are lacking in mental fibre and are unable to discern
the difference between right and wrong or are too weak in
character to do right whenever there is any inducement to
do wrong. Sixty-five per cent of the children have a mother or
a father, or both, who are feeble-minded. This country has so
far taken no steps to segregate these irresponsible persons,
so the number of them is constantly increasing….
This army furnishes the ranks of the criminals, paupers,
drunkards, the ne’er-do-wells, and others who are social misfits. Their incapacity would be a priori cause of believing that
they eventually will become public charges in one form or
another, and investigation, in fact, proves that the groups of
criminals, paupers, etc., actually do contain large percentages
of people mentally irresponsible.
Source: The New York Times, March 10, 1912.
processes of higher thought. These early test questions
asked children to manipulate unfamiliar objects such as
blocks or fgures and to solve puzzles and match familiar
parts of objects. The test later became the Stanford–Binet
scale, which remains one of the most widely used intelligence tests.
From these beginnings in intellectual testing,
general intellectual functioning is now defned by an
intelligence quotient (IQ or equivalent) that is based
on assessment with one or more of the standardized,
individually administered intelligence tests, such as the
Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children, 4th edition
(WISC-IV), the Stanford–Binet, 5th edition (SB5), and
the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children, 2nd
edition (KABC-II). These tests assess various verbal and
visual–spatial skills (such as knowledge of the world,
reasoning, and similarities and differences) and mathematical concepts, which together are presumed to constitute the general construct known as “intelligence.”
By convention, IQ scores (with a mean of 100 and
a standard deviation of 15) are derived from a standardized table based on a person’s age and test score.
Because intelligence is defned along a normal distribution, approximately 95% of the population has
scores within 2 standard deviations of the mean (i.e.,
between 70 and 130). Subaverage intellectual functioning is defned, accordingly, as an IQ of about 70
or below (approximately 2 standard deviations below
the mean).
As we will discuss later in this chapter, the defnition of intellectual disability includes not only subaverage intellectual functioning, but also a subaverage
level of adaptive functioning. Adaptive functioning
refers to how effectively individuals cope with ordinary life demands, and how capable they are of living
independently and abiding by community standards
(Hodapp et al., 2011). Note that some children and
adolescents may learn to adapt quite well to their
environment despite their lower intelligence as measured by an IQ test; therefore, they would not be
considered to have an intellectual disability. Table 5.1
gives examples of the three major categories of
adaptive behavior (conceptual, social, and practical
adaptive skills). Similar to IQ, these skills are typically
TABLE 5.1
|
Specific Examples of Adaptive
Behavior Skills
Conceptual Skills
Receptive and expressive language
Reading and writing
Money concepts
Self-directions
Social Skills
Interpersonal
Responsibility
Self-esteem
Gullibility (likelihood of being tricked or manipulated)
Naiveté
Follows rules
Obeys laws
Avoids victimization
Practical Skills
Personal activities of daily living such as eating, dressing,
mobility, and toileting
Instrumental activities of daily living such as preparing meals,
taking medication, using the telephone, managing money,
using transportation and doing housekeeping activities
Occupational Skills
Maintaining a safe environment
Source: From the American Association on Intellectual and Developmental
Disabilities(AAIDD).
C h a p t e r 5 Intellectual Disability (Intellectual Developmental Disorder)
127
assessed using standardized instruments such as the
Adaptive Behavior Assessment System—Second
Edition (ABAS-II; Harrison & Oakland, 2003) or the
Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales—Second Edition
(VABS-II; Sparrow, Cicchetti, & Balla, 2005).
the controversial IQ
If a person’s intelligence is relatively stable over time, it
would be tempting to conclude that it is largely innate
and fxed. On the other hand, if intellectual and cognitive development is signifcantly shaped by environment,
perhaps cognitive growth can be stimulated at an early
age and the level of intellectual disability decreased.
Because intelligence is measured in relation to agemates, IQ generally is stable from childhood through
adulthood (Carr, 2005; Whitaker, 2008). One exception to this general rule is IQ that is measured during
early infancy, when considerable fuctuation can still
occur. For typically developing children, IQ measured
prior to the frst birthday has virtually no correlation
with the IQ score achieved at age 12; however, by the
time children are 4 years old, the correlation with IQ
12 years later is high (r 5 0.77) (Neisser et al., 1996).
The picture is dramatically different, however,
for infants and children with developmental delays
or intellectual disability. At the lower IQ levels (say,
below 50), even the youngest infants show IQ stability over time, with correlations between infant
and childhood test scores ranging from 0.50 to 0.97
(Sattler, 2006). Researchers have discovered a similar pattern of IQ stability from middle childhood
to young adulthood among children with mild to
moderate intellectual delays (Mortensen et al., 2003;
Toth & King, 2010).
Even though the IQ of cognitively delayed infants
and young children is unlikely to change, proper environmental circumstances will help children reach their
fullest potential. Since the early 1960s, researchers in
child development and intellectual disability have been
investigating ways to provide early stimulation programs that will help children with developmental delays
to build on their existing strengths. Despite its strong
genetic component, mental ability is always modifed by experience. Not surprisingly, infancy through
early childhood offer the most signifcant opportunity for infuencing intellectual ability because of the
young child’s rapid brain development and response to
environmental stimulation (Campbell & Ramey, 2010;
Pungello et al., 2010).
The importance of genetic makeup not with standing, for some individuals IQ can and does change by
10 to 20 points between childhood and adolescence
128
P a r t 2 Neurodevelopmental Disorders
(Simonoff, Bolton, & Rutter, 1996). Differences in
outcome vary widely in relation to opportunities for
each child to learn and develop. Children who live in
healthy environments, where caregivers provide appropriate levels of stimulation and help them manage
ambient levels of stress, are most likely to reach their
full potential. Moreover, tests can sample only a limited spectrum of intellectual ability, and are incapable
of accounting for each individual’s unique learning
history (Sattler, 2006).
Are We Really Getting Smarter?
Scores have risen sharply since the beginning of IQ
testing, ranging from a 5- to a 25-point increase in
a single generation (Kanaya, Scullin, & Ceci, 2003).
When James Flynn brought this phenomenon of rising IQ scores to the attention of scientists in 1987,
it became known as the “Flynn effect.” The gain has
averaged about 3 IQ points per decade, adding up to
more than a full standard deviation since the 1940s.
Once a test is re-normed (about every 15 to 20 years)
the mean is reset to 100, resulting in a brief reversal
of this gain in IQ scores.
In attempting to explain the Flynn effect, scientists have considered the rising standards of living,
better schooling, better nutrition, medical advances,
more stimulating environments, even the infuence
of computer games and complex toys (Flynn, 2007;
2012). IQ tests themselves have once again come
under scrutiny, as have children’s exposure to problems similar to those on the tests—like the mazes and
puzzles they see on their cereal boxes and fast-food
bags. Yet the consistent IQ gains are too large to be
the result simply of increased familiarity with testing
methods.
Although the exact cause of the effect remains
unknown, experts on children’s intelligence suspect
that the gains refect a meaningful aspect of intellectual growth and development. A relatively permissive and child-focused parenting style has emerged
during recent decades, which may have given children greater facility with language and stronger
overall cognitive capacity. Moreover, there are unprecedented cultural differences between successive
generations—daily life and occupational experiences
are far more complex today than in the past. There
is a possible downside to the Flynn effect, however.
Test scores drop an average of 5.6 points among persons with borderline and mild intellectual disability
after a test is re-normed, which can have a signifcant impact on a child’s eligibility for proper educational placement and other related services (Kanaya
et al., 2003).
Are IQ Tests Biased or Unfair?
Many express concern over the relatively lower mean
of the distribution of intelligence test scores of African
Americans, typically about 1 standard deviation (about
15 IQ points) below that of whites (Jencks & Phillips,
1998). Although recent estimates suggest that African
Americans have gained 4 to 7 IQ points on nonHispanic whites since 1972 (Dickens & Flynn, 2006),
considerable concern remains as to why differences in
test results exist at all.
The controversy in IQ score differences is fueled
by researchers who argue that IQ is 80% heritable
and therefore largely genetically determined (Rushton
& Jensen, 2006). Other researchers argue that economic and social inequality—not test bias or racial
differences—are the simplest explanations for existing
group differences in test performance between African
Americans and whites (Brooks-Gunn, Klebanov et al.,
2003; Nisbett et al., 2012). For example, when personal
and family background characteristics are statistically
controlled for, African American and white children
achieve similar test scores. However, once these students enter school, the gap between white and African
American children grows, which researchers believe
may be due to lower-quality schools in the lowerincome neighborhoods where some African-Americans
live (Levitt & Fryer, 2004; Nisbett, 2009). A much
higher proportion of African Americans and children
from other ethnic minority groups in the United States
as well as in other countries are poor and have fewer
opportunities for advancement as compared with their
white counterparts. Poverty and inequality are linked
to poor nutrition, inadequate prenatal care, fewer intellectual resources, and similar realities that can have
negative effects on children’s developing intelligence
(Christian et al., 2013; Sternberg, 2010).
Section Summary
Intelligence and Intellectual Disability
●
●
●
●
The early history of intellectual disability was plagued by
ignorance and blame.
By the mid-twentieth century, progress toward understanding intellectual disability moved more rapidly, as
parents, researchers, politicians, and the public sought
better answers regarding its causes and better ways to
assist both children and adults with ID.
Intellectual disability refers to limitations in both intelligence
and adaptive behavior. However, many persons with this disorder are capable of learning and of leading fulflling lives.
Despite its drawbacks, the IQ has become a principal standard for diagnosing intellectual disability, combined with
other skills and abilities of the child.
FEATURES OF INTELLECTUAL
DISABILITIES
M AT T H E W
Gaining at His Own Pace
Matthew was almost 6 years old when he was referred
for a psychological assessment. His brief school record
described him as “developmentally delayed,” and the
school was concerned that his speech and social skills
were very limited. He also had temper tantrums at
home, and his new frst-grade teacher had expressed
concerns about his aggressive behavior toward other
children in his class.
I frst met with Matt in his home. “Show me some
of your favorite toys or games,” I suggested, unsure of
how comfortable he was with a stranger at his home. He
was a thin boy, with curly hair and a cautious, reserved
expression. He looked me over for what must have been
several minutes while I spoke with his mother and father.
Although he said “OK,” I wasn’t sure he meant it—he
stayed put and seemed uninterested in my request. Matt
had turned 6 a few months ago, but I noticed that his
clothes, games, and vocabulary were closer to those of my
3-year-old daughter. “I don’t want to talk about school
stuff!” he exclaimed, quite loudly, when I asked about his
favorite subjects. “I only like recess and lunchtime—the
stuff they won’t let you do till the bell rings!” There was
a certain degree of truth, and humor, to his statement,
although I don’t think he intended it as such.
Matt became a bit more interested when I brought out
some testing materials. He completed with ease a puzzle
designed for toddlers and was able to make the sounds
of animals in the puzzle. But his emotional expression
remained subdued, with little spontaneous laughter or
joy. He seemed watchful and cautious. “Tell me about
this story,” I said to Matt, holding up a card showing
some animals arguing over a ball. “What do you think
is going on in this picture, and what are the characters,
like the elephant and the zebra, thinking and feeling?”
Matt started right in: “He’s mad ’cuz the zebra grabbed
the ball and ran away with it into the woods. That’s all
I see.” My attempts to elicit more detail were met with
only an inquisitive look.
After a few minutes of this, we took a break and
brought out his toys. “Do you like Harry Potter?” he
asked. We found some common ground among the characters in the book, and under these “ideal” conditions,
Matt’s communication became more at ease and spontaneous. He expressed a wide range of emotion throughout
the interview, and settled in to his own comfortable level
of relating. Gradually, his language production increased
as we continued with the more relaxed play sessions.
(continues)
C h a p t e r 5 Intellectual Disability (Intellectual Developmental Disorder)
129
(continued)
In private, Matt’s mother told me about his behavior
problems around other children, such as hitting, biting,
throwing objects, and demanding attention. I saw a brief
episode of it myself when his 3-year-old sister came into
the room: “Get out! This man is here to play with me!”
Overall, Matt behaved like a much younger child—for
example, by shouting or pushing when he couldn’t get
his way immediately. When we met for the second time,
in my offce, Matt’s WISC-IV full-scale IQ was assessed
at 64, and his adaptive abilities score was 68, based on
his mother’s report on the Vineland Adaptive Behavior
Scales. Despite his mild intellectual disability, however,
Matt has been gaining over the past year in school, and
he is showing a healthy gain in his developmental milestones as well. (Based on authors’ case material.)
You judge a person by how they look or how they talk or
what the tests show, but you can never really tell what is
inside the person.
—Ed, 27 years old, who was labeled mentally retarded and placed
in a state institution at age 15 (Bogdan & Taylor, 1982)
Intellectual disabilities encompass perhaps the widest
variation in cognitive and behavioral abilities of any
childhood disorder. Some of these children function
quite well in school and the community, whereas others with signifcant physical and cognitive impairments
require daily supervision and assistance. The situations
of Matthew, age 6, and Vanessa, age 8, illustrate some
of the unique challenges children with intellectual disability face every day.
Matthew was diagnosed as having mild intellectual
disability. Although his speech and language development were delayed, he was developing effective verbal
skills and was capable of attending a regular classroom. Establishing friendships with children at school
was sometimes problematic because he was often slow
at understanding the rules of games and was teased by
some children because of his slowness.
Vanessa was diagnosed as having moderate intellectual disability. She could feed and dress herself with
minimum assistance, and she communicated in short
sentences, although her speech was not always discernible to people outside her family. Vanessa required daily
assistance to complete her routines, but she too was
able to attend a local school during part of the day. As
these cases show, the special needs of both children were
sometimes overshadowed by economic and educational
limitations, which required creativity and coordinated
assistance on the part of parents, teachers, and other
professionals.
130
P a r t 2 Neurodevelopmental Disorders
VA N E S S A
Gaining at Home
Vanessa is an 8-year-old girl with moderate intellectual
disability (IQ 5 52) and limited communication skills.
She was diagnosed prior to her fourth birthday, after
medical and psychological examinations were undertaken to determine why she was not making many
speech sounds or learning basic self-help skills. Vanessa’s
mother told us about how her daughter’s special needs
were poorly met while she was a resident in a special
school for children with intellectual disabilities, and how
this led to her parents’ decision to raise Vanessa at home
with the help of their community:
When our family moved here we were told that
we would receive $75 per month to care for her at
home, or she could live at the Children’s Training
Center. Vanessa had been diagnosed with moderate
intellectual disability prior to her fourth birthday, and
we knew that we could not care for her daily needs at
home with the limited assistance being offered. So we
made the diffcult decision to place her at the training
center. But, even though Vanessa came home every
weekend, we felt there was something missing from
her life; something beyond staff care and attention
was needed to foster her growth.
About 2 years later, things changed dramatically.
Vanessa was injured by another resident, and we decided
that she should return home once and for all. We made
every effort to fnd services she needed for her training
and education in our own community. She now attends
an integrated classroom at the same school as her older
brother, and her teachers have noticed strong gains in
her behavior and language. She participates in recreational programs, and has become an accomplished
swimmer and basketball player. (Based on authors’ case
material.)
clinical description
When the psychiatrist interviewed me he had my records in
front of him—so he already knew I was mentally retarded.
It’s the same with everyone. If you are considered mentally
retarded there is no way you can win. There is no way they
give you a favorable report.
—Ed, describing part of his intake interview at the state institution
(Bogdan & Taylor, 1982)
Children with intellectual disability show a considerable range of abilities and interpersonal qualities. With
proper assistance, children with mild intellectual impairments, such as Matthew, can carry out their daily
routines much like other children. They can attend a
regular classroom, adjust to the demands of physical
and intellectual challenges, and develop meaningful
and lasting relationships with peers and adults. Many
show normal physical development and can learn the
physical coordination required to ride a bike. Others,
like Vanessa, who have more severe impairments, will
require greater daily supervision and care throughout
their childhood and sometimes into early adulthood,
at which time they may have developed the necessary
skills to live more independently.
Both Matthew and Vanessa, however, experience
limitations that involve most areas of daily living. Their
most obvious diffculties are learning to communicate
effectively, due to their limited speech and language
skills. Although Matthew eventually learned effective
verbal communication, for several years Vanessa had
to rely on sign language and nonverbal expressions or
gestures to express her needs. Both children had problems developing friendships with other children because of their limited ability to comprehend what other
children were expressing, especially during games and
social activities that require stamina and formal rules.
Many cognitive abilities, such as language and problem solving, are affected; therefore, most children with
ID have diffculty with some aspect of learning. The
degree of diffculty depends on the extent of cognitive
impairment, which is the primary reason current defnitions of intellectual disability emphasize this aspect.
Table 5.2 summarizes the DSM-5 diagnostic criteria
for intellectual disability (also known as intellectual
developmental disorder). These criteria consist of three
core features that describe this disorder in both children
and adults. First, such individuals must have defcits in
the intellectual functions of reasoning, problem-solving,
planning, abstract thinking, judgment, academic learning,
and learning from experience. Such defcits are typically
determined by clinical assessment and intelligence testing.
But note that the DSM-5 does not provide specifc IQ
cutoff limits for meeting diagnostic criteria, as was the
case in previous editions.
The second criterion for diagnosing intellectual
disability requires defcits in adaptive functioning,
which result in failure to meet developmental and
sociocultural standards for personal independence
and social responsibility. In effect, a subnormal IQ
score is not suffcient to warrant a diagnosis of intellectual disability. A person also must show signifcant
limitations in adaptive behavior, such as communication, self-care, social/interpersonal skills, or functional
academic or work skills (Lancioni et al., 2009). This
aspect of the defnition is important because it specifcally excludes persons who may function well in their
own surroundings, yet for various reasons they may
not perform well on standard IQ tests. Importantly,
TABLE 5.2
|
Diagnostic Criteria for Intellectual
Disability (Intellectual Developmental
Disorder)
Intellectual disability (intellectual developDSM-5
mental disorder) is a disorder with onset
during the developmental period that includes both
intellectual and adaptive functioning deficits in
conceptual, social, and practical domains. The following
three criteria must be met:
(A) Deficits in intellectual functions, such as reasoning,
problem-solving, planning, abstract thinking, judgment,
academic learning and learning from experience, confirmed by both clinical assessment and individualized,
standardized intelligence testing.
(B) Deficits in adaptive functioning that result in failure to
meet developmental and sociocultural standards for
personal independence and social responsibility. Without
ongoing support, the adaptive deficits limit functioning
in one or more activities of daily life, such as communication, social participation, and independent living, across
multiple environments, such as home, school, work, and
community.
(C) Onset of intellectual and adaptive deficits during the
developmental period.
Note: The diagnostic term intellectual disability is the equivalent term for the ICD-11
diagnosis of intellectual developmental disorders.
Specify current severity (see Table 5.3): Mild, Moderate,
Severe, or Profound
Source: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition, American
Psychiatric Association.
whether a child or adolescent exhibits these various
adaptive skills is related not only to ability, but also
to experience and opportunity. Using public transportation, walking to a neighborhood store, and making
simple purchases all can be affected by the individual’s
place of residence (urban versus rural, for example), or
their concerns about neighborhood safety. Clinicians
and educators must make educated guesses regarding
a person’s potential for performing a certain task if the
person has not had experience with a particular skill
on the test.
The third and fnal criterion for intellectual disability stipulates that the child’s intellectual and adaptive
defcits must have begun during the developmental
period (generally considered to be before age 18). The
purpose of establishing this upper limit for the age of
onset is twofold. First, it acknowledges that intellectual
disability is a developmental disorder that is evident
during childhood and adolescence. Problems in learning and comprehension typically occur during this time
of rapid brain development. Second, this age criterion
rules out persons who may show mental defciencies
caused by adult-onset degenerative diseases, such as
Alzheimer’s disease, or by head trauma.
C h a p t e r 5 Intellectual Disability (Intellectual Developmental Disorder)
131
A diagnosis of intellectual disability specifes
the level of impairment—mild, moderate, severe, or
profound—similar to other disorders in the DSM-5.
Although these four levels of impairment remain the
same as in previous editions, they are specifed in considerably more detail, as described below. Similarly,
as mentioned above, the DSM-5 does not provide
specifc IQ cutoffs for meeting diagnostic criteria,
to allow greater focus on adaptive functioning and
levels of support that may be required. However, individuals with ID generally have an IQ score 2 standard
deviations or more below the population mean (e.g.,
70 points or below, allowing for a margin of error of
5 points).
Changes in DSM-5 criteria for ID have sought to
focus more on the nature or qualities of the person
rather than on the IQ score, based on years of controversy over IQ measurement and social infuences (Toth
& King, 2010). These criteria are not likely to result in
a major shift in the numbers of people diagnosed with
ID—as before, the majority of people diagnosed with
intellectual disability fall into the mild range. However,
because the ramifcations of diagnosing someone with
intellectual disability can be serious, every attempt is
made to consider other factors that may limit one’s
mental abilities. As Ed described so well, a careful bal-
ance must be struck between identifying the special
needs of persons with intellectual disabilities and labeling them as having a disability on the basis of somewhat arbitrary criteria (Schalock et al., 2007).
In addition, the defnition and identifcation of
intellectual disability depend somewhat on our social
institutions. When children enter the school system, it
is a signifcant point at which their abilities are compared and defciencies are most likely to be detected.
If children are placed in a poorly matched learning
environment, their developmental progress can be disrupted. Following their school years, persons with mild
intellectual disability often blend into the larger population, and their “diagnosis” no longer has as much
meaning (Hodapp et al., 2011).
severity levels
The four levels of severity noted in Table 5.3 are defned on the basis of adaptive functioning in three primary domains: conceptual, social, and practical. These
defnitions refect a major shift in focus away from IQ
scores and more toward needed supports. This shift
emphasizes the resources and strategies necessary to
promote the overall adjustment and well-being of a
person with intellectual disability. Specifc needs of
T A B L E 5 . 3 | Severity levels for Intellectual Disability (Intellectual Developmental Disorder)
Severity
Level
Mild
Conceptual Domain
●
●
●
For preschool children: no
obvious conceptual differences.
For school-aged children and
adults: learning difficulties
in academic skills involving
reading, writing, arithmetic,
time, or money, the support
needed in one or more areas
to meet age-related expectations.
In adults: impaired abstract
thinking, executive function,
short-term memory, and
functional use of academic
skills. There is a somewhat
concrete approach to problems and solutions as compared with age-mates.
Social Domain
Practical Domains
Children:
Children:
●
●
●
●
●
●
●
Show immaturity in social interactions.
Have difficulty in perceiving peers’ social
cues.
Show immature or more concrete communication, conversation, and language
for age.
Show difficulty regulating ageinappropriate emotion and behavior;
may be noticed by peers.
Have limited understanding of risk in
social situations.
Display immature social judgment for
age.
Are at risk of being manipulated by others (gullibility).
●
●
May function age-appropriately and
maintain personal care.
May need some support with complex daily living tasks as compared
with peers.
Adults:
●
●
●
●
●
Typically need support with shopping, transportation, home and child
care, organizing, cooking, and money
management.
Participate in recreational skills similar to age-mates, but need support
with well-being and organization.
May hold jobs that do not emphasize
conceptual skills.
Need support with health care and
legal decisions and with learning a
vocation.
Often need support raising a family.
(continues)
132
P a r t 2 Neurodevelopmental Disorders
T A B L E 5 . 3 | Severity levels for Intellectual Disability (Intellectual Developmental Disorder) (continued)
Severity
Level
Moderate
Conceptual Domain
Social Domain
Practical Domains
Conceptual skills lag markedly
behind those of peers.
Individual:
Adults:
●
●
●
●
●
Severe
For preschoolers, language
and pre-academic skills
develop slowly.
For school-aged children,
academic skill develops
slowly and are limited as
compared with those of
peers.
For adults, academic skill
development is typically at
an elementary level, and support is required for all use of
academic skills and work and
personal life.
Adults need daily assistance
with conceptual tasks of dayto-day life.
Individual:
●
●
●
Attains few conceptual skills.
Has little understanding of
written language or of concepts involving numbers.
Needs caretakers to provide
problem-solving support
throughout life.
●
●
●
●
●
●
●
Individual:
●
●
●
●
●
●
Profound
Shows marked differences from peers
in social and communicative behavior
across development.
Uses less complex spoken language as
primary tool for social communication.
Shows capacity for relationships in lifelong ties to family and friends.
May inaccurately perceive or interpret
social cues.
Shows limited social judgment and
decision-making abilities.
Needs caretakers to assist with life
decisions.
Has friendships with typically developing
peers that are often affected by
communication or social limitations.
Needs social and communicative support
in work settings.
Exhibits limited spoken language in
terms of vocabulary and grammar.
Speaks using single words or phrases
and may be supplemented through augmentative means.
Exhibits speech and communication
focused on the present.
Uses language for social communication
more than for explication.
Understands simple speech and gestural
communication.
Has relationships with family members
and familiar others for pleasure and help.
Individual:
Conceptual skills generally
involve the physical world
Has very limited understanding of
rather than symbolic processes.
symbolic communication in speech or
gestures.
Individual:
May understand some simple instrucMay use objects in goaltions or gestures.
directed fashion for self-care,
Nonverbally, nonsymbolically expresses
work, and recreation.
most desires and emotions.
Uses visuospatial skills, such
Enjoys relationships with well-known
as matching and sorting
family members, caretakers, and familiar
based on physical characterothers.
istics. However, co-occurring
Initiates a response to social interactions
motor and sensory impairthrough gestural and emotional cues.
ments may prevent funcMay have co-occurring sensory and
tional use of objects.
physical impairments, which may prevent many social activities.
●
●
●
●
●
●
Individual:
●
●
●
●
●
●
●
●
●
Needs support for daily living
(e.g., eating, dressing, bathing,
elimination).
Needs supervision at all times.
Unable to make responsible decisions regarding well-being of self or
others.
Needs long-term teaching and ongoing support in all domains to acquire
skills.
Maladaptive behavior, including
self-injury, is present in a significant
minority.
Individual:
●
●
Eat, dress, eliminate, and practice
hygiene age-appropriately after
extended teaching, time practicing,
and reminders.
Participate in all typical household
tasks after extended period of teaching and ongoing.
May achieve independent employment in jobs that require limited
conceptual communication skills, but
need considerable support from
co-workers/supervisors.
Develop a variety of recreational
skills, but will need additional,
extended supports and learning
opportunities.
Maladaptive behaviors present in a
significant minority and cause social
problems.
●
●
●
●
●
●
●
Is dependent on others for all aspects
of daily physical care, health, and
safety, but may participate in some of
these activities.
Without severe physical impairments
may assist with some daily work tasks
at home.
May perform simple actions with
objects, with extended support,
related to vocational activities.
Enjoys recreational activities with the
support of others.
May have physical and sensory
impairments that prevent participation (beyond watching) in home, recreational, and vocational activities.
May exhibit maladaptive behaviors
(only a significant minority.)
Source: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition, American Psychiatric Association.
C h a p t e r 5 Intellectual Disability (Intellectual Developmental Disorder)
133
the individual are evaluated and strategies and services are developed to optimize individual functioning. Concrete examples of support areas and support
activities are shown in Table 5.4.
Children with intellectual disability vary widely in
their degree of disability. Some show cognitive impairments from early infancy, such as limited vocalizations
or poor self-regulation, whereas other impairments
may go relatively unnoticed throughout the elementary
school years. Because of the wide variation in cognitive
functioning and impairment, classifcation systems for
intellectual disability have always attempted to delineate
various degrees of cognitive impairment. The DSM-5
has continued the tradition by specifying disability as
mild, moderate, severe, or profound; however, these designations are based primarily on ability and needed supports rather than on IQ scores, as in the past.
Children and adolescents with mild intellectual
disability constitute the largest group; it is estimated
that as many as 85% of people with ID have the mild
form of the disorder (King et al., 2009). Children with
mild intellectual disability often show small delays in
development during the preschool years, but typically
are not identifed until academic or behavior problems
emerge during the early elementary years. This category also has an overrepresentation of minority group
members, most likely due to the social and economic
disparities noted previously.
As a group, children with mild intellectual disability
typically develop social and communication skills during the preschool years (ages 0–5 years), perhaps with
modest delays in expressive language. They usually have
minimal or no sensorimotor impairment and engage with
peers readily (although peers may see them as immature).
Like Matthew, some children with mild ID may fnd
school and peer relationships to be challenging. By their
late teens, these children can acquire academic skills up
to approximately the sixth-grade level. During their adult
years, they usually achieve social and vocational skills
adequate for minimum self-support, but may need supervision, guidance, and assistance, especially when under
unusual social or economic stress. With appropriate supports, individuals with mild intellectual disability usually
live successfully in the community, either independently
or in supervised settings.
Persons with moderate intellectual disability constitute about 10% of individuals with ID. Children
and adolescents at this level of impairment are more
intellectually and adaptively impaired than someone
with mild intellectual disability, and usually they are
identifed during the preschool years, when they show
delays in reaching early developmental milestones. By
134
P a r t 2 Neurodevelopmental Disorders
TABLE 5.4
|
Examples of Support Areas and
Support Activities
Teaching and Education Activities:
●
●
●
●
Interacting with trainers and teachers and fellow trainees
and students
Learning and using problem-solving strategies
Using technology for learning
Learning and using functional academics (reading signs,
counting change, etc.)
Home Living Activities:
●
●
●
●
Preparing and eating food
Housekeeping and cleaning
Dressing
Bathing and taking care of personal hygiene and grooming
needs
Community Living Activities:
●
●
●
●
Using transportation
Participating in recreation and leisure activities
Visiting friends and family
Shopping and purchasing goods
Employment Activities:
●
●
●
●
Learning and using specific job skills
Interacting with co-workers
Completing work-related tasks with speed and quality
Accessing and obtaining crisis intervention and assistance
Health and Safety Activities:
●
●
●
●
Accessing and obtaining therapy services
Avoiding health and safety hazards
Accessing emergency services
Maintaining mental health/emotional well-being
Behavioral Activities:
●
●
●
●
Learning and making appropriate decisions
Incorporating personal preferences into daily activities
Maintaining socially appropriate behavior in public
Controlling anger and aggression
Social Activities:
●
●
●
●
Participating in recreation and leisure activities
Making appropriate sexual decisions
Making and keeping friends
Engaging in loving and intimate relationships
Protection and Advocacy Activities:
●
●
●
●
Managing money and personal finances
Protecting self from exploitation
Exercising legal rights and responsibilities
Using banks and cashing checks
Source: Frequently Asked Questions on Intellectual Disability and the AAIDD
Definition, by American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities
(AAIDD), 2010.
the time they enter school, these children may communicate through a combination of single words and
gestures, and show self-care and motor skills similar to
an average 2- to 3-year-old. Many persons with Down
syndrome function at the moderate level of impairment.
Because their social judgment and decision-making
abilities are limited, children and youth with moderate
ID often require supportive services to function on a
daily basis.
Like Vanessa, most individuals with this level of
intellectual disability acquire limited communication
skills during their early years, and by age 12 they may be
using practical communication skills. They beneft from
vocational training and, with moderate supervision, can
attend to their personal care. They also can beneft from
training in social and occupational skills, but they are
unlikely to progress beyond the second-grade level in
academic subjects. Adolescents with moderate intellectual disability often have diffculty recognizing social
conventions, such as appropriate dress or humor, which
interferes with peer relationships. By adulthood, persons
with moderate intellectual disability typically adapt well
to living in the community and can perform unskilled or
semiskilled work under supervision in sheltered workshops (specialized manufacturing facilities that train
and supervise persons with intellectual disability) or in
the general workforce.
Those with severe intellectual disability constitute approximately 4% of persons with ID. Most of
these individuals suffer one or more organic causes of
impairment, such as genetic defects, and are identifed at
a very young age because they have substantial delays in
development and visible physical features or anomalies.
Milestones such as standing, walking, and toilet training may be markedly delayed, and basic self-care skills
are usually acquired by about age 9. In addition to intellectual impairment, they may have problems with physical mobility or other health-related problems, such as
respiratory, heart, or physical complications.
Most persons functioning at the severe level of
intellectual disability require some special assistance
throughout their lives. During early childhood they
acquire little or no communicative speech; by age
12, they may use some two- to three-word phrases.
Between 13 and 15 years of age, their academic and
adaptive abilities are similar to those of an average
4- to 6-year-old. They proft to a limited extent from instruction in pre-academic subjects, such as familiarity
with the alphabet and simple counting, and can master
skills such as sight reading “survival” words such as
“hot,” “danger,” and “stop.” During their adult years,
they may be able to perform simple tasks in closely
supervised settings. Most adapt well to life in the
community, in group homes or with their families, unless they have an associated disability that requires
specialized nursing or other care (Toth & King, 2010).
Persons with profound intellectual disability constitute approximately 2% of those with ID. Such individuals typically are identifed in infancy because of marked
delays in development and biological anomalies such
as asymmetrical facial features. During early childhood
they show considerable impairments in sensorimotor
functioning; by the age of 4 years, for example, their
responsiveness is similar to that of a typical 1-year-old.
They are able to learn only the rudiments of communication skills, and they require intensive training to
learn basic eating, grooming, toileting, and dressing
behaviors.
Persons with profound intellectual disability require lifelong care and assistance. Almost all of these
individuals show organic causes for their ID, and many
have severe co-occurring medical conditions, such as
congenital heart defect or epilepsy, that sometimes
lead to death during childhood or early adulthood.
Most of these individuals live in supervised group
homes or small, specialized facilities. Optimal development may occur in a highly structured environment
with constant aid and supervision and an individualized relationship with a caregiver. Motor development,
as well as self-care and communication skills, may improve if appropriate training is provided. For example,
persons with profound intellectual disability usually
can perform simple tasks, such as washing their hands
and changing their clothes, provided they have close
supervision.
The DSM-5 criteria for ID focuses on needed supports, which refects the consensus that determining
the level of functioning of an individual with intellectual disability should be based on the interaction
between the person and the environment (AAIDD,
2010). The criteria also remove specifc IQ cutoff
scores or ranges, which acknowledges that IQ scores
are approximations of a person’s mental abilities that
require additional knowledge of their actual abilities. With a DSM-5 diagnosis of intellectual disability
(moderate severity), Vanessa would likely also receive
the following descriptive diagnosis: “Vanessa is a child
with intellectual disability who needs limited supports
in home living, academic skills, and development of
self-help skills.” Matthew’s diagnosis of intellectual
disability (mild severity) might also clarify: “Matt is a
child with intellectual disability who requires support
on an as-needed basis, especially during stressful or
demanding times—for example, during the transition
to school, when making new friends, and when faced
with new academic challenges.”
C h a p t e r 5 Intellectual Disability (Intellectual Developmental Disorder)
135
Based on available evidence and estimates, the total
number of children and adults with intellectual disability is approximately 1% of the general population
(Maulik et al., 2011). However, each person applies his
or her own cognitive abilities in unique ways that may
be more or less adaptive in her or his own environment. Thus, prevalence estimates vary across time and
across countries as a result of IQ measurement problems, different defnitions of ID, and varying study designs (Witwer, Lawton, & Aman, in press).
Sex Ratio
Among children with mild intellectual disability (by far
the most common diagnosis), males outnumber females
at a ratio of 1.6:1. Males outnumber females among
the other levels of severity as well, but these ratios are
inconsistent across studies and tend to be small. Similar to racial differences in the diagnosis of intellectual
disability, gender differences in ID may be an artifact of identifcation and referral patterns rather than
true differences in prevalence (Einfeld et al., 2010). If
a true male excess of intellectual disability does exist, researchers suspect this may be due to the occurrence of X-linked genetic disorders such as fragile-X
syndrome (discussed later in the chapter), which affect
males more often than females (Handen, 2007).
Socioeconomic and Cultural Infuences
It is a well-established fnding that intellectual disability
is more prevalent among children of lower socioeconomic status (SES) and children from minority groups
(Witwer et al., in press). This link is found primarily
among children in the mild intellectual disability range;
children with more severe levels are identifed almost
equally in different racial and economic groups. Whether
or not signs of organic etiology are present, diagnoses
of mild intellectual disability increase sharply from near
zero among children from higher SES categories to about
2.5% in the lowest SES category (APA, 2000). These fgures indicate that SES factors play a suspected role both
in the cause of intellectual disability and in the identifcation and labeling of persons with intellectual disability
(Maulik et al., 2011).
The overrepresentation of minority and low-SES
children in the group with mild intellectual disability
is a complicated and unresolved issue. As we noted,
average IQ levels for the African American population
are lower than IQ levels found in the white population,
resulting in more African American children among
samples of children with mild intellectual disability.
What specifc environmental circumstances might create such an imbalance in IQ fndings? To answer this
136
P a r t 2 Neurodevelopmental Disorders
Black-White IQ differences (by points)
prevalence
220
218
216
214
212
210
28
Child
characteristics
Family
income
Maternal
Home
characteristics environment
● F I G U R E 5 . 1 | Factors accounting for differences in IQ
scores between white and African American children at
5 years of age.
Based on data from Brooks-Gunn, J., Klebanov, P. K., Smith, J., Duncan, G. J., & Lee, K.
(2003). The Black-White test score gap in young children: Contributions of test and family characteristics. Applied Developmental Science, 7(4), pp. 239–252
question, Brooks-Gunn et al. (2003) tested the theory
that the differences can be partially explained based
on social and economic disadvantage. They accounted
for initial IQ differences of over 17 points in African
American versus white children by the independent effects of economic deprivation, home environment, and
maternal characteristics. As shown in ● Figure 5.1,
initial IQ differences were almost 18 points between
a sample of African American and white children at
5 years of age, controlling for gender and birth weight.
However, these differences were reduced by about 71%
after adjusting for differences in poverty and home environment. Similar gains in IQ performance are found
when children are adopted from working-class into
middle-class homes, reinforcing the signifcant role of
the environment in shaping one’s intellectual and adaptive abilities (Nisbett et al., 2012). These data remind
us that the likelihood of a diagnosis of intellectual disability is shaped and infuenced by social and cultural
forces such as racial discrimination, poverty, and cultural insensitivity (Emerson, 2012).
Section Summary
Features of Intellectual Disabilities
●
●
The DSM-5 criteria for intellectual disability consist of defcits in intellectual functioning (confrmed by IQ testing
and clinical assessment), defcits in adaptive functioning,
and onset of intellectual and adaptive defcits during the
developmental period.
Children with intellectual disability vary widely in their degree of disability and level of functioning.
●
●
●
IQ scores are no longer used to determine level of impairment. Rather, DSM-5 describes four levels of severity—
mild, moderate, severe, or profound—based on adaptive
functioning that determines a person’s level of needed
supports.
Intellectual disability occurs in an estimated 1% of the
population, more often among males than females.
Intellectual disability occurs more often among children
from lower socioeconomic and minority groups. Economic
disadvantage and discrimination practices often account
for these fndings.
DEVELOPMENTAL COURSE AND ADULT
OUTCOMES
To appreciate the manner in and extent to which children with intellectual disabilities achieve various developmental milestones, consider how typically developing
children express themselves. An infant exploring his or
her world relies on primitive sensorimotor functions—
touching, tasting, and manipulating objects—to learn
about the environment. At this stage of development
everything is new, and the brain is establishing literally
millions of new connections each day.
Then, between 18 and 24 months of age, the toddler
begins to acquire language and to draw on memories
of past experience to aid in understanding the present.
For an intellectually normal child, it is during this stage
that the child’s environmental conditions and opportunities are known to play a crucial role in fostering
enthusiasm for learning and in establishing the roots
of intellectual sophistication. Although the majority of
children with ID progress through each developmental
milestone in roughly the same manner as other children, important differences in their developmental accomplishments are evident.
Much of the knowledge about other issues involved
in the developmental course and adult outcomes for
children with intellectual disability is derived from
studies of children with Down syndrome. Chromosome
abnormalities are the single most common cause of moderate to severe intellectual disability. Down syndrome
is the most common disorder resulting from these
abnormalities. children with Down syndrome, along
with their parents, have frequently participated in studies comparing their development with that of their
normally developing peers.
Dan, a 15-year-old with Down syndrome, describes
how his early development was similar to, but much
slower than, that of his younger brother.
Dan, with moderate intellectual disability, is describing how his younger brother Brian, with normal
DAN
With His Brother’s Help
When I was almost 3 and my sister was 5, we had a
baby brother. I helped feed him and take care of him
until he was almost 3. I was 5 by then, and could do
most of the things he could do, but about that time he
caught up to me. I could still do some things he couldn’t,
but not many. He could do some things I had trouble
with. We became good friends. Every time he learned a
new skill, I would either learn it with him, or he would
teach me later. I was really little for my age, so we were
pretty close in size. We shared a bunk bed, toys, and
clothes. We learned to do a lot of things together. When
he learned to ride a bike, and I wasn’t ready to learn yet,
mom and dad got me a Powerwheel motorized bike so I
could ride, too. When he learned to read, he taught me
how, too. When he played baseball and football, he took
me with him.
In those days, I still went to school in another district,
so most of my friends were his friends. Now that we go to
the same school it is sometimes hard for him to remember that I have my own friends, too. I have to tell him I
am the big brother. He sometimes gets teased at school
because he is my brother, but he is learning to explain
instead of fght. Mostly, the kids are all nice to us. (Based
on authors’ case material.)
intelligence, caught up with him by the time Brian was
2 years old, and progressed through developmental milestones at a faster pace. Does Dan’s development follow the same organized sequence as Brian’s?
Will his development show specifc defcits in certain intellectual abilities such as language? Or will
he eventually catch up? This case illustrates the developmental-versus-difference controversy (Weiss,
Weisz, & Bromfeld, 1986), an issue that has intrigued
those in the feld of child development and intellectual disability for some time. Simply stated, the
developmental-versus-difference controversy is this: Do
all children—regardless of intellectual impairments—
progress through the same developmental milestones
in a similar sequence, but at different rates? Or do children with intellectual disability develop in a different,
less sequential, and less organized fashion?
The developmental position, which applies primarily to individuals not suffering from organic impairment, consists of two primary hypotheses: First,
the similar sequence hypothesis argues that all children, with or without intellectual disability, pass
through stages of cognitive development in an identical (invariant) order; they differ only in their rate and
C h a p t e r 5 Intellectual Disability (Intellectual Developmental Disorder)
137
138
P a r t 2 Neurodevelopmental Disorders
same developmental sequences as his younger brother,
but at a slower pace.
motivation
Many children who fall within the range of mild intellectual disability are bright enough to learn and to
attend regular schools and classrooms. However, they
are more susceptible to a sense of helplessness and frustration, which places additional burdens on their social
and cognitive development. As a consequence, they begin to expect failure, even for tasks they can master; in
the absence of proper instruction, their motivation to
tackle new demands decreases (Harris, 2006).
Ed, describing his memory of comments made by
his teacher in elementary school, expresses this phenomenon well:
Her negative picture of me stood out like a sore
thumb. That’s the problem with people like me—the
schools and teachers fnd out we have problems, they
notice them, and then we are abandoned. That one
teacher was very annoyed that I was in her class. She
had to put up with me. (Bogdan & Taylor, 1982)
Children with intellectual disability, consequently,
expect little success, set lower goals for themselves, and
settle for minimal success when they are able to do better, as compared with typically developing children of
their same mental age (Weisz, 1999). This learned helplessness may be unwittingly condoned by adults. When
they are told a child is “retarded,” adults are less likely
to urge that child to persist after failure than they are to
urge a normal child at the same level of cognitive development. On the other hand, young children with mild
intellectual disability improve in their ability to remain
on task and they develop goal-directed behavior when
©Denis Kuvaev/Shutterstock.com
upper limit of development (Bennett-Gates & Zigler,
1998). Second, the similar structure hypothesis suggests that children with intellectual disability demonstrate the same behaviors and underlying processes
as typically developing children at the same level of
cognitive functioning (such as Dan and his younger
brother were at ages 5 and 2). That is, if children with
intellectual disability are matched to typically developing children by their mental age, then the children
with intellectual disability will show equivalent performance on cognitive tasks, such as problem solving,
spelling, and moral reasoning. The developmental position rejects the notion of a specifc defcit or difference among children with intellectual disability and
instead emphasizes how these children traverse the
stages more slowly and attain a lower developmental
ceiling than typically developing children (BennettGates & Zigler, 1998).
In contrast, the difference viewpoint argues that
cognitive development of children with intellectual disability differs from that of children without intellectual
disability in more than developmental rate and upper
limit. According to this position, even when his mental
age is matched to his younger brother’s, Dan will show
qualitatively different reasoning and problem-solving
strategies, and he may never be able to accomplish
some tasks beyond a certain level.
Evidence supports the developmental hypothesis
for children with familial, not organic, types of intellectual disability, but this issue has not yet been resolved. Children with familial intellectual disability
generally follow developmental stages in an invariant
order, the same as children with normal intellectual
abilities, with the possible exception of some children with co-occurring brain abnormalities or autism
(Bennett-Gates & Zigler, 1998). The similar structure
hypothesis has also been supported for children with
familial intellectual disability, with some exceptions.
Children with familial intellectual disability show
slight defcits in memory and information processing
as compared with mental-age–matched children without intellectual disability (Schuchardt, Gebhardt, &
Mäehler, 2010), which may be due to the children’s
diffculty in staying motivated to perform repetitive,
boring tasks (Weisz, 1999).
The picture for children with organically based
intellectual disability (such as Dan, who has Down
syndrome) is more straightforward. They often have
one or more specifc defcit areas that cause them to
perform more poorly than mental-age–matched children without intellectual disability. Thus, Dan is likely
to show some differences in his performance in certain areas of development, including his expressive
language. Nevertheless, he will likely pass through the
“Acknowledge our children’s differences but respect their
uniqueness.” —Parent of a child with Down syndrome
provided with stimulating environments and caregiver
support (Wilkins & Matson, 2009).
changes in abilities
Intellectual disability is not necessarily a lifelong disorder. Although it is a relatively stable condition from
childhood into adulthood, the IQ score can fuctuate in relation to the level of impairment and type of
intellectual disability. Children such as Matthew who
have mild intellectual disability may, with appropriate training and opportunities, develop good adaptive skills in other domains and may exceed the level
of impairment required for a diagnosis of intellectual
disability.
The major cause of a child’s intellectual disability certainly affects the degree to which his or her IQ
and adaptive abilities may change. The IQ of children with Down syndrome, who are not representative of the course of intellectual disability in general,
may plateau during the middle childhood years and
then decrease over time. For example, from 1 to 6
years of age, children with Down syndrome often
show signifcant age-related gains in adaptive functioning, but as they grow older, their pace of development levels off or even declines (Margallo-Lana et
al., 2007). A similar deceleration is often seen in the
rate of social development of these children as they
grow older (Hazlett et al., 2011). This observation
has been termed the slowing and stability hypothesis
(Hodapp et al., 2011), and it affrms that children
with Down syndrome may alternate between periods
of gain in functioning and periods of little or no advance. Although these children continue to develop in
intelligence, they do so at progressively slower rates
throughout the childhood years.
language and social behavior
Research on language development and social functioning among children and adolescents with Down
syndrome suggests that their development follows a
largely predictable and organized course (Filippi &
Karmiloff-Smith, 2013). Because their cognitive development, play, self-knowledge, and knowledge of others
are interrelated in organized and meaningful ways, the
underlying symbolic abilities in children with Down
syndrome are believed to be largely intact.
However, important differences in language
development exist between children with Down syndrome and their typically developing age-mates. Perhaps the most striking difference for children with
Down syndrome is the considerable delay in the expressive language development that is necessary to
establish independent living skills. Their expressive
language is often much weaker than their receptive
language, especially as they attain communication
abilities beyond the 24-month level (Filippi &
Karmiloff-Smith, 2013).
Another major milestone of infancy and early childhood development is the ability to form secure attachments with caregivers. Although their attachments form
more slowly than usual, many children with Down
syndrome form secure attachment relationships with
their caregivers by 12 to 24 months of developmental
age (Dykens, Hodapp, & Evans, 2006). Still, a signifcant number of these children may have problems in
developing a secure attachment because they express
less emotion than other children. In one study, children
with Down syndrome were not picked up and held by
either the mother or the stranger in the strange situation to the same extent as non-delayed children. (See
Table 2.2 for a description of the “strange situation”
method of assessing child–caregiver attachment.) Even
when these children made approaches with appropriate
signals for contact, mothers and strangers rarely completed the contact, presumably because the children
did not show the distress signals of crying, reaching, or
holding on that typically tell the parent “I want to be
picked up!” (Vaughn, Contreras, & Seifer, 1994). This
fnding has important implications for parents of young
children with Down syndrome: Even though they may
show few signals of distress or desire for contact, these
infants and toddlers need to be held and nurtured just
as others do.
Following the attachment period, the next important developmental milestones relate to the emergence
of a sense of self, which establishes the early foundations of personality. Like other children, toddlers with
Down syndrome begin to delight at recognizing themselves in mirrors and photos, although this milestone
is often delayed. The experience of self-recognition in
most infants is immediately met by smiles and laughter, and this experience is also found among toddlers
with Down syndrome (Mans, Cicchetti, & Sroufe,
1978). This positive affect accompanying their visual
self-recognition suggests that these children feel good
about themselves. However, as toddlers and preschoolers, children with Down syndrome show delayed and
aberrant functioning in their internal state language,
the language that refects the emergent sense of self and
others (through the use of words such as “mad” and
“happy”). Because internal state language is critical to
regulating social interaction and providing a foundation for early self–other understanding, these children
may be at increased risk for subsequent problems in
the development of the self-system (Huck, Kemp, &
Carter, 2010).
C h a p t e r 5 Intellectual Disability (Intellectual Developmental Disorder)
139
Children with intellectual disability, especially
those with moderate to mild impairments, learn symbolic play—games, puppets, and sports—in much the
same manner as do other children. Nevertheless, they
often fail to gain their peers’ acceptance in regular
education settings, because they may have defcits in
social skills and social–cognitive abilities (Cook &
Oliver, 2011). Concerns about the social development
of children with intellectual disability are increasing
as a result of the movement to include children with
different levels of ability in regular classrooms and
schools, rather than only placing them in institutions
or specialized facilities. Typically developing children
seem to prefer playing with other typically developing children, and as a result, children with intellectual disability are more socially isolated from other
children their age (Guralnick, Connor, & Johnson,
2011). These integrated classrooms allow children
with intellectual disabilities, despite their limited
social skills, to interact with typically developing
peers, which in turn has a positive impact on their
social status (Leffert, Siperstein, & Widaman, 2010;
Siperstein, Glick, & Parker, 2009).
emotional and behavioral problems
PAT T I E
Disturbed or Disturbing?
Pattie was labeled mentally retarded and lived in over
20 homes and institutions before being committed to a
state school at age 10. At the age of 20, she discussed
some of her experiences and feelings: “I guess I was very
disturbed. I call it disturbed, but it was when I was very
upset. A lot of people at (the institution) … told me I was
disturbed—that I was disturbed and that I was retarded—
so I fgure that all through my life I was disturbed.
Looking at the things I done, I must have been disturbed.
… Upset and disturbed are the same in my mind. Crazy
to me is something else. It is somebody that is really
gone. I mean really out. Just deliberately kill somebody
just to do it. That is what I call crazy. I guess what I was
was emotionally disturbed—yeah. Emotionally disturbed
is a time when too many things are bothering me. They
just build up till I get too nervous or upset. My mind just
goes through all these changes and different things. So
many things inside that were bothering me.” (Based on
Bogdan & Taylor, 1982.)
Pattie’s description of her feelings while living in various institutions illuminates how “disturbing” her behavior could be. But are her feelings a function of her
140
P a r t 2 Neurodevelopmental Disorders
environment and personal limitations? Many children
and adolescents with intellectual disability have to face
many obstacles related to their intellectual, physical,
and social impairments, and often they have little control over their own lives.
In the early 1970s, a major study was conducted
to gain some understanding of the extent of psychiatric disorders among children and adults with and
without intellectual disability (Rutter et al., 1976).
Ratings by both parents and teachers revealed that
about one-third of the children with mild intellectual
disability and one-half of the children with more severe forms of intellectual disability showed major signs
of emotional disturbance, suggesting that these problems are common. Since then, research has estimated
that the risk of emotional and behavioral disturbances among children with intellectual disabilities
is approximately three to fve times greater than
among typically developing children (Einfeld, Ellis,
& Emerson, 2011). These problems are due largely
to limited communication skills, additional stressors,
and more neurological defcits faced by these children
and youths (Adams & Oliver, 2011).
The nature and course of psychiatric disorders in children and adolescents with and without
intellectual disabilities is likely very similar. Problem
behaviors of youths with mild and moderate disabilities are similarly stable and persistent over the course
of their development (de Ruiter et al., 2008; Einfeld
et al., 2011). Impulse control disorders, anxiety disorders, and mood disorders are the most commonly
diagnosed psychiatric diagnoses for children with
intellectual disability. Although these problems are
sometimes severe and often require intervention, they
are considered to be part of the spectrum of problems that coexist with intellectual disability, and as
not indicators of other psychiatric illnesses (Hodapp
et al., 2006). By early adulthood, persons with intellectual disabilities continue to show a greater risk
for psychopathology than the general population,
although problems in attention and aggression show
a signifcant decline from childhood rates (Buckles,
Luckasson, & Keefe, 2013).
Adjustments usually are needed in how DSM-5
diagnostic criteria for other mental disorders are applied, however. The frequency of temper tantrums,
hyperactivity, and mood disorders among these children requires consideration of what is normal or
typical for other children with similar levels of intellectual disability. For example, the diagnosis of attention-defcit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) requires
the presence of behavioral disturbance that is inappropriate for an individual’s developmental level.
Attention spans, distractibility, and on-task behaviors
vary considerably among individuals with profound
intellectual disability. An individual with profound
intellectual disability must be compared with other
children with profound intellectual disability for the
purpose of diagnosing any other psychiatric disturbance (APA, 2013).
Internalizing Problems
Adolescence is a developmental period of increased
risk for mood disorders and other internalizing symptoms, which is especially true for those with intellectual disability (Hodapp & Dykens, 2009). Like their
normally developing peers, adolescents with Down
syndrome and other forms of intellectual disability
may show a decline in their previously sociable and
cheerful behaviors, and in some cases they may suffer from signifcant symptoms of depression and social
withdrawal.
ADHD-Related Symptoms
Teachers and parents of children and adolescents with
intellectual disability commonly report ADHD-related
symptoms that require adjustments in instruction and
child-management strategies (Neece et al., 2013).
When a teacher is present to prompt the appropriate
behavior and participate in the activity, children with
intellectual disability with and without ADHD generally will remain on task. However, when instructed to
work without teacher assistance, differences between
those with and without ADHD emerge (Handen et
al., 1998). When children with intellectual disability and ADHD are placed on stimulant medication,
they are able to remain on task for longer periods and
their accuracy on cognitive tasks improves, similar to
the responses of children with normal IQs (Simonoff
et al., 2013).
Other Symptoms
Children and adults with intellectual disability may
show additional symptoms that can be particularly
troublesome. Pica (discussed in Chapter 14), which
can result in the ingestion of caustic and dangerous substances, is seen in its more serious forms
among children and adults with intellectual disability.
Self-injurious behavior (SIB) is a serious and sometimes life-threatening problem that affects about one
in fve young children with ID (MacLean & Dornbush,
2012). Some common forms of SIB include head banging, eye gouging, severe scratching, rumination, some
types of pica, and inserting objects under the skin. The
long-term prognoses for pica and SIBs are not favorable. Emotional withdrawal, stereotypies (frequent
repetition of the same posture, movement, or form of
speech—e.g., head banging, hand or body movements),
and avoidance of eye contact are often still evident
more than 20 years later among persons with more severe forms of intellectual disability (Taylor, Oliver, &
Murphy, 2011).
Thus, children with intellectual disability may
show emotional and behavioral problems that require
special recognition and learning strategies. In general,
these problems do not constitute major psychiatric
disorders, but they do refect the greater challenges
these children may have in learning to express their
needs and in adapting to their surroundings. A 7-yearold girl with mild intellectual disability, for instance,
may be at a developmental level comparable to that
of a typically developing 4-year-old. In the classroom,
therefore, she may have diffculty sitting in her seat
and remaining on task. She may not always control
her emotions or her behavior as well as other 7-yearolds in the class, leading to occasional outbursts of
laughter or anger. It is important to keep these problems within a developmental perspective. We would
not expect a 4-year-old to behave as well in the classroom as an older child, and expectations and teaching
methods have to be adjusted accordingly. As expressed
so well by Ed and Pattie, labeling a child with a diagnostic term that implies pathology or inability is
often ill-conceived and counterproductive. Such terms
must be used sparingly—only in circumstances, such
as self-injurious behaviors, for which special attention
is warranted.
other physical and Health disabilities
Children with intellectual disabilities may also suffer
other physical and developmental disabilities that can
affect their health and development in pervasive ways.
Such disabilities are usually related to the degree of
intellectual impairment. Based on a meta-analysis of
31 studies, the prevalence of chronic health conditions
in this population is much higher than in the general
population (Oeseburg et al., 2011). ● Figure 5.2 shows
some of the more common developmental disabilities
found among children with intellectual disabilities.
Despite major co-occurring physical and intellectual
disabilities, children and adults with Down syndrome
now have a life expectancy approaching 60 years,
largely as a result of better medical treatments for
respiratory infections and congenital heart disorders (Dykens, 2013). However, most individuals with
Down syndrome who live beyond the age of 40 demonstrate cognitive decline (much like Alzheimer’s disease) due to gene damage on chromosome 21 (Torr
et al., 2010).
C h a p t e r 5 Intellectual Disability (Intellectual Developmental Disorder)
141
30
20
10
Prevalence
per 100 children
0
tic r
tis rde
Au iso
d
e
n om
ow r
D nd l er
sy ona ord
iti is
os t d
pp an
O vi
de
y
et r
xi de
r
An iso
d
l
ra
eb
er y
C ls
pa
y
ps
Ep
ile
F I G U R E 5 . 2 | Chronic health conditions among
children with intellectual disabilities.
●
Based on Data from Oeseburg et al. (2011)
Section Summary
Developmental Course and Adult Outcomes
●
●
●
●
Children with intellectual disability follow developmental
stages in the same order as do typically developing children. However, their goals and motivation are reduced
over time because of feelings of frustration, which often
lead to expectations of failure.
Adaptive skills and level of impairment may improve over
time, especially for children with mild intellectual disability, if appropriate training and opportunities are provided.
Developmental disabilities, such as speech and language
problems and behavioral disturbances, are common. Emotional and behavioral problems are considered part of the
spectrum of problems coexisting with intellectual disability, rather than indicators of mental disorder.
Children with intellectual disability have a greater chance
of having other physical and developmental disabilities,
such as cerebral palsy, epilepsy, and emotional and behavioral disorders that can afect their health and development in pervasive ways.
CAUSES
It is astounding to consider that there are over 1,000
genetic disorders associated with intellectual disability,
in addition to other organic causes (Hodapp & Dykens,
2009). Yet, despite the number of known causes, scientists are unsure of the causes of the majority of intellectual disability, especially mild ID. A genetic or
environmental cause is known for almost two-thirds
of individuals with moderate to profound intellectual
disability, whereas the causes are known for only about
142
P a r t 2 Neurodevelopmental Disorders
one-quarter of the individuals with mild intellectual
disability (McDermott et al., 2007; van Bokhoven,
2011). Some causes happen before birth (prenatal), as
is the case with all genetic disorders and accidents in
the womb. Other causes are birth-related (perinatal)
insults, such as prematurity or a lack of oxygen (anoxia) at birth. Still other causes are an infammation of
the brain lining (meningitis), head trauma, and other
factors that occur after birth (postnatal).
Historically, causes of intellectual disabilities were
divided into two fairly distinct groups—an organic
group and the cultural–familial group (Hodapp &
Dykens, 2003). The causes in the organic group have
a clear biological basis and are usually associated with
severe and profound intellectual disability, whereas
causes in the cultural–familial group have no clear organic basis and are usually associated with mild intellectual disability (Hodapp et al., 2006).
Four major categories of risk factors contribute to
intellectual disabilities: biomedical, social, behavioral,
and educational. These four risk factors interact across
time and even across generations from parent to child
and provide a more comprehensive explanation of the
interacting causes of problems associated with intellectual impairments (AAIDD, 2010; Chapman, Scott, &
Stanton-Chapman, 2008). The defnitions, characteristics, and causes of intellectual disability on the basis of
these four risk factors are summed up in Table 5.5.
Although a distinction between organic and nonorganic risk factors clarifes the underlying causes of
intellectual disability, keep in mind that the distinctions
are less clear in milder forms of intellectual disability
than they are in more severe forms. For example, the
large majority of persons at more severe levels of intellectual impairment (96%) show a clear etiology for the
disorder, whereas a sizable percentage (32%) of those
with mild impairments does not (Simonoff et al., 1996).
Considerable knowledge exists about organic
intellectual disability because of the strong biological factors involved. Also, the increased ability
to diagnose organic problems has led to increased
estimates of this type of intellectual disability relative to cultural–familial causes—about one-third to
one-half of all persons with intellectual disability
show a clear organic cause (Hodapp et al., 2006).
In stark contrast, the cultural–familial group remains somewhat of a mystery, although it comprises
one-half to two-thirds of all persons with intellectual disability (Witwer et al., in press). As noted in
Table 5.5, the prime suspects are environmental and
situational factors such as poverty, inadequate child
care, poor nutrition, and parental psychopathology,
which mostly affect the psychological, and not the
biological, development of the child. However, more
T A B L E 5 . 5 | Risk Factors for Intellectual Disability
Timing
Biomedical
Social
Behavioral
Educational
Prenatal
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
7.
1.
2.
3.
4.
1.
2.
3.
4.
1. Parental cognitive
disability without
supports
2. Lack of preparation for
parenthood
Perinatal
1. Prematurity
2. Birth injury
3. Neonatal disorders
1. Lack of access to
prenatal care
1. Parental rejection of
caretaking
2. Parental abandonment
of child
1. Lack of medical referral
for intervention services
at discharge
Postnatal
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
1. Impaired child–caregiver
interaction
2. Lack of adequate
stimulation
3. Family poverty
4. Chronic illness in the
family
5. Institutionalization
1. Child abuse and neglect
2. Domestic violence
3. Inadequate safety
measures
4. Social deprivation
5. Difficult child behaviors
1. Impaired parenting
2. Delayed diagnosis
3. Inadequate early
intervention services
4. Inadequate special
education services
5. Inadequate family
support
Chromosomal disorders
Single-gene disorders
Syndromes
Metabolic disorders
Cerebral dysgenesis
Maternal illness
Parental age
Traumatic brain injury
Malnutrition
Meningoencephalitis
Seizure disorders
Degenerative disorders
Poverty
Maternal malnutrition
Domestic violence
Lack of access to
prenatal care
Parental drug use
Parental alcohol use
Parental smoking
Parental immaturity
© Cengage Learning ®
specifc cause-and-effect relationships have not been
Inheritance and the role of the
determined. Accordingly, both genetic and environenvironment
mental factors are implicated in milder forms of
intellectual disability, but in a manner as yet to be
The study of human intelligence has received the
determined (Toth & King, 2010).
lion’s share of attention in terms of the underlying
The relative importance of the environment also
processes involved in genetic makeup and the envistands out in the two-group distinction. The socioecoronmental factors that infuence genetic expression.
nomic background of the organic group is about the
Still, the long-standing debate concerning the relative
same as that for the general population, which fts with
contributions of genes and environment is far from
the notion that severe forms of intellectual disability can
being fully resolved (McDermott et al., 2007). Conaffect anyone, regardless of SES. The familial group is
ceivably, genetic infuences on development are pooverrepresented by those of lower SES and social disadtentially modifable by environmental input, although
vantage and is signifcantly related to a family history of
the practicality of the modifcations is another matintellectual disability. This fts with the assertion that an
ter. Similarly, environmental infuences on developimpoverished social environment can infuence intellecment involve the genes or structures to which the
tual growth and ability in subtle, yet crucial, ways.
genes have contributed (Neisser et al., 1996). Simply
In most cases, the risk factors for intellectual disstated, children do not inherit an IQ—they inherit a
abilities have been supported empirically, with some
genotype, which is a collection of genes that pertain
adjustments as noted earlier in terms of nonorganic
to intelligence. The expression of the genotype in the
risk factors. First, the percentage of individuals with
environment—the gene–environment interaction—is
a clear organic cause has increased over the past few
decades because of the greater knowledge of
genetic and organic causes. Also, the original
assumption that mild intellectual disability is
not due to biomedical (organic) causes had to
be tempered by fndings that epilepsy, cerebral
palsy, and other organic disorders are found
more often among persons with mild intellectual disability than among those without
intellectual disabilities (Hodapp et al., 2011). © 2011 Greg Cravens, Reprinted with permission of Universal Uclick. All rights reserved.
C h a p t e r 5 Intellectual Disability (Intellectual Developmental Disorder)
143
referred to as the phenotype. The heritability of a
trait describes the proportion of the variation of a trait
attributable to genetic infuences in the population
(Neisser et al., 1996). Heritability of any given trait,
therefore, can range from none (0%) to 100% genetically determined.
Is it possible to estimate the heritability of intelligence and, by implication, the heritability of intellectual disability? This intriguing question can now be
answered with some degree of confdence, but little
fanfare. The overwhelming evidence points to a heritability of intelligence of approximately 50%; that
is, both genetic and nongenetic factors play powerful roles in the makeup and expression of intelligence
(Davis, Arden, & Plomin, 2008).
There are so many specifc genetic causes of intellectual disability that some skepticism about the importance of environmental effects still remains. The
diffculty of identifying, pinpointing, and measuring
specifc, nongenetic variables certainly adds to this
dilemma. However, considerable evidence has demonstrated that major environmental variations do affect cognitive performance and social adjustment in
children from disadvantaged backgrounds (Ramey,
Ramey, & Lanzi, 2007). For example, children born to
socially disadvantaged parents and then adopted into
more privileged homes have higher IQ scores, stronger
self-esteem, and fewer acts of delinquency than siblings
reared by their disadvantaged, biological parents (Juffer
& van IJzendoorn, 2007; van der Voort et al., 2013).
The prenatal environment may infuence IQ to
a greater extent than was previously appreciated. A
review of studies of twins and nontwin siblings revealed that a shared prenatal environment (i.e., all
children shared the same mother) accounted for 20%
of IQ similarity...
Purchase answer to see full
attachment