ANTH 332 Lab Assignment 2:
DUE WEDS APRIL 29, 11:58pm Pacific Time
“Evidence from hunter-gatherer and subsistence agricultural populations for the universality of
contagion sensitivity” (Apicella et al. 2018)
As you will see as the class progresses, mating psychology is integrated with some aspects of disgust
psychology, so this assignment has you evaluate an article on one aspect of disgust.
This assignment is aimed at testing your understanding of the scientific method, and how to read and
evaluate a research article. Each question is worth 20 points. Make sure you answer all parts of each
question. Quotations from the article are acceptable as answers so long as they are identified as such
(presented in quotations marks and cited correctly—e.g., (Apicella et al. 2018:355), where 355 in this
example is the page number. Type your answers into this sheet after the question you are answering.
1. What is the psychological phenomenon examined in this study? Define the phenomenon. Describe
an example of the phenomenon given in the article. Give a second example that you have personally
observed: be specific.
2. What is the study’s hypothesis?
3. What were the study populations? What was the researchers’ rationale for using these
populations? What bias is avoided by using these populations?
4. What were the study’s predictions? (make sure to include all the predictions)
5. What methods were used to test the predictions in each population?
6. What was the mean (mathematical average) rejection rate among the Hadza for the three
contaminants compared to the control? What was the mean rejection rate among the Tannese across
the four contaminants, compared to the controls?
7. Among the Hadza, what was the most common reason given for not eating each of the following
(include percentage):
honey contaminated by coughing?
broth contaminated by coughing?
honey contaminated by poison?
broth contaminated by poison?
honey contaminated by bead?
broth contaminated by bead?
8. Although mean rejection rates were higher for contaminants than for control items, mean rejection
rates for the latter were still fairly high. Given the study’s hypothesis, what is a possible explanation
for this response to non-contaminants? In other words, does this finding falsify the hypothesis or
provide support for it?
Evolution and Human Behavior 39 (2018) 355–363
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Evolution and Human Behavior
journal homepage: www.ehbonline.org
Evidence from hunter-gatherer and subsistence agricultural populations
for the universality of contagion sensitivity
Coren L. Apicella a,⁎, Paul Rozin a, Justin T.A. Busch b, Rachel E. Watson-Jones b, Cristine H. Legare b
a
b
The University of Pennsylvania, United States
The University of Texas at Austin, United States
a r t i c l e
i n f o
Article history:
Initial receipt 10 November 2017
Final revision received 6 March 2018
Keywords:
Biological reasoning
Contagion
Contamination
Disgust
Folkbiology
Pathogen avoidance
Small-scale societies
a b s t r a c t
The phenomenon of magical contagion – the unobserved passage of properties between entities that come into
physical contact – was described by anthropologists over a century ago, yet questions remain about its origin,
function, and universality. Contagion sensitivity, along with the emotion of disgust, has been proposed to be
part of a biologically-evolved system designed to reduce exposure to pathogens by increasing the avoidance of
“contaminated” objects. Yet this phenomenon has not been studied using systematic psychological comparison
outside of industrialized populations. Here we document contagion sensitivity in two culturally, geographically,
and economically distinct populations with little exposure to Western biomedicine and formal education: the
Hadza hunter-gatherers of Tanzania and Tannese subsistence-agriculturalists of Vanuatu. In both populations,
a majority of individuals rejected familiar and palatable foods when contaminating items touched the food but
were subsequently removed. The Tannese children in our study showed a similar response, consistent with previous research with Western children. Our data support the proposal that contagion sensitivity is universal in
human populations.
© 2018 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
1. Introduction
The belief that physical contact between two entities often entails
the passage of properties between them, even after contact has been
severed, was labeled as the law of contact or magical contagion by anthropologists N100 years ago (Frazer, 1922/1890; Freeland, 1980;
Mauss, 1972/1902; Tyler, 1974/1871). Magical contagion was thought
to be a ubiquitous and fundamental feature of magical practices and rituals in traditional societies and folklore.1 A common instantiation of this
principle relates to food and disgust: a favored or acceptable food is
often rejected after it has, even briefly, contacted a certain class of offensive objects. A widely held view is that both the contamination
⁎ Corresponding author.
E-mail address: capicella@psych.upenn.edu (C.L. Apicella).
Tyler, Frazer and Mauss provide treatises on the mental foundations of religion, mythical thought and magic. The laws of magic, including the law of contact, were formed by
Tyler and later developed by Frazer and Mauss. All provide the example of magical punishment, whereby a person can be acted upon by others through the use of an object in which
she had once been in contact, including their clothing hair and nails. Frazer further described examples of magical contact that can occur between friends and other relations,
such that the behavior of one affects the behavior of another. Likewise, he documents examples of sympathetic eating such that an individual is said to acquire the traits of the animal or person she consumes. None of the authors systematically cataloged examples of
magical contagion. Nevertheless, we provide a summary of their examples, but note that
most examples are of backward contagion which does not fit the pathogen model
(Table S1).
1
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.03.003
1090-5138/© 2018 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
sensitivity surrounding offensive items and its associated disgust response are part of a biologically-evolved system designed to reduce
the transmission of pathogens and disease (e.g., Oaten, Stevenson, &
Case, 2009; Curtis, Aunger, & Rabie, 2004; Curtis & Biran, 2001; Tybur,
Lieberman, Kurzban, & DeScioli, 2013). Indeed, contagion is regularly
associated with the emotion of disgust and a defining feature of
disgust-eliciting objects is their contaminating properties (Rozin &
Fallon, 1987). Though systematic cross-cultural data are lacking, studies
with U.S. adults in the late 1980's suggest that contagion is widespread
in Western, educated adults (Rozin, Millman, & Nemeroff, 1986; Rozin,
Nemeroff, Wane, & Sherrod, 1989). Here we examine the presence of
contagion beliefs in two culturally, geographically and economically diverse and remote populations with relatively little experience with
Western biomedicine and formal education: the Hadza huntergatherers of Tanzania and the Tannese subsistence agriculturalists of
Vanuatu.
In the current study, we examined contagion in the domain of pathogen avoidance (e.g., rotten or contaminated food and bodily fluids) and
poison avoidance (e.g., toxic plants and inedible objects). The properties
of contagion are consistent with cues that correlated with pathogen
presence in ancestral environments (Tybur et al., 2013). Disgust may
have evolved to regulate the avoidance of substances harboring pathogens (Rozin, Haidt, & McCauley, 2008; Tybur, Lieberman, & Griskevicius,
2009) and may have been co-opted to regulate behavior in other domains related to reproduction and social transgressions (Rozin, Haidt,
356
C.L. Apicella et al. / Evolution and Human Behavior 39 (2018) 355–363
& McCauley, 1993; Schaich Borg, Lieberman, & Kiehl, 2008; Tybur et al.,
2009).
According to Rozin and Nemeroff (1990), the properties of contagion
are as follows: First, the contaminant must physically contact the target
entity. Second, contagion is dose insensitive; even brief contact with the
contaminated object is sufficient to produce a strong negative response
and this does not increase much with higher doses. Third, rejection of
the contacted target is permanent so long as the person in question remembers the past contact - thus the description of contagion as “once in
contact, always in contact” (Mauss, 1972/1902). Fourth, contagion is
manifested much more generally, and in higher magnitude, if the source
entity is hazardous, hence the frequent use of the word contamination
to describe contagion. Fifth, contagion beliefs can account for both the
transmission of specific attributes from the source to the target
(e.g., “you are what you eat”, Nemeroff & Rozin, 1989), but also a general
negative valence. Finally, the contagion process may best be described
as a transfer of some kind of essence from the source to the target
(Raman & Gelman, 2004).
Because viruses and bacteria tend to be invisible, cognitive (contagion sensitivity) and affective (disgust) processes may have evolved to
prevent individuals from making contact with hazardous items (Tybur
et al., 2013). Specifically, contagion beliefs may be shaped by a specialized learning mechanism designed to modify the disgust response
adaptively depending on local environment and culture (Curtis, de
Barra, & Aunger, 2011). That is, the disgust system is specially designed
to interact with local conditions such that the items that induce disgust
and contaminate will vary between groups in ways that are beneficial
for preventing the spread of disease within those groups. That said,
some disgust cues (e.g. bodily fluids, rotten foods, toxic plants) are expected to be culturally invariant because of their adverse effects in all
environments. The emotion of disgust, with its distinctive facial expression (Brown, 1991; Ekman & Friesen, 1971) and characteristic feelings
of revulsion (Angyal, 1941; Rozin & Fallon, 1987), is likely universal
(e.g. Curtis & Biran, 2001), however, it is currently unknown whether
contagion beliefs involving disgust-eliciting items are also universal.
A number of observations support the hypothesis that the original
function of disgust and contagion was pathogen avoidance (Curtis,
2013; Rozin et al., 1993; Rozin, Haidt, & McCauley, 2016; Tybur et al.,
2013). First, the transmission of infectious disease is a ubiquitous problem and natural selection has produced an array of taxa with various
pathogen-avoidance mechanisms. For instance, mangabey (Cerocebus
albigena) movement patterns respond, in part, to the risk of parasitic infection from contaminating fecal matter of conspecifics (Freeland,
1980). Second, physical contact with an infected entity can, and often
does, transmit pathogens from the source to the target (Rozin et al.,
1986; Rozin & Fallon, 1987). Third, a wide range of data supports the
link between the emotion of disgust and items that spread disease
(Oaten et al., 2009).
Contagion beliefs exhibit design features indicative of adaptations
including reliability, precision, efficiency, complexity, and logic (see
Williams, 1966). Disgust-eliciting items reliably contaminate items
once physical contact is made, regardless of the item it is contaminating,
and does so, with precision. A pen and a spoon would both become contaminated after making contact with fecal matter, but these effects
would not generalize to other uncontacted spoons and pens. Contagion
beliefs also efficiently solve the problem of pathogen exposure since
contagion leads to revulsion and rejection of potentially hazardous,
pathogen-laden items. The fact that exposure to contaminants are largely dose-insensitive, such that brief contact will have contaminating effects, suggests that the system is well-calibrated for avoiding harmful
micro or ultra-microscopic bacteria and viruses. Finally, this constellation of features displays a degree of complexity that makes arguments
that it arose by chance unlikely. It is hard to imagine another specific
and recurring problem that contagion beliefs so fittingly solve.
The strong inference that a feature of human behavior is evolved, in
the absence of an historical record, also depends on assembling a range
of convergent evidence. The most persuasive evidence is presence in
other primates and/or presence at birth. That said, adaptations do not
need to be present at birth, rather, they need to develop reliably and
at a time during development when the trait would be needed
(Cosmides & Tooby, 1997). Pathogen and poison contagion awareness
develops robustly in early and middle childhood in Western industrialized populations (Legare, Wellman, & Gelman, 2009). And while there
are clear developmental trends toward greater awareness and understanding with age (Au & Romo, 1999; Au, Sidle, & Rollins, 1993;
Fallon, Rozin, & Pliner, 1984; Hejmadi, Rozin, & Siegal, 2004; Rozin,
Fallon, & Augustoni-Ziskind, 1985), 3- and 4-year-olds have shown initial contamination understanding in a few studies (Kalish, 1996, 1999;
Siegal & Share, 1990). Other research suggests that a rudimentary
awareness of plant toxicity may be present in infancy (Wertz & Wynn,
2014a, 2014b).
Other questions about the adaptationist account of pathogen and
poison contagion remain unresolved. Many elicitors of disgust are not
actually harmful or contagious. A notable example is moral disgust.2
Again, explanations for other forms of disgust do not preclude
pathogen-avoidance accounts. In fact, it has been argued that disgust
in these other domains was co-opted from its original purpose (pathogen-avoidance) to serve different functions (i.e. Rozin et al., 1993,
2008; Tybur et al., 2013). One even wonders whether magical practices
relying on contagion/contact, were also co-opted from this original
purpose?3 Another problem is that many contagion responses are resistant to acts like sterilization which eliminate the contagion (Nemeroff &
Rozin, 1994; Rozin et al., 1986). Since many of the safety practices that
are used to destroy, remove, or deactivate pathogens (e.g. pressure,
chemicals, radiation) are recent technological inventions in human history, this is not a serious problem for the evolutionary account. Genetic
evolution is a slow process and adaptive lag is anticipated given the
speed at which technology has changed the environment in which
humans operate.
Contagion beliefs about pathogens and poison are shaped by cultural
input and experience (Curtis, Danquah, & Aunger, 2009). Global public
health research has demonstrated that extensive education is often required to increase compliance with sanitary behavior (Biran et al.,
2014; Freeman et al., 2014). Contagion beliefs are heavily influenced
by learning about the presence of pathogens and toxins and how they
are transmitted. The germ theory of disease rose to attention in late
19th century France and is now globally accepted, with numerous
public-health interventions and awareness campaigns happening
worldwide (Biran et al., 2014; Freeman et al., 2014).
Strong evidence to support the idea that pathogen and poison contagion beliefs are part of an evolved species-typical architecture would be
to demonstrate its universality. Testing evolutionary hypotheses that
predict universality is best accomplished by sampling diverse populations isolated from the influence of Western biomedicine and formal
education (Apicella & Barrett, 2016). Whereas there is some evidence
for the presence of a disgust face in slash and burn farmers (i.e., the
Fore of New Guinea) (Ekman, 1992), contagion beliefs about pathogens
have primarily been studied in industrialized populations (e.g., Hejmadi
et al., 2004; Rozin et al., 1986).
The objective of the current studies was to test for pathogen and poison contagion sensitivity in two small-scale societies that differ markedly from both each other and Western populations. We predicted that
across populations, adults would demonstrate reluctance to consume
edible and desirous substances upon contact with disgust-eliciting,
pathogenic, and poisonous objects. We also examined whether contagion beliefs were sensitive to contact with items posing a greater risk
of harm by testing contaminant versus control items. We predicted
2
To the extent that behaviors spread through social networks (i.e., Centola, 2010;
Christakis & Fowler, 2013) immoral behavior may, in fact, be “contagious”.
3
Note that some examples of magical contagion are of backward contagion, which does
not correspond well to the pathogen model.
C.L. Apicella et al. / Evolution and Human Behavior 39 (2018) 355–363
that participants would be less likely to consume an edible substance
after contact with a contaminant (e.g. feces) than a control item (e.g. a
decorative bead). Third, we examined how contamination beliefs
change over development in the Tannese. We predicted that children
(6–11-year-olds) would be more rejecting than older children or adults
of all items that make contact, whether or not a potential pathogen
might be involved. We also predicted that with age, maturation, experience, and socialization, rejection would focus more on contacts with entities that would potentially be contaminants. Reference to germs
would suggest some cultural contamination, given the late arrival of
germ theory in Western cultures.
The tests employed involved multiple suspected disgust-eliciting or
harmful entities, which briefly contacted (fell into but removed) a target, a familiar and liked food modified to be appropriate to the cultural
context. For the Hadza, the target foods were honey and boiled broth
and for participants in Tanna, cooking yams. Images of the physical
“pots” containing the target foods were identical across cultures. Controls are contact of the same liked food(s) with non-disgust-eliciting
or harmless entities that are common in each population. The scenarios
were coordinated so that the situations were as parallel as possible,
based on the knowledge of the relevant author of the paper with one
or the other culture, and with the tasks as similar as possible to standard
contagion tasks used with American participants.
2. Study 1: contagion among the Hadza
The Hadza are one of a few remaining hunter-gatherer populations
left in the world and provide one of the best models available for how
our hunter-gatherer ancestors lived, especially for behaviors related to
diet and food practices (for discussion see Apicella & Crittenden,
2016). They reside in Northern Tanzania in the Great Rift Valley close
to a shallow and seasonal lake (i.e. Eyasi) in mobile camps that number
about 30 individuals. Major foods in the Hadza diet include: meat,
honey, baobab fruit, berries and tubers. Men hunt animals with bow
and arrow, collect honey, and engage in some foraging activities.
Women, on the other hand, exclusively forage, collect water, and firewood. Foraged items include nuts, fruits, and tubers. It is estimated
that fewer than 400 Hadza still practice a hunter-gatherer lifestyle
though about 1000 individuals claim Hadza identity (Marlowe, 2010).
The current research focuses on a subset of Hadza on the Eastern side
of Lake Eyasi, whose subsistence still primarily relies on hunting and
gathering.
While some sex differences in food preferences exist (e.g. men rank
meat higher than berries and for women the ranking is reversed)
(Berbesque & Marlowe, 2009) nearly all Hadza list honey as a favorite
food (Marlowe et al., 2014). Few foods exist that the Hadza actively
avoid (e.g., hyena and snakes). It is perhaps fear that leads them to
avoid snakes since they are considered dangerous (Marlowe, 2010). It
is unknown why the Hadza do not eat hyena. Some maintain that in
the past they would leave dead bodies for hyenas to eat, so it might be
that carrion eating is a basis for rejection hyena meat. The Hadza also
do not eat termites even though they are plentiful and when asked
why, some claim it is because they bite (Marlowe, 2010). Few foods
are considered taboo. “Epeme” meat, which includes meat that derives from organs including the kidneys, lungs, and heart of an animal, is forbidden to be consumed by females, boys, and “nonepeme” men. Offenders are thought to be in danger of sickness or
death. To become an “epeme man”, one either has to reach full adulthood or kill a large animal. The Hadza occasionally attribute negative
outcomes (e.g. teeth falling out) to epeme meat violations (Marlowe,
2010). Adult men do not eat tortoises claiming that it will render the
poison on their arrows (panjube) ineffective (Marlowe, 2010). We
know of no other food taboos. Generally, the Hadza have few supernatural beliefs (Apicella, 2017) so these beliefs concerning food are
particularly striking.
357
2.1. Method
2.1.1. Participants
Data collection took place over two consecutive years (2013, 2014)
by a Tanzanian research assistant. In 2013, data collection was directly
supervised by one of the authors (C.L.A.) and in 2014, it was supervised
by a PhD student at the University of Cambridge. In 2013, 51 Hadza participants, ages 20 to 71 (M = 40.06, SD = 12.05) from 7 different camps
around the Eastern side of Lake Eyasi were recruited to participate. Fiftyseven percent (N = 29) of the participants were men. During the summer of 2014, 66 individuals from 4 different Hadza camps were visited
and all adults in each camp were recruited to participate. Adults ranged
from 18 to 75 years old (M = 39.52, SD = 14.66). Thirty-two (48.5%) of
the participants were men. All interviews took place in private and were
conducted in Swahili. While Hadzane is the native language spoken by
the Hadza, all participants understood basic Swahili and storyboards
were used to facilitate comprehension.
2.1.2. Procedures
In 2013, participants were first asked to name a food that they find
disgusting and will not eat: “Tafadhali taja chakula (vyakula) ambacho
(ambavyo) kinakuletea (vinakuletea) kinyaa na huwezi kuvila”. Please
note that “kinyaa” is a good cognate for disgust and refers not only to
something that is disgusting/repugnant, but can also be used to refer
to filth and excrement. Participants were also explicitly asked whether
they like to eat hyena meat and to provide a reason for their response.
Following questioning, participants were shown six different storyboards/scenarios where three different items (i.e. a piece of hyena, a
man coughing and poison4) made contact with either 1) a pot of
honey or 2) a pot of boiling meat broth (see Supplementary materials
for storyboards) but were subsequently removed. Both honey and
broth are staples of the Hadza diet in both children and adults. In fact,
both are commonly used to wean children off breast milk. The storyboards contained simple illustrations, accompanied by brief and plausible scenarios of how the contaminant got into the pot. Participants were
asked whether they would eat the honey (or broth) and to provide a
justification for their answer.
In 2014, two storyboards were shown to the Hadza where a bead
from a woman's headband – a typical decorative band worn around
the forehead – fell into 1) a pot of honey or 2) a pot of boiling broth.
The wording was paralleled the initial study in 2013 (see Supplementary materials).
Fourteen of the participants who were questioned in 2014 also participated in 2013. The overlapping participants' ages range from 28 to
63 years (M = 40.36, SD = 10.7) and six (42.9%) are men. While the
bead data were collected in a separate year, we still compare rates of endorsement of eating the contaminated food (i.e. honey and broth) by
contaminant type (i.e. hyena, cough, poison, and bead). We also do
this separately for our Hadza informants who participated both years.
2.1.3. Coding
Local research assistants translated participants' responses from
Swahili to English. Research assistants in the U.S. coded participants' responses into nine categories using these translations. The sickness/disease code was used for any response that indicated that consuming
the target would result in illness. The germs code was used for any response that indicated that the foreign object would introduce germs
into the target items. The presence of item code was used anytime a participant simply stated that the presence of the foreign object was the
reason for eating or not eating the target items. The dirty code was
used if a participant stated that the foreign object made the target
4
Panjube is a poison that Hadza manufacture by pounding the branches and seeds of
certain plants (e.g., Adenium obesum) to use on the tips of their arrows. It is stored as a
dried up ball of dough covered in ash, until applied to arrowheads. It causes cardiac arrest
in animals when it enters their bloodstream (Bartram, 1997).
358
C.L. Apicella et al. / Evolution and Human Behavior 39 (2018) 355–363
unclean or dirty. The poison code was used if a participant said the foreign object would make the target poisonous to eat. The choke code
was used if the participant stated that you could choke on the foreign
object. The change in smell/taste code was used anytime a participant
said that the addition of the foreign object would alter the targets' flavor
or smell. If a participant stated that it was fine to eat the target the ok to
eat code was used. Finally, if a participant said they were unsure if they
would eat the target or did not know, the unsure code was used.
2.2. Hadza results
When asked to name a disgusting food that they will not eat, snake
and hyena were the most commonly reported items, named by 37% and
57% of participants, respectively. Baboon and honey badger were each
named by one respondent. One respondent reported that there were
no foods that they found disgusting. When explicitly asked if they like
to eat Hyena, all but two of the 51 respondents said no. The two most
common reasons provided for not eating hyena were 1) that it is not
meat (25%) and 2) hyenas eat humans (29%).
Table 1 reports the proportion of Hadza participants that endorsed
being okay with eating the honey and broth by item type (i.e. hyena,
cough, poison and bead). Rejection rates were high. Since, presumably
all subjects would eat the uncontaminated broth or honey, even just a
few rejections would be notable. For the three contaminants; the percent of Hadza who endorsed being okay with eating the food varied between 4 and 10% for honey and 6 and 12% for broth. These are all
massively below the expected rate of 100% for uncontaminated food.
In fact, binomial proportions tests reveal that individuals endorsed eating both honey and broth after contaminated with hyena, a cough and
poison significantly b0.50 (all p b 0.000) suggesting that the Hadza
were not just randomly answering the question. Moreover, we can say
with 95% certainty that no N16% of the Hadza would eat the food
items contaminated with hyena, no N24% would eat the foods contaminated by a cough and no N13% of Hadza would eat the foods contaminated with poison. Acceptance rates for the honey and broth after
making contact with a bead are also far below 100%, but much higher
than for the three contaminants. For both our overlapping and nonoverlapping participants, the point estimates for the percentage of participants who would accept the food items, range between 36 and 44%.
Table 1 reports the data separately for the subjects who did and did not
also participate in 2013. That said, chi-square proportion tests of the
number of participants who would eat the honey (χ 2(1) = 0.005, p
= 0.95) and broth (χ2(1) = 0.07, p = 0.78) after a bead fell into it, do
not differ between the two groups.
Table 1
Proportion of Hadza participants that endorsed being okay with eating the honey and
broth by contaminate type (i.e. hyena, cough, and poison) and control (i.e. bead). Upper
and lower exact 95% binomial confidence bounds are reported. For the bead condition
our values are disaggregated by whether or not they also participated in 2013.
Honey
Hyena (2013)
Cough (2013)
Poison (2013)
Bead non-overlapping participants (2014)
Bead overlapping participants (2014)
Broth
Hyena (2013)
Cough (2013)
Poison (2013)
Bead non-overlapping participants (2014)
Bead overlapping participants (2014)
Percentage
Lower
confidence
bound
Upper
confidence
bound
0.10
0.05
0.04
0.44
0.43
0.03
0.01
0
0.30
0.18
0.21
0.16
0.13
0.58
0.71
0.06
0.11
0.00
0.40
0.35
0.01
0.04
0
0.27
0.13
0.16
0.24
0.06
0.55
0.65
Note: For the three contaminants (N = 51); for bead non-overlapping participants (N =
52); for bead overlapping participants (N = 14).
To test whether there is a contamination response for hyena, cough,
and poison versus the bead, we run six separate Fisher's Exact tests
excluding those subjects in the control group who also participated in
the study in 2013, so that there is independence between the groups.
Note that the Fisher Exact test is similar to Pearson's Chi-Square but
preferred when cell values are small and/or zero, as is the case here.
All six tests showed an association between item type and response
such that each of the contaminants (i.e. hyena, cough and poison)
elicited greater rates of rejection of the broth and honey compared to
the bead (all p b 0.001).
Whereas the Hadza were much more likely to endorse eating foods
after contamination with the bead than after contamination with hyena,
coughing or poison, the differences could theoretically be due to differences in the participants sampled during the two years rather than differences in the item types. That said, we think this implausible for three
reasons. First, Hadza in 2013 and 2014 were collected from the same
geographic region of Hadzaland and no differences in age (t = −0.30
(87), p = 0.771) or frequency of gender (χ2 (1, N = 89) = 1.292 p =
0.256) between the two samples was found after excluding participants
for whom we have data from both years. Second, when examining the
point estimates for our small sample of overlapping participants from
both years (n = 14), the proportion of participants endorsing eating
foods after being contaminated by hyena (honey = 7%, broth = 7%), a
cough (honey = 0%, broth = 29%) and poison (honey = 7%, broth =
0%) was much smaller than when the bead was the contaminant
(honey = 43%, broth = 36%). Finally, even despite our very small sample of overlapping participants from both years (n = 14), we were still
able to detect differences in choices among these subjects using
McNemar's related samples tests in the cough vs. bead for honey condition (p = 0.031). We also reach significance at the 10% level for poison
vs. bead for honey (p = 0.062) and broth (p = 0.062).
We also use Cochran's Q test for related samples to compare whether the distribution of responses differs between hyena, cough and poison in both the honey and broth condition. The distributions of
responses did not vary when the contaminant was hyena, cough, or poison (p = 0.417) in the honey. However, the distribution of yes/no responses varied between the three contaminates in the broth condition
(p = 0.034). Multiple pairwise comparisons using McNemar's test suggest that individuals were more likely to endorse eating the broth after
being her contaminated by a cough than by poison (p = 0.031). No differences were found between hyena and poison (p = 0.250) and hyena
and cough (p = 0.453).
Participants provided a number of reasons for not eating the
honey and broth after being contaminated by the different items (see
Table 2).
Restatements about the presence of the item were common responses. In fact, restatements were most common when hyena contaminated the honey (75%) and broth (75%). The lethal qualities of poison
and pathogen transmission of the cough lead to health risks so it is possible that other explanations come more readily. When a person
coughed over the honey, the most common reason reported for not eating the honey was fear of getting sick (94%). Some participants even
mentioned the possibility of contracting tuberculosis. For the broth condition, sickness was also the most common reason cited for not eating
the broth after a cough (63%) though the number of participants citing
this as a concern decreased and the number of participants who restated
the presence of someone coughing in the broth increased (29%). Interestingly, four of the individuals who said that they would eat the
broth after a sick Hadza coughed over it claimed that boiling action
would kill the germs/disease. When the contaminant was poison, the
vast majority of participants claimed that they would not eat the
honey (94%) or broth (98%) because it was poisonous. Finally, the
most common reason for not eating the honey after being contaminated
with a bead was dirtiness (41%), but the most common response was
that it was okay to eat the honey after contacting the bead (42%). For
the bead contacting the broth, most participants stated they would
C.L. Apicella et al. / Evolution and Human Behavior 39 (2018) 355–363
359
Table 2
Proportion of participants providing each response type to open-ended questions about decision to eat or not eat the honey or broth by contaminate.
Honey
Sickness/disease
Germs
Presence of item
Dirty
Poison
Choke
Change in smell/taste
Ok to eat
Unsure
Broth
Hyena
(n = 51)
Cough
(n = 51)
Poison
(n = 51)
Bead
(n = 66)
Hyena
(n = 51)
Cough
(n = 51)
Poison
(n = 51)
Bead
(n = 66)
0.08
0
0.75
0
0
0
0.16
0.04
0
0.94
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
0.02
0
0
0.94
0
0.14
0.02
0
0.03
0.02
0.02
0.41
0
0
0
0.42
0
0.02
0
0.75
0
0.04
0
0.12
0
0
0.63
0
0.29
0
0
0
0.02
0
0
0
0
0
0
0.98
0
0.10
0
0
0
0
0.52
0.09
0
0
0
0.39
0
not consume it by restating the presence of the bead in the broth (52%)
while many also stated that it was okay to eat the broth (39%). Thus the
reason pattern was different for each contaminant, with poison dominating for poison, sickness for cough, restatement for hyena, and dirtiness or restatement for bead.
3. Study 2: contagion among the Tannese of Vanuatu
Vanuatu is a Melanesian island nation in the South Pacific and one of
the most remote, culturally and linguistically diverse countries in the
world (Norton, 1993). Our participants are from the island of Tanna –
one of the larger islands located in the Tafea province of Vanuatu, with
approximately 30,000 inhabitants – and predominately speak Bislama
and English (as well as a large number of indigenous languages).
Tanna provides a uniquely informative context for studying contagion
because it is a population that has relatively recently begun to attend
formal schools and still relies on the natural world for survival through
subsistence agriculture, foraging, and fishing (Peck & Gregory, 2005;
Watson-Jones, Busch, & Legare, 2015).
Despite the influence of Presbyterianism on the island (see Supplementary materials), many villages have maintained kastom (custom),
or “ancestrally enjoined rules for life” (Keesing, 1982, p. 360). In a recent
survey of national identity in Vanuatu, maintaining kastom, as well as
being Christian were considered two of the most important aspects of
what it means to be from Vanuatu (Clarke, Leach, & Scambary, 2013).
Formal education is also a relatively recent institution on Tanna. Missionaries set up rudimentary schools in the early 1900s (Gregory &
Gregory, 2002). There was, however, no standard curriculum until the
last three decades when British and French run schools began providing
primary and secondary education (Peck & Gregory, 2005). Additionally,
parents in Tanna often have to pay to send their children to school, and
children from kastom villages have only recently begun to attend. Most
children spend time in similar-aged peer groups or helping their parents with the family gardens and/or tending domesticated animals. On
Tanna, people live primarily from subsistence agriculture (Cox et al.,
2007), consume domesticated animals (e.g., pigs and chickens) and
some hunting of bats and fishing. Thus, children in Tanna live “close”
to nature (Unsworth et al., 2012; Watson-Jones, Busch, Harris, &
Legare, 2017). For example, they are likely to be observers of, or participants in, subsistence agriculture, coastal fishing practices, pig and
chicken husbandry, and the hunting of flying foxes (Busch, WatsonJones, & Legare, 2018).
3.1. Method
3.1.1. Participants
Data was collected with n = 20 children (6–11-year-olds), n = 21
adolescents (13–17-year-olds), n = 22 adults. All participants were recruited and participated in the study in and around the village of
Lenakel on the island of Tanna, Vanuatu.
3.1.2. Procedure
After providing informed consent, participants were presented with
eight storyboards describing scenarios where a foreign object comes
into contact with a pot of cooking yams. The study procedure was conducted in the national language, Bislama, and all participants completed
the study individually. Half of the scenarios described control items
contacting the yams, and half described contaminant items contacting
the yams. For all eight scenarios the researcher read a short description
to the participant: “Imagine there is a pot of yams for you to eat. Now
imagine [a contaminant/control item is introduced] to the pot of yams.
[The contaminant/control item is removed] from the pot of yams and
the food continues to cook.” In addition to hearing this scenario, the researcher presented participants with a picture board, which depicted
simple illustrations of the foreign object entering the yams (see Supplementary material for full stimuli and slight variations). Participants
were asked two follow up questions for each of the eight scenarios,
1) “would you still eat the yams, yes or no?” and 2) “why or why not.”
The four true contaminants included blood, cough, chicken feces, and
a fly. The four control items included ocean water, a stone, a bead, and
a leaf.
3.1.3. Coding
Local research assistants translated participants' responses from
Bislama to English. Research assistants in the U.S. coded participants' responses into nine categories using these translations. The nine coding
categories were the same as those used on the Hadza data.
3.2. Vanuatu results
First, we present data on participants' binary yes/no responses to
whether the yams could still be consumed after the removal of the contaminant. Then we present data on participants' responses to the openended follow-up question about why the yams could or could not be
consumed.
Rejection of a potentially contaminated food was scored as 1, while
acceptance was scored as 0. The data show there was no difference in
endorsement of eating/not eating the yams after any of the contaminate
items (feces, blood, fly, or cough) had fallen into the food (p = 0.166,
related-samples Cochran's q test). There was also no difference in endorsement of eating/not eating the yams after any of the control items
(leaf, stone, bead, ocean water) had fallen into the food (p = 0.130,
related-samples Cochran's q test, see Table 3 for means and standard
deviations for each of the items). Therefore, summary scores were created for the contaminated items (0–4) and the control items (0–4). A
repeated–measures ANOVA with age group (6–12-year-olds; 13–17year-olds; adults) as the between-subjects factor and responses to the
items that fell into the food (contaminated, control) as the withinsubjects factor revealed no significant effect of age, F(2, 60) = 2.40, p
= 0.099, n2p = 0.074. There was a main effect of item type (contaminated or control), F(1, 63) = 36.11, p b 0.0001, n2p = 0.367. Participants
360
C.L. Apicella et al. / Evolution and Human Behavior 39 (2018) 355–363
olds, adults) as the between-subjects factor and item type (contaminants, controls) as the within-subjects factor. Since these studies examine contamination beliefs, we only analyzed responses relevant to
contamination (i.e., disease, germs, dirty, and okay to eat).
For responses appealing to sickness and disease as the reason for not
eating the yams, a repeated measures ANOVA with age group (6–12year-olds, 13–17-year-olds, adults) as the between-subjects factor and
item type (contaminate, control) as the within-subjects factor revealed
a main effect of item type, F(1, 60) = 72.94, p = 0.0001, n2p = 0.549. Participants referenced sickness and disease in their explanations more for
the contaminated items (M = 1.30, SD = 1.24) than for the control
items (M = 0.16, SD = 0.41). There was also a main effect of age
group, F(2, 60) = 8.78, p = 0.0001, n2p = 0.226. Bonferroni corrected
post hoc tests revealed that adolescents (M = 2.33, SD = 1.56) provided
more explanations appealing to sickness and disease than did younger
children (M = 0.65, SD = 0.75), p = 0.0001, and marginally more
than adults, (M = 1.36, SD = 1.40), p = 0.051. The analysis also revealed an interaction between item type and age group, F(2, 60) =
6.82, p = 0.002, n2p = 0.185. Bonferroni corrected pairwise comparisons
revealed that, for the contaminated items, adolescents (M = 2.05, SD =
1.36) provided more explanations appealing to sickness and disease
than did younger children (M = 0.60, SD = 0.75), p = 0.0001, and marginally more than adults (M = 1.23, SD = 1.11), p = 0.055. For the control items, there was no difference between the three age groups in
providing a response referencing sickness and disease.
Statements appealing to germs as the reason for not eating the yams
following an item falling into it were summed to create scores for the
contaminate items (0–4) and the control items (0–4). A repeated measures ANOVA with age group (6–12-year-olds, 13–17-year-olds, adults)
as the between-subjects factor and item type (contaminate, control) as
the within-subjects factor revealed a main effect of item type, F(1, 60)
= 48.34, p b 0.0001, n2p = 0.446. Participants referenced germs as the
reason for not eating the yams for the contaminated items (M = 0.49,
SD = 0.59) more than for the control items (M = 0.02, SD = 0.13).
There was no main effect of age group, F(2, 60) = 2.36, p = 0.103, n2p
= 0.073.
Responses indicating that the item would make the yams dirty were
summed to create a score for the contaminate items (0–4) and the control items (0–4). A repeated measures ANOVA with age group (6–12year-olds, 13–17-year-olds, adults) as the between-subjects factor and
item type (contaminate, control) as the within-subjects factor revealed
Table 3
Proportion of participants that endorsed eating the yams by age group and item.
Age groups
13–17-year-olds
Adults
Collapsed
Contaminates
Cough
0.05 (0.22)
Feces
0
Fly
0
Blood
0.05 (0.22)
6–12-year-olds
0.05 (0.22)
0
0.05 (0.22)
0.05 (0.22)
0.09 (0.30)
0
0.05 (0.21)
0.09 (0.30)
0.06 (0.24)
0
0.03 (0.17)
0.06 (0.24)
Controls
Leaf
Bead
Ocean
Stone
0.33 (0.48)
0.24 (0.44)
0.38 (0.50)
0.10 (0.30)
0.36 (0.49)
0.23 (0.43)
0.45 (0.51)
0.32 (0.48)
0.28 (0.45)
0.20 (0.40)
0.31 (0.47)
0.18 (0.39)
0.10 (0.31)
0.15 (0.37)
0.10 (0.31)
0.15 (0.37)
Note. N = 63 (20 6–11-year-olds, 21 13–18-year-olds, and 22 adults). (Standard
deviation.)
were much less likely to endorse eating the contaminated items (M =
0.16, SD = 0.45) than the control items (M = 0.98, SD = 1.24).
Participants' responses to the open-ended, “why,” questions were
coded into the same nine categories as were used with the Hadza (see
Supplementary material). Both explanations for the endorsement of
eating and the endorsement of not eating the yams following the
items falling in the pot were used in the following analyses. The most
common responses across both item types (contaminates and controls)
were responses indicating a reluctance to eat the yams due to the presence of the item in the yams (81% of participants provided this type of
response at least once). Next, most common were responses appealing
to sickness and disease (75% of participants), followed by responses indicating that the item would change the smell or taste of the of (68%),
followed by responses that the item would make the food dirty (65%),
followed by responses indicating that it is okay to eat the item (49%),
and lastly followed by responses referring to germs (46%), see Table 4
for means and standard deviations for each type of response by age
group and item type.
We examined whether there was any difference in the frequency of
explanation types by item type (contaminant vs. control) and age group
(child, adolescent, adult). Summary scores of participants' responses indicating they would not eat the yams were created for the contaminate
items (0–4) and the control items (0–4). Then a repeated measures
ANOVA was conducted with age group (6–12-year-olds, 13–17-year-
Table 4
Mean number of response types to open-ended questions about decision to eat or not.
AGE GROUPS
6–12-year-olds
13–17-year-olds
Adults
Collapsed
Contaminates
Sickness/disease
Germs
Presence of item
Dirty
Poison
Choke
Change in smell/taste
Ok to eat
Unsure
0.60 (0.75)
0.65 (0.49)
2.4 (1.05)
0.40 (0.82)
0.05 (0.22)
0
0.05 (0.22)
0.10 (0.45)
0.10 (0.31)
2.05 (1.36)
0.57 (0.68)
0.52 (0.68)
0.81 (0.98)
0
0
0
0.14 (0.36)
0.05 (0.22)
1.23 (1.11)
0.27 (0.55)
2.05 (1.24)
0.41 (0.59)
0
0
0.09 (0.29)
0.18 (0.39)
0.09 (0.29)
1.30 (1.24)
0.49 (0.59)
1.62 (1.30)
0.54 (0.82)
0.02 (0.13)
0
0.05 (0.21)
0.14 (0.40)
0.08 (0.27)
Controls
Sickness/disease
Germs
Presence of item
Dirty
Poison
Choke
Change in smell/taste
Ok to eat
Unsure
0.05 (0.22)
0
2.1 (1.3)
0.40 (0.82)
0.10 (0.31)
0.15 (0.37)
0.65 (0.49)
0.35 (0.67)
0.20 (0.41)
0.29 (0.46)
0.05 (0.22)
0.38 (0.97)
0.95 (0.86)
0.52 (0.60)
0.19 (0.40)
0.76 (0.44)
0.86 (0.96)
0.48 (0.75)
0.14 (0.47)
0
1.04 (1.36)
0.91 (0.97)
0.36 (0.58)
0.23 (0.43)
0.64 (0.49)
1.05 (1.21)
0.18 (0.66)
0.16 (0.41)
0.02 (0.13)
1.16 (1.39)
0.76 (0.91)
0.33 (0.54)
0.19 (0.40)
0.68 (0.47)
0.76 (1.01)
0.29 (0.63)
Note. N = 63 (20 6–12-year-olds, 21 13–17-year-olds, and 22 adults). (Standard deviations.)
C.L. Apicella et al. / Evolution and Human Behavior 39 (2018) 355–363
no main effect of item type, F(1, 60) = 2.47, p = 0.121, n2p = 0.040 or of
age group, F(2, 60) = 2.74, p = 0.072, n2p = 0.084.
Participants reports of the yams being okay to eat if the item was
taken out were summed to create a score for the contaminate items
(0–4) and the control items (0–4). A repeated measures ANOVA
with age group (6–12-year-olds, 13–17-year-olds, adults) as the
between-subjects factor and item type (contaminate, control) as
the within-subjects factor revealed a main effect of item type, F(1,
60) = 28.82, p = 0.0001, n2p = 0.324. Participants provided more
“okay to eat if the item is taken out” responses for the control items
(M = 0.76, SD = 1.01) than for the contaminate items (M = 0.14,
SD = 0.40). There was no effect of age group, F(2, 60) = 2.29, p =
0.110, n2p = 0.071.
4. Discussion
The present study provides new insight into our understanding of
the origins of contagion sensitivity by testing for its presence in two
populations with markedly divergent social, cultural, and economic
ways of life from each other and from Western societies. We find evidence that contagion sensitivity is present in both Hadza huntergatherers and Tannese subsistence-agriculturalists. These findings,
coupled with other work showing that the emotion of disgust is widespread and probably universal, are consistent with the hypothesis that
disgust and contagion are part of a evolved system designed to prevent
exposure to pathogens and disease. While our participants had little to
no formal Western education and remain relatively isolated from Western influences, we nevertheless cannot rule out that contagion sensitivity is learned. Likewise, we also stress that our results do not imply that
cultural differences – especially regarding the items deemed disgusting
– do not exist. Instead, our data support the proposal that evolution has
shaped contagion sensitivity to be flexible, allowing it to interact with
local ecological and cultural contexts. Such a system allows for individuals to learn through direct experience and from each other about
which items should be avoided.
We see large differences in both populations in rates of rejection between contaminants and control items. For the Hadza, the mean rejection rate was 94% for the three contaminants in both honey and
boiling broth, compared to the control item (a bead), where rejections
averaged 68%. Because we used both honey and broth, we can rule out
the possibility that food type is driving the results. The results from
the Tannese correspond well to those from the Hadza; there was a
mean rejection rate of 94% across the four contaminants, compared to
66.5% for the four controls.
Notably, there were high rejection rates for the control items in
both populations. For the Hadza, a single plastic bead, used in decorative headwear, led to food rejections more than half the time. Because the bead was in contact with the body of a person before
making contact with the food, it may have become viewed as a potentially harmful contaminant. Indeed, the most common response
for not eating the honey after a bead fell into it, was for sanitary reasons (e.g. dirty). Among the Tannese, the bead also shows a higher
rejection rate than the other three control entities further suggesting
that it may have been viewed as harmful to participants in both societies. It is also possible that a bead in any of the foods (honey, broth,
yams) could be considered “matter out of place” (Douglas, 1966) or a
choking hazard, and hence produced a rejection. In prior work on
contamination, a modest percentage of Western adult respondents
show rejection of favored beverages after brief contact with harmless, control, entities, such as birthday cake candles (Rozin et al.,
1986; see also Rozin, Grant, Weinberg, & Parker, 2007). More directly
related to the present work, 32% of both Indian and American 8-yearolds rejected a favored juice that was briefly contacted by a piece of
spinach (Hejmadi et al., 2004).
All four of the Vanuatu contaminants are potentially infectious, so
we cannot make the infectious versus non-infectious “bad” entity
361
distinction that was possible with the Hadza. For the case of the
Hadza, only one of the three contaminants (i.e., cough) was actually
contagious, although there is a possible infectious component to
hyena meat. Low levels of the arrow poison may be harmful if ingested.
The poison is a on the Hadza arrows is made from a flowering plant in
the genus Adenium, which contains a cardiotoxic glycoside. While ingestion of plants containing these poisons can cause gastrointestinal upset
and vomiting, for death to occur upon ingestion, very large amounts
would need to be consumed. The plant is also extremely bitter and the
Hadza avoid eating meat around the opening where arrows have
pierced the animal, possibly because of the taste. Indeed, some Hadza
cited the poison's bitter taste when asked why they would not eat the
honey or broth. Contagion sensitivity to the arrow poison cannot be justified in terms of the multiplication of very small doses of microorganisms in the body. However, Westerners often treat toxins as if
they are micro-organisms.
Hyena meat effectively transmitted its disgust-eliciting properties to
the foods: rejection rates were over 90%. When asked why, the most
common response was a restatement of the situation, indicating
possible dumbfounding. Here we use dumbfounding to mean the inability of participants to provide articulable reasons for rejecting the food
items so that they instead rely on restatements. Similar responses
were also observed in Westerners with cockroach contamination
(Haidt, Bjorklund, & Murphy, 2000; Rozin et al., 1986, 1989). Rejection
of the hyena could be due to mistaken beliefs about microbial contamination, but in prior work (Rozin et al., 1986), sterilization of an animal
carcass (i.e. roach) has only a very modest effect on rejection. It may
be that the disgust responses entail activation of contamination
responses, even in cases where there is not (as for the case of many
disgust-eliciting entities) an actual microbial threat. Why do the
Hadza consider eating hyena disgusting – an aversion shared with
San hunter-gatherers living thousands of miles away (Tanaka,
1996)? It is possible that hyena is, itself, tainted by the laws of contagion due to its contact with rotting human and animal corpses. Thus
rejection of the honey and broth is merely a second order effect: rotting carcasses contaminate hyenas, which in turn, contaminate other
items.
It is striking that the “cough” rejection level is not higher than the
virtually harmless hyena meat. It is also notable that rejection only
slightly decreased when the cough went into boiling broth (which
would kill micro-organisms), as opposed to the honey. These findings,
coupled with very low levels of formal schooling in the Hadza, as well
as high rejection levels of the bead, argue against the idea that contagion
sensitivity arises from learning about germ theory. However, it is important to note that a few Hadza explicitly mentioned germs and indeed,
they are not completely isolated and some do attend school. Future
work is needed to explicitly test knowledge of germ theory in the
Hadza and Tannese.
For the Tannese, all the contaminants have clear contagious potential (fly, feces, cough from sick person, and blood), but in all cases the
contaminant is added to cooking yams, which continue to cook and
should thereby be sterilized by heat. Based on the Tannese responses
to the open-ended follow-up questions, it is clear they believe these
contaminants may cause disease. It is unknown, however, whether
they understand the process of sterilization by heat. Germs are infrequently mentioned as a cause of rejection, and the most common reason
was the “presence of the contaminant” in the cooking yams. As with the
Hadza, mere presence (essentially a description of the situation) is the
most common reason for rejection of contaminants, again indicating a
sort of dumbfounded response, as observed with Westerners (Rozin
et al., 1986, 1989). It is also possible that contagion sensitivity evolved
to cause rejection of potentially contaminating entities, without any
knowledge or influence of the role of heat sterilization, a rare event in
nature. Contagion responses are surprisingly resistant to explicit heat
sterilization manipulations in Americans (Nemeroff & Rozin, 1994;
Rozin et al., 1986).
362
C.L. Apicella et al. / Evolution and Human Behavior 39 (2018) 355–363
There is clear evidence for a contamination response in 6–12 year
olds from Tanna, as there is in Western and Hindu-Indian children
(Hejmadi et al., 2004). The data from Tanna also suggest that children
may be more likely to reject food contaminated by the non-contagious
control items than adults though this result did not reach significance.
This raises the possibility of the role of cultural pedagogy in the development of children's disgust response (Liberman, Woodward, Sullivan, &
Kinzler, 2016). Children may initially develop a broad and inexact conception of contagion due to an error-management strategy (avoid all
foods that have any contaminant in case that contaminant is physically
harmful or contagious (see error management theory, Haselton & Buss,
2000). This inexact conception of contagion might then be refined over
ontogeny, through cultural input, to specific, truly contagious entities.
There is some evidence that the avoidance of contaminants as a method
to avoid disease and sickness becomes more potent with age in Tanna,
although the adolescents show the strongest motivations to avoid contagious contaminants for this reason. This may result from the fact that
the adolescents are much more likely to have received formal schooling
than the adults, and have learned about disease transmission and this
information would be particularly salient if learned recently. As far as
we know, there is no comparable evidence comparing adolescents
with adults for any other population, but in most cultures, the adults
have also received some formal schooling.
The current study has some potential limitations. First, we use hypothetical questions which could lead to demand characteristics. However, we limited this possibility by 1) keeping our research assistants
blind to the hypotheses, 2) using scripted questions and 3) utilizing storyboards which drew visual attention away from the research assistant.
Because the participants offered explanations for refusing the items that
were not part of the questioning, we do not think demand characteristics to be a major problem here. A second concern is that in the Hadza,
our control item elicited a contagion response though rates of rejection
were much lower than the three contaminants. Nonetheless, we think
the finding demonstrates the strength of contagion sensitivity. It
would have been useful to have control conditions where contaminants
are proximally close, but not touching, the foods. Contact is critical in
most studies of contagion from developed cultures, but there may be a
residual effect from proximity.
Overall, the results are strikingly similar to the results from the U.S.
and India (Hejmadi et al., 2004; Rozin et al., 1986, 1989). Individuals
in both societies show a strong rejection response to desirable food entities that have contacted any inappropriate entity. Consistent with the
developmental data from India and the U.S. (Hejmadi et al., 2004;
Legare et al., 2009), the basic contagion response is present by age six
(or younger). These results provide some of the strongest support for
the position that contagion sensitivity is a universal feature of human
cognition. The results also provide evidence that, together with the
emotion of disgust, contagion is part of a system that evolved to prevent
exposure to dangerous and pathogenic items. An important design feature of this system is that it allows for environmental and social input
such that individuals can learn from their own direct experience, and
from others, about items that they should and should not eat. This
type of system would allow humans to effectively deal with novelty
and develop preferences and aversions to those items they are likely
to encounter in their environment, as opposed to evolving countless
aversions to the many harmful items found in the world. Thus, we
argue that contagion sensitivity and the emotion of disgust are panhuman but that experience and culture will influence which items are
favored and which are rejected.
The nature and origin of disgust and contagion continue to be issues
of concern in psychology. The evidence presented in this paper suggests
that contagion sensitivity is likely universal, appearing in childhood,
which is consistent with a biological evolutionary account. Nevertheless, questions remain. One is whether the contagious (presumably
pathogen linked) beliefs extend to the spread of immorality, as for the
case of moral disgust in other societies. Another has to do with how
and when non-contagious entities come to be treated as contagious –
a question that has not been addressed in the general contagion literature. A third has to do with understanding of the fundamental difference
between contagious and non-contagious illnesses. As globalization proceeds, it is becoming increasingly difficult to obtain data from the few
remaining populations that have not been heavily exposed to formal education. We urge further exploration of contagion-sensitivity and disgust in small-scale populations.
Acknowledgements
CA would like to thank Duncan Stibbard Hawkes, Deus Haraja,
Ibrahim Mabulla and Charles Endeku for their research assistance in
the field and Audax Mabulla for his continued support. CL would like
to thank the Tafea Cultural Center in Tanna, Chief Peter Marshall,
Chief Kaimua, Chief Yappa, George, Jimmy Takaronga, Teana Tufunga,
Jean-Pascal, Janet, Anna, Bev, and Aurora Brinkman. CA also thanks
Daudi Peterson and Stephanie Schnorr for helpful advice. CA was supported by funds from the University of Pennsylvania. CL, JB, and RW
were supported by funds from the John Templeton Foundation [grant
number 40102] and an ESRC (Economic and Social Research Council)
Large Grant [REF RES-060-25-0085].
Contributions
CA, CL, and PR designed the study. JB, RW, CA and CL collected and
analyzed data. CA, CL and PR wrote the manuscript. All authors provided
extensive feedback on each draft.
Appendix A. Supplementary data
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.03.003.
References
Angyal, A. (1941). Disgust and related aversions. The Journal of Abnormal and Social
Psychology, 36(3), 393–412.
Apicella, C. L. (2017). High levels of rule-bending in a minimally religious and largely egalitarian forager population. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 1–16.
Apicella, C. L., & Barrett, H. C. (2016). Cross-cultural evolutionary psychology. Current
Opinion in Psychology, 7, 92–97.
Apicella, C. L., & Crittenden, A. N. (2016). Hunter-gatherer families and parenting. In D. M.
Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Au, T. K., & Romo, L. F. (1999). Mechanical causality in children's “folkbiology”. In D. L.
Medin, & S. Atran (Eds.), Folkbiology. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Au, T. K., Sidle, A. L., & Rollins, K. B. (1993). Developing an intuitive understanding of conservation and contamination: Invisible particles as a plausible mechanism.
Developmental Psychology, 29(2), 286–299.
Bartram, L. E. (1997). A comparison of Kua (Botswana) and Hadza (Tanzania) bow and
arrow hunting. In H. Knecht (Ed.), Projectile technology (pp. 321–343). Boston:
Springer.
Berbesque, J. C., & Marlowe, F. W. (2009). Sex differences in food preferences of Hadza
hunter-gatherers. Evolutionary Psychology, 7(4), 601–616.
Biran, A., Schmidt, W., Varadharajan, K. S., Rajaraman, D., Kumar, R., Greenland, K., ...
Curtis, V. (2014). Effect of a behavior-change intervention on handwashing with
soap in India (SuperAmma): A cluster-randomized trial. The Lancet Global Health,
2(3), 145–154.
Brown, D. E. (1991). Human universals. New York: McGraw-Hill, 118.
Busch, J. T. A., Watson-Jones, R. E., & Legare, C. H. (2018). Cross-cultural variation in the development of folk ecological reasoning. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39(3), 310–319.
Centola, D. (2010). The spread of behavior in an online social network experiment.
Science, 329(59996), 1194–1197.
Christakis, N. A., & Fowler, J. H. (2013). Social contagion theory: Examining dynamic social
networks and human behavior. Statistics in Medicine, 32(4), 556–577.
Clarke, M., Leach, M., & Scambary, J. (2013). Reconciling custom, citizenship and colonial
legacies: Ni-Vanuatu tertiary student attitudes to national identity. Nations and
Nationalism, 19(4), 715–738.
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1997, January 13). Evolutionary psychology: a primer. Retrieved
from http://www.psych.ucsb.edu/research/cep/primer.html.
Cox, M., Alatoa, H., Kenni, L., Naupa, A., Rawlings, G., Soni, N., ... Bulekone, V. (2007). The
unfinished state: Drivers of change in Vanuatu. Retrieved from http://www.ausaid.
gov.au/publications/pdf/vanuatu_change.pdf.
Curtis, V. A. (2013). Don't look, don't touch, don't eat: The science behind revulsion. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
C.L. Apicella et al. / Evolution and Human Behavior 39 (2018) 355–363
Curtis, V. A., Aunger, R., & Rabie, T. (2004). Evidence that disgust evolved to protect from
risk of disease. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences,
271(Suppl. 4), S131–S133.
Curtis, V. A., & Biran, A. (2001). Dirt, disgust, and disease: Is hygiene in our genes?
Perspectives in Biology and Medicine, 44, 17–31.
Curtis, V. A., Danquah, L. O., & Aunger, R. V. (2009). Planned, motivated, and habitual hygiene behavior: An eleven country review. Health Education and Research, 24(4),
655–673.
Curtis, V. A., de Barra, M., & Aunger, R. (2011). Disgust as an adaptive system for disease
avoidance behavior. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 366(1563), 389–401.
Douglas, M. (1966). Purity and danger: An analysis of concepts of pollution and taboo. New
York: Routledge.
Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 169–200.
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1971). Constants across cultures in the face and emotion.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 17(2), 124–129.
Fallon, A. E., Rozin, P., & Pliner, P. (1984). The child's conception of food: The development
of food rejections with special reference to disgust and contamination sensitivity.
Child Development, 55, 566–575.
Frazer, J. G. (1922). The golden bough: A study in magic and religion. New York: Macmillan
(Original work published 1890).
Freeland, W. J. (1980). Mangabey (Cerocebus albigena) movement patterns in relation to
food availability and fecal contamination. Ecology, 61(6), 1297–1303.
Freeman, M., Clasen, T., Dreibelbis, R., Saboori, S., Greene, L., Brumback, B., ... Rheingans, R.
(2014). The impact of a school-based water supply and treatment, hygiene, and sanitation programme on pupil diarrhoea: A cluster-randomized trial. Epidemiology and
Infection, 142(2), 340–351.
Gregory, J. E., & Gregory, R. J. (2002). Breaking equilibrium: Three styles of education on
Tanna, Vanuatu. Journal of Human Ecology, 13(5), 351–356.
Haidt, J., Bjorklund, F., & Murphy, S. (2000). Moral dumbfounding: When intuition finds no
reason. University of Virginia, Charlottesville: Unpublished manuscript.
Haselton, M. G., & Buss, D. M. (2000). Error management theory: A new perspective on
biases in cross-sex mind reading. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78,
81–91.
Hejmadi, A., Rozin, P., & Siegal, M. (2004). Once in contact, always in contact: Contagious
essence and conceptions of purification in American and Hindu Indian children.
Developmental Psychology, 40, 467–476.
Kalish, C. W. (1996). Preschoolers' understanding of germs as invisible mechanisms.
Cognitive Development, 11(1), 83–106.
Kalish, C. W. (1999). What young children's understanding of contamination and contagion tells us about their concept of illness. In M. Siegel, & C. C. Peterson (Eds.),
Children's understanding of biology and health. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Keesing, R. M. (1982). Kastom in Melanesia: An overview. Man, 13(4), 297–301.
Legare, C. H., Wellman, H. M., & Gelman, S. A. (2009). Evidence for an explanation advantage in naïve biological reasoning. Cognitive Psychology, 58(2), 177–194.
Liberman, Z., Woodward, A. L., Sullivan, K. R., & Kinzler, K. D. (2016). Early emerging system for reasoning about the social nature of food. Proceedings of the National Academy
of Sciences, 113(34), 9480–9485.
Marlowe, F. (2010). The Hadza: Hunter-gatherers of Tanzania. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Marlowe, F. W., Berbesque, J. C., Wood, B., Crittenden, A., Porter, C., & Mabulla, A. (2014).
Honey, Hadza, hunter-gatherers, and human evolution. Journal of Human Evolution,
71, 119–128.
Mauss, M. (1972). A general theory of magic. New York: Norton (Original work published
1902).
Nemeroff, C. J., & Rozin, P. (1989). “You are what you eat”: Applying the demand-free “impressions” technique to an unacknowledged belief. Ethos, 17(1), 50–69.
Nemeroff, C. J., & Rozin, P. (1994). The contagion concept in adult thinking in the United
States: Transmission of germs and interpersonal influence. Ethos, 22(2), 158–186.
Norton, R. (1993). Culture and identity in the South Pacific: A comparative analysis. Man,
28(4), 741–759.
Oaten, M., Stevenson, R. J., & Case, T. I. (2009). Disgust as a disease avoidance mechanism.
Psychological Bulletin, 135, 303–321.
363
Peck, J. G., & Gregory, R. J. (2005). A brief overview of the Old New Hebrides.
Anthropologist, 7(4), 269–282.
Raman, L., & Gelman, S. A. (2004). A cross-cultural developmental analysis of children's
and adults' understanding of illness in South Asia (India) and the United States.
Journal of Cognition & Culture, 4(2), 293–317.
Rozin, P., & Fallon, A. E. (1987). A perspective on disgust. Psychological Review, 94(1),
23–41.
Rozin, P., Fallon, A. E., & Augustoni-Ziskind, M. (1985). The child's conception of food: The
development of contamination sensitivity to “disgusting” substances. Developmental
Psychology, 21, 1075–1079.
Rozin, P., Grant, H., Weinberg, S., & Parker, S. (2007). “Head versus heart”: Effect of monetary frames on expression of sympathetic magical concerns. Judgment and Decision
making, 2, 217–224.
Rozin, P., Haidt, J., & McCauley, C. R. (1993). Disgust. In M. Lewis, & J. M. Haviland (Eds.),
Handbook of emotions (pp. 575–594). New York: Guilford Press.
Rozin, P., Haidt, J., & McCauley, C. R. (2008). Disgust. In M. Lewis, & J. M. Haviland-Jones
(Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 637–653). New York: Guilford Press.
Rozin, P., Haidt, J., & McCauley, C. R. (2016). Disgust. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, & L.
F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions. New York: Guilford Press.
Rozin, P., Millman, L., & Nemeroff, C. (1986). Operation of the laws of sympathetic magic
in disgust and other domains. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50,
703–712.
Rozin, P., & Nemeroff, C. (1990). The laws of sympathetic magic: A psychological analysis
of similarity and contagion. In J. W. Stigler, R. A. Shweder, & G. Herdt (Eds.), Cultural
psychology: Essays on comparative human development (pp. 205–232). New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Rozin, P., Nemeroff, C., Wane, M., & Sherrod, A. (1989). Operation of the sympathetic magical law of contagion in interpersonal attitudes among Americans. Bulletin of the
Psychonomic Society, 27, 367–370.
Schaich Borg, J., Lieberman, D., & Kiehl, K. A. (2008). Infection, incest, and iniquity: Investigating the neural correlates of disgust and morality. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience., 20(9), 1529–1546.
Siegal, M., & Share, D. L. (1990). Contamination sensitivity in young children.
Developmental Psychology, 26, 455–458.
Tanaka, J. (1996). The world of animals viewed by the San hunter-gatherers in Kalahari.
African Study Monographs, 22, 11–28.
Tybur, J. M., Lieberman, D., & Griskevicius, V. (2009). Microbes, mating, and morality: Individual differences in three functional domains of disgust. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 97(1), 103–122.
Tybur, J. M., Lieberman, D., Kurzban, R., & DeScioli, P. (2013). Disgust: Evolved function
and structure. Psychological Review, 120(1), 65–84.
Tyler, E. B. (1974). Primitive culture: Researches into the development of mythology, philosophy, religion, art, and custom. New York: Gordon Press (Original work published
1871).
Unsworth, S. J., Levin, W., Bang, M., Washinawatok, K., Waxman, S. R., & Medin, D. L.
(2012). Cultural differences in children's ecological reasoning and psychological
closeness to nature: Evidence from Menominee and European American children.
Journal of Cognition and Culture, 12(1–2), 17–29.
Watson-Jones, R. E., Busch, J. T. A., Harris, P. L., & Legare, C. H. (2017). Does the body survive death? Cultural variation in beliefs about life everlasting. Cognitive Science, 41,
455–476.
Watson-Jones, R. E., Busch, J. T. A., & Legare, C. H. (2015). Interdisciplinary and crosscultural perspectives on explanatory coexistence. Topics in Cognitive Science, 7(4),
611–623.
Wertz, A. E., & Wynn, K. (2014a). Selective social learning of plant edibility in 6- and 18month-old infants. Psychological Science, 25(4), 874–882.
Wertz, A. E., & Wynn, K. (2014b). Thyme to touch: Infants possess strategies that protect
them from dangers posed by plants. Cognition, 130(1), 44–49.
Williams, G. C. (1966). Adaptation and natural selection: A critique of some current evolutionary thought. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Purchase answer to see full
attachment