Social Psychology (PSYC 386, section LN1)
Paper Assignment for Spring, 2021
Darley, Teger, & Lewis, 1973, also looked at the Bystander Effect. Read their paper.
You’ll also need Principles.
Your paper must address the following points.
Explain “Latané and Darley’s Model of Helping.”
Use Principles as your source. You can stop explaining at “Assume Responsibility.”
Show me you understand the concept. Explain, don’t copy, don’t quote, DO cite.
If you don’t know what I’m talking about ASK ME.
IMPORTANT Do NOT use the internet as a source. Wikipedia is too detailed and poorly
sourced, verywellmind.com and simplypsychhology.org are insufficient. The best way to
stay out of trouble is to use our (free) textbook.
Download and read Darley, Teger, and Lewis, 1973. Skip the abstract. Do
this carefully; this reading is the most difficult part of the assignment.
Show me you understand Darley, et al. (1973). Explain, don’t copy, don’t quote, don’t
paraphrase. I want your words, even if you don’t think they’re very good. Quotes show
me you don’t understand it. Ask me for help if you need it.
What were Darley, et al. trying to demonstrate about the Model of Helping? Did they
succeed? (Hint: look at the percent of participants who responded in the three
conditions.) Refer back to your descriptions of The Model of Helping and Darley et al.’s
study.
Type it up, put it into APA, submit by Sunday, May 16, 11:59 PM.
There is no time for late papers. Late papers will take a letter grade penalty each day
it’s late. Get this done early.
APA: Let’s keep it simple. I only insist on the following.
a. Paper has a title page, with the page number in the upper right corner. All other
pages have the correct page number also in the upper right corner.
b. Cite! You’ll only need two sources: Principles and Darley, Teger, & Lewis, 1973.
c. The last page has your two references in proper APA, 7th edition format. I’ve done
Principles for you, if you know where to find it. Darley, et al., isn’t hard, but the fact that
I’ve used three different formats (at least one wrong, on purpose) should tell you that
you need to look it up. Try the Purdue OWL.
d. It’s not APA, but use proper grammar and spelling.
How long should it be? I don’t count pages. It could be done in two pages (not counting
the title page, body page, references page.) How long will it take to do the assignment?
Three to four hours, I think.
Submit it through Blackboard, through Safe Assign. Submit, it should go to Safe Assign
automatically. Go back and look at your originality report, and see if you’re too close to
somebody else’s writing. If it looks suspiciously close, change your writing and resubmit.
NOTE: References are going to be perfect matches, and since the paper will be really
short, there may be more suspicious matches than usual. If you have matches to
internet sources, absolutely reword those, and I don’t mean “change a couple of words
and now it’s my writing.”
Safe Assign gets slow. Don’t wait until the last minute. Safe Assign may take an hour to
clear if lots of people are using it.
Grading
The paper will be scored on a 0 – 10 point basis.
Did you understand the sources and write good summaries of them? 0 – 4 points.
Did you understand Darley, et al.’s results and explain how the results supported / did
not support their hypotheses? 0 – 4 pts.
Did you meet the minimal APA requirements I asked you to, including the reference list?
0 – 2 pts.
Points will be deducted for grammar and spelling mistakes.
Lots of points, maybe all of them, will be deducted for plagiarism.
The work is the understanding, not the writing. Understand Darley, Teger, and Lewis
(1973) and you should be able to easily pull a B on this assignment.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
1973, Vol. 26, No. 3, 395-399
DO GROUPS ALWAYS INHIBIT INDIVIDUALS'
RESPONSES TO POTENTIAL EMERGENCIES? 1
JOHN M. BARLEY 2
ALLAN I. TEGER 3
Princeton University
University of Pennsylvania
LAWRENCE D. LEWIS
Princeton University
Previous studies of bystander intervention in emergencies have found that an
individual is more likely to intervene if he witnesses the emergency alone than
as a member of a group. The present study qualifies this general finding in
the framework of group communication processes. Pairs of subjects working
on a task overheard a loud crash in an adjoining room. Some pairs of
subjects were seated in a pattern that facilitated the visual communication
exchanges that naturally occur when a noisy event takes place and others
were seated so as to block these communications. When the emergency
occurred, groups which could exchange reactions were not reliably less likely
to respond than were a third group of subjects who faced the emergency
alone. The blocked communications groups tended not to respond and responded significantly less than the other two conditions. These results were
interpreted as supporting the hypothesis that a group of people who witness
an ambiguous event interact to arrive at a definition or interpretation of it,
which then guides each member's reactions to the event.
From studies of bystander intervention in
emergencies, one empirical generalization
emerges: An individual who witnesses a potential emergency alone is more likely to
intervene than one who witnesses it as a
member of a group. This has been found
whether the emergency involves smoke pouring into a waiting room (Latane & Darley,
1968), a noisy accident to a girl in a nearby
room (Latane & Rodin, 1969), a person
stealing a case of beer from a liquor store
(Latane & Darley, 1970), or a child crying
in another room (Staub, 1970, true of older
children but not younger ones).
One explanation for this effect postulates
a decision process on the part of the individual bystander which is itself the result of
two other processes. First, in our culture, "it
is considered desirable to appear poised and
collected in times of stress [Latane & Darley, 1969, p. 249]." Second, when an am1
This research was supported by National Science
Foundation Grant 2293.
2
Requests for reprints should be sent to John
Darley, Department of Psychology, Princeton University, Princeton, New Jersey 08540.
3
The second author was a postdoctoral fellow at
the Educational Testing Service at the inception of
the study.
biguous event occurs, an individual bystander
will be considerably influenced by the ways
in which other bystanders are reacting to the
event.
Therefore when a bystander is faced with
the calm reactions of other bystanders, he
may infer that they do not define the event as
an emergency, and so he begins to define it
himself as no emergency. Thus a state of
"pluralistic ignorance" may develop (Latane
& Darley, 1969). Although the bystander
apparently conforms to the passive, nonhelping behavior of the other people, he does so
because he has decided nothing serious is
taking place, so that it would be inconsistent
with his own thinking to intervene. He has
been influenced by the other people, but influenced to accept a particular cognitive definition of the event. This is similar to the
process which Asch has labeled, "a change in
the object of judgment" rather than a change
in the judgment of the object. Inaction, then,
is a rational response to a situation that is
reinterpreted to require no intervention,
rather than an irrational compliance with
others' inaction. This explanation might be
called the "definition of the situation" hypothesis and can also account for the increase
395
396
J. DARLEY, A. TKGKR, AND L. LKWIS
in helping behavior that other investigators
have found when an individual observes another person helping (Bryan & Test, 1967;
Ross, 1970).
This suggests that, as in the case of conformity research (Allen & Levine, 1968;
Asch, 1952), any signals which break the
uniformity of the group's apparent unanimous indifference to the emergency may free
the individual to consider the possibility that
the event is in fact an emergency and act accordingly. One set of signals which may work
in this manner is the group of signals which
we might call "startle responses" (orienting
toward the noise, jumping, facial expressions
of concern, etc.). In previously mentioned
experiments, in which many subjects apparently defined the event as a nonemergency
and offered no help, the subjects were either
engaged in tasks which made observing each
other's startle responses unlikely (Latane &
Barley, 1969; Latane & Rodin, 1969; Staub,
1970) or the nature of the emergency was
such as not to provoke visible startle responses.
There is some reason to believe that the
startle response of an individual, if it is visible, might be taken as particularly revealing
of his true thoughts. Goffman (1969) and
others have suggested that we tend to place
more faith in information from others which
is spontaneous and apparently out of their
control. Thus facial expressions are often seen
as a more valid indication of the feelings of
others than is their overt behavior, for the
former are thought to be more spontaneous
and less subject to control than the latter.
The startle response, then, is a naturally
occurring response that signals some break in
the unanimity of the group's passive reactions
to a potential emergency, and thus provides
a mechanism for testing the definition of the
situation explanation for the typical lack of
response of groups witnessing emergencies.
An emergency was staged in a room adjacent
to one in which two subjects were working
on a visual perception task. The emergency
was a noisy crash which signaled that some
precariously balanced construction equipment
had fallen on a workman. Some of the groups
overheard the emergency while facing each
other, and thus were in a position to see
each other's startle response. Other groups
were oriented away from each other. In a
third condition, subjects faced the emergency
alone.
The predictions were that the subjects who
overheard the crash while alone would intervene, while the nonfacing groups would redefine the event as no emergency and therefore fail to intervene. These experimental
conditions replicate those of previous experiments, and the predicted findings would
replicate these results.
The orientation
hypothesis predicts that the subjects from
the facing groups will be more likely to define
the event as an emergency, and thus more
likely to offer help than the subjects from the
nonfacing groups.
METHOD
Subjects
Fifty male Princeton University undergraduates
served as subjects. All were volunteers who were
paid for their participation.
Procedure
Subjects, either in pairs or alone, arrived at the
experimenter's office where they were given a short
printed paragraph explaining the two types of activities in which they would be participating. The first
activity centered around various tests of vision
while the second involved an artistlike sketching
situation. They then accompanied the experimenter
and his assistant down the corridor to a room
where the vision testing was conducted. Here they
took several vision tests, while the assistant recorded
their responses.
After the vision tests had been completed, the
experimenter explained that he had arranged to
borrow a room from another research team for the
sketching task which would take up the remainder
of the session. Subjects then followed the experimenter upstairs to the sketching room where the
door was found to be locked. The experimenter
expressing consternation over the fact that the door
was "supposed to be left open" for him, muttered
something about checking to see if there was anyone
around who had the key. He knocked next door and
after a short delay a workman (actually a confederate of the experimenter) with screwdriver in
hand opened the door. The experimenter pointed
down the hall to the sketching room and asked if
the workman had a key that would open the door.
The workman replied that he didn't but speculated
that they could get in through a closed but unlocked
floor-to-ceiling partition which separated the two
rooms. The experimenter asked the subjects to
follow him through the workman's room as he pulled
open the sliding partition, admitted the subjects
Do GROUPS INHIBIT RESPONSES TO EMERGENCIES?
and himself into the sketching room, thanked the
workman, and pulled the partition shut.
Both rooms were filled with an array of electronic
equipment and construction materials, apparently
being used by the other investigators from whom
the room had been borrowed. Part of the construction materials included several large, heavy woodenframed metal screens balanced against a wall, which
the subjects had to step around in order to enter
the sketching room through the partition.
When the subjects had been seated, the experimenter -read a prepared set of instructions which
directed them to make a sketch of a model horse
that was in front of them. They were told not to
be concerned about how artistic the sketches were
but that the experimenter merely wanted a rough
idea of perspective in sketching from a real model
as compared to other groups who would do their
sketching from a photograph of the model. Subjects
were instructed not to talk and to do their sketches
independently. The experimenter then announced
that he was going back downstairs to correlate the
results of their vision tests and would be back in
about 10 minutes. (Actually the experimenter joined
the experimental assistant in an observation room
to observe and record the session through a oneway mirror which was disguised by drawings which
were hanging in front of it.)
Experimental Conditions
In the facing condition pairs of subjects were
seated face-to-face across a table on which there
was a large plastic model of a horse along with
sketching pads and drawing pencils. For the alone
condition, single subjects were seated in one of these
same two positions 50% of the time on one side
of the table and the remaining 50% on the opposite
side. In the nonfacing condition pairs of subjects
were seated back to back, and each subject had his
own horse model in front of him on a small stand.
Ten subjects were run in the alone condition and
10 pairs of subjects in each of the other two conditions. The experimenter was blind as to which of the
two group conditions was being run until, of course,
he entered the sketching room and could see the
arrangement of the chairs.
The Emergency Incident
Four minutes after the experimenter had left, the
workman staged the emergency incident. Pushing
over the heavy screens that had been balanced
against the wall on his side of the partition produced a resounding crash which was immediately
followed by an exclamation of "Oh, my leg I" and
a series of painful groans lasting for 3 seconds, which
were actually emanating from a concealed tape recorder. After an interval of 5 seconds there was
another shorter set of groans heard for 1.5 seconds
followed by complete silence. The confederate positioned himself on the floor next to the fallen screens,
holding his leg.
397
TABLE 1
LlKiaiHOUl) AND Sl'KKD OF HKU'ING RESPONSES
"/„
Average time of
responders in
seconds
Alone
90
Facing
80
Nonfacing
20
11.9
(9 individuals)
16.6
(8 groups)
17.0
(2 groups)
14.4
Condition
M
responding
(N = 10)
63.3
Average
time of
all subjects in
seconds
46.7
85.3
291.4
141.1
Dependent Measures and Debriefing
Subjects' reactions to the incident were recorded
and timed. The reaction time was measured from
the moment of the crash until the occurrence of
some overt helping response. For most subjects who
helped, the measure of response latency terminated
when they grasped the handle of the partition and
began ,to slide it back to investigate the situation
in the other room. The shouting of an inquiry
through the partition was also considered a helping
response and the reaction time measure was ended
as soon as the subject called out loudly enough to
be heard and acknowledged by the confederate.
After 6 minutes of observation the experimenter
returned to the sketching room and inquired how
things were going. If there was no spontaneous
report of the incident, the experimenter commented
that a secretary had mentioned that she had passed
by the room several minutes before and had heard
some sort of "commotion." After listening to the
subjects' replies, the experimenter carefully explained
the nature and necessity of the experimental deception and assured the subjects that the other person
was uninjured. He then administered a short questionnaire to obtain subjects' impressions of the incident and its perceived seriousness, previous experience with emergencies, and possible suspicion about
the experimental procedure.
RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
The results supported the experimental
hypothesis: As Table 1 indicates, 80% of
the groups in a face-to-face orientation responded to the crash with the offer of some
kind of help whereas only 20% of those
groups not facing each other reacted when
the incident occurred. (Fisher's exact test,
p < .01). Ninety percent of subjects reacted
to the crash when alone. Therefore 99% of
a set of two-person groups could be expected
to contain at least one individual who responds (1 — .10 2 ). If the effect of the group
on helping behavior is due to anything
other than a simple increase in number of
398
J. BARLEY, A. TEGEK, AND L. LEWIS
people over the alone condition, then the
rate of helping in a group should be significantly different from 99%. The &Q% response
rate of the face-to-face groups was not significantly different from the value 4 (p — .24)
while the 20% response rate of the nonfacing condition was significantly smaller
(Fisher's exact test, p < .Ol). 5
Helping rate was thus affected not simply
by the presence of other bystanders, but by
their physical orientation vis-a-vis each other;
groups in a facing orientation were more
likely to respond than nonfacing groups. In
fact, the facing groups were not significantly
slower or less likely to respond than were
individuals who were alone when the incident
occurred.
What happens in the face-to-face situation
which increases the level of helping over the
nonfacing situation? There are a number of
processes which may be involved—all of
which are related to the definition of the
situation. First, the observer receives contradictory information (the other observer does
not give help, indicating that the situation is
not an emergency; the other observer also
shows a startle response or worried expression, indicating that the situation is an
emergency). Since all the available information from the other person does not support
any single interpretation, the first observer
feels free to respond as do the subjects in
the alone condition—that is, as though it were
an emergency. Second, the startle response is
a spontaneous communication and as such it
is given greater weight (following Goffman)
than the more controllable and less spontaneous decision to refrain from helping. Third,
the individual not only observed the startle
response sequence of the bystander, but also
produced one himself. Realizing that the
other observer in the face-to-face situation
4
For simplicity these calculations were made, assuring an expected response rate of 100% rather
than 99%.
5
If the speed of response results are analyzed by
analysis of variance procedures and multiple comparisons (on the time scores), the above results
emerge more strongly (F = 9.60, p
Purchase answer to see full
attachment