DEPRESSION
AND
ANXIETY 0 : 1–16 (2010)
Review
A REVIEW OF ACUTE STRESS DISORDER IN DSM-5
Richard A. Bryant, Ph.D., Matthew J. Friedman, M.D., David Spiegel, M.D., Robert Ursano, M.D.,
and James Strain, M.D.
Acute stress disorder (ASD) was introduced into DSM-IV to describe acute stress
reactions (ASRs) that occur in the initial month after exposure to a traumatic
event and before the possibility of diagnosing posttraumatic stress disorder
(PTSD), and to identify trauma survivors in the acute phase who are high risk
for PTSD. This review considers ASD in relation to other diagnostic approaches
to acute stress responses, critiques the evidence of the predictive power of ASD,
and discusses ASD in relation to Adjustment Disorder. The evidence suggests
that ASD does not adequately identify most people who develop PTSD. This
review presents a number of options and preliminary considerations to be
considered for DSM-5. It is proposed that ASD be limited to describing severe
ASRs (that are not necessarily precursors of PTSD). The evidence suggests that
the current emphasis on dissociation may be overly restrictive and does not
recognize the heterogeneity of early posttraumatic stress responses. It is proposed
that ASD may be better conceptualized as the severity of acute stress responses
that does not require specific clusters to be present. Depression and Anxiety
0:1–16, 2010. r 2010 Wiley-Liss, Inc.
Key words: acute stress disorder; posttraumatic stress disorder; DSM-V
A
cute stress disorder (ASD) was introduced in DSM-IV
as a new diagnosis to describe acute stress reactions
(ASRs) that may precede posttraumatic stress disorder
(PTSD). In the prelude to DSM-5, it is appropriate to
review the utility of ASD as a diagnosis and to determine
the extent to which it adds value to the current diagnosis
of PTSD. The ASD diagnosis was introduced for two
primary reasons: to describe ASRs that occur in the
initial month after trauma exposure, which have earlier
gone unrecognized or were labeled adjustment disorders,[1] and to identify trauma survivors who are high risk
for developing subsequent PTSD.[2] At the time of its
introduction, there was far less evidence than we have
now to support the definition of the diagnosis.[3] This
review addresses (a) the definition of ASD, (b) the
distinction between ASD and ASRs, (c) the overlap
between ASD and Adjustment Disorder, (d) the capacity
of ASD to predict subsequent PTSD, (e) the role of
dissociation in ASD, (f) the benefits of the ASD to
enhance early intervention, (g) the range and utility of
emotional responses in the A2 definition, (h) crosscultural considerations for ASD, (i) the utility of an ASD
diagnosis, and (i) finally, a proposal for the modified ASD
definition in DSM-V. This article was commissioned by
the DSM-5 Anxiety, Obsessive–Compulsive Spectrum,
r 2010 Wiley-Liss, Inc.
Post-Traumatic, and Dissociative Disorders Work Group.
It represents the work of the authors for consideration
by the work group. In the course of this review, the
DSM-IV Source Book and DSM-IV Options Book
were also reviewed. Recommendations provided in this
article should be considered preliminary at this time;
they do not necessarily reflect the final recommendations or decisions that will be made for DSM-5, as the
DSM-5 development process is still ongoing. It is
possible that this article’s recommendations will be
School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, New
South Wales, Australia
Correspondence to: Richard A. Bryant, School of Psychology,
University of New South Wales, N.S.W., 2052, Australia.
E-mail: r.bryant@unsw.edu.au
The authors report they have no financial relationships within the
past 3 years to disclose.
Received for publication 20 March 2010; Revised 24 June 2010;
Accepted 24 June 2010
DOI 10.1002/da.20737
Published online in Wiley Online Library (www.wileyonlinelibrary.
com).
2
Bryant et al.
revised as additional data and input from experts and
the field are obtained.
DEFINITION OF ACUTE STRESS
DISORDER
Table 1 presents the DSM-IV criteria for ASD. The
primary difference between ASD and PTSD is the
duration of the symptoms and the former’s emphasis on
dissociative reactions to the trauma. ASD refers to
symptoms manifested during the period from 2 days to
4 weeks posttrauma, whereas PTSD can only be
diagnosed from 4 weeks. In terms of dissociation, the
diagnosis of ASD requires that the individual has at
least three of the following: (a) a subjective sense of
numbing or detachment, (b) reduced awareness of one’s
surroundings, (c) derealization, (d) depersonalization,
or (e) dissociative amnesia. There are other additional,
albeit minor, differences, which mainly involve less
stringent requirements to meet ASD avoidance and
arousal clusters relative to PTSD. Whereas PTSD
requires three avoidance or numbing symptoms and
two arousal symptoms, the ASD criteria require
‘‘marked’’ avoidance and arousal. Accordingly, it is
possible for an individual to satisfy criteria for ASD and
to not satisfy PTSD diagnostic criteria after 1 month
has transpired, even if the symptomatology has
remained unchanged. For example, an individual may
have satisfied ASD diagnosis by displaying marked
avoidance and arousal symptoms but may not have met
TABLE 1. DSM-IV-TR diagnostic criteria for ASD
A1. Exposure to catastrophic stressor
A2. Intense emotional reaction to stressor
B. During or after experiencing the distressing event, the individual
has three (or more) of the following dissociative symptoms
(1) Sense of numbing, detachment, or absence of emotional
responsiveness
(2) Reduction in awareness of surroundings (e.g., ‘‘being in a daze’’)
(3) Derealization
(4) Depersonalization
(5) Dissociative amnesia
C. The traumatic event is persistently reexperienced in at least one
of the following ways: recurrent images, thoughts, dreams,
illusions, flashback episodes, or a sense of reliving the experience;
or distress on exposure to reminders of the traumatic event
D. Marked avoidance of stimuli that arouse recollections of the
trauma (e.g., thoughts, feelings, conversations, activities, places,
people)
E. Marked symptoms of anxiety or increased arousal (e.g., difficulty
sleeping, irritability, poor concentration, hypervigilence,
exaggerated startle response, motor restlessness)
F. The disturbance causes significant distress or impairment
G. The disturbance lasts for a minimum of 2 days and a maximum of
4 weeks and occurs within 4 weeks of the traumatic event
H. Disturbance is not due to the effects of substance use or medical
condition or is not better accounted for by brief psychotic
disorder, and is not merely an exacerbation of a preexisting Axis I
or II disorder
Depression and Anxiety
the PTSD criteria for multiple symptoms within each
cluster. This inconsistency could result in individual
satisfying criteria for a psychiatric disorder at 3 weeks
posttrauma but not receiving a diagnosis 2 weeks later.
This is not necessarily problematic if one conceptualizes these two disorders as distinct responses (acute
versus chronic stress reactions).
DISTINCTION BETWEEN ASD
AND ACUTE STRESS REACTIONS
One of the goals of ASD was to describe ASRs.
There is much evidence in the literature of a range of
distressing responses in the aftermath of trauma,
including posttraumatic anxiety, mood disturbances,
sleep problems, aggression, substance abuse, and many
other physical and psychological symptoms.[4,5] That
is, by focusing on acute posttraumatic stress reactions,
the ASD diagnosis does not encompass a broader array
of psychological reactions that commonly occur and
can be as distressing as ASD responses. For example,
despite not yet being formally recognized as a
psychiatric disorder, prolonged grief may develop after
traumatic loss with documented adverse mental health
outcomes.[6] This condition is currently recognized 6
months after the loss because most bereaved individuals adapt during this time.[7] Before that time,
however, and certainly within the initial month, many
bereaved people suffer intense grief reactions.
Despite the broad range of emotional reactions
reported by trauma survivors in the acute phase,
ASD is conceptualized in DSM-IV as on the one
hand depicting an array of relatively common pathological acute responses to trauma, and yet also
constrained by symptom similarity as a precursor to
PTSD. Accordingly, it has focused on anxiety and
dissociative responses. There is strong evidence that
the symptoms reported in ASD are common in the
aftermath of trauma, including re-experiencing[8,9] and
avoidance.[10,11] Arousal symptoms are also common,
including insomnia,[10,12,13] concentration deficits,[10,11]
irritabilty,[12] and agitation.[12,13] A criticism of the
current focus of ASD on anxiety responses is that it
neglects other early distressing emotional reactions,
including depression, guilt, shame, anger, disgust, shock,
or somatic reactions.[5] In the current diagnostic system,
these reactions—which may be as impairing as fear/
anxiety responses—may be diagnosed as Adjustment
Disorder when they contribute to marked impairment.
The theoretical position that underlies the DSM-IV
conceptualization of ASD can be contrasted with the
description of ASRs in the tenth edition of the
International Classification of Diseases.[14] Instead of being
considered a precursor of subsequent psychopathology,
the ICD-10 conceptualizes ASR as a transient reaction
that can be evident immediately after the traumatic
event and usually resolves within 2–3 days after a
trauma. The ICD description of ASR includes primarily
Review: Acute Stress Disorder
dissociative (daze, stupor, amnesia) and anxiety (tachycardia, sweating, flushing) reactions. Some commentators
have suggested that this wide-ranging description may
be more useful for clinicians than the more focused
DSM-IV criteria.[5,15] This approach allows for the
consideration of acute distress which may warrant
intervention (e.g., sleep disturbance) but does not
attempt to predict subsequent disorder. Further, ASR
notes that the very initial period after trauma exposure
may result in a rather amorphous distress state that
encompasses many emotional responses that cannot be
readily classified into different responses.[16] In the
subsequent period, responses may crystallize into more
classifiable responses, such as anxiety or mood reactions.[17] Although there is some evidence that people
who suffer ASR are more likely to suffer persistent
psychological reactions beyond 48 hr,[18,19] this knowledge
base is very limited. By definition, ASR occurs before
ASD can begin—the first 48 hr. ASD covers the time
period from 48 hr to 1 month, when PTSD could begin.
Thus, those whose symptoms persist or start after 48 hr
may represent a different group of trauma responders.
Related to ASR is the construct of combat stress
reactions (CSR). This is a similar construct to ASR,
except that it is not time-limited. This classification can
describe combat personnel who are not able to function
at any point after being exposed to severe stress in the
course of combat. This construct has a very long
historical tradition in military circles as a means to
describe the broad range of psychological responses in
the immediate and longer term periods following
combat.[1] One important difference of CSR in contrast
to ASD and ASR is that there is no requirement of
functional impairment, which may lead to overidentification of people in need of mental health services.[2]
There are longer term follow-up studies of CSR
personnel, and these tend to report statistically higher
rates of PTSD and dysfunction in those with initial
CSR.[20] Despite this relationship, there is a dearth of
evidence about the specificity of CSR to predict
subsequent PTSD.
DISTINCTION BETWEEN ASD
AND ADJUSTMENT DISORDER
Adjustment disorder currently describes a broad
range of psychological responses to a stressful event
(that may not necessarily be traumatic). These
responses can occur immediately after the event, and
although it has traditionally been regarded as a shortterm adjustment problem, there is also provision for
the diagnosis to be made when the person has chronic
impairment secondary to the consequences of the
event. It is distinguished from ASD in several ways.
First, whereas ASD is limited to fear/anxiety responses,
Adjustment Disorder encompasses all forms of distressing responses (e.g., depression, anger, guilt). Second,
although Adjustment Disorder is predictive of
3
subsequent impairment,[21] the disorder intentionally
describes current dysfunction and it does not contain
the explicit goal of identifying people who will suffer
subsequent impairment. Third, whereas ASD can be
diagnosed from 2 days after the event up to 1 month,
Adjustment Disorder can be diagnosed immediately
after the event. It should be noted that Adjustment
Disorder can have broad clinical utility because it
allows a clinician to describe the various psychological
disturbances that a person may be experiencing in the
immediate aftermath of an aversive event. For example,
after the New York terrorist attacks of 9/11, clinicians
were able to offer many survivors formal mental health
care by describing their diverse reactions with Adjustment Disorder. Thus, Adjustment Disorder in DSMIV can be used, by temporal definition, to cover
situations described as ASR (F43.0) in ICD-10,
whereas ASD cannot.
A limitation of Adjustment Disorder is that its broad
definition has not resulted in focused treatment
interventions to alleviate the condition. One potential
advantage of ASD is that it has identified a specific
form of initial adjustment difficulties that occur in the
acute phase of trauma that is distinguished by its
symptom structure and is amenable to early interventions. That is, whereas most difficulties that are
described by the Adjustment Disorder diagnosis are
diverse, poorly defined, and not specific to a treatment
intervention, ASD has allowed the identification of an
anxiety-based acute response that responds positively
to treatment.
PREDICTIVE UTILITY OF ASD
OF SUBSEQUENT PTSD
As noted above, one goal of the ASD diagnosis was to
identify people in the initial month following trauma
exposure who are not experiencing a transient stress
reaction, but rather display severe acute responses and
the prodromal symptoms of PTSD. To evaluate the
capacity of ASD to identify people who are at high risk
for PTSD development, Bryant recently conducted a
systematic analysis of the literature of prospective
studies of ASD and PTSD by searching PsycINFO,
MEDLINE, PubMed, and PILOTS for Englishlanguage articles published between 1994 and 2009
using keywords that combined ASD and stress disorders/
PTSD. Studies were then included if they assessed ASD
within 1 month of trauma exposure and prospectively
followed the same participants over time to assess
PTSD diagnosis. Inclusion required studies to use
measures of ASD and PTSD that permitted diagnostic
prevalence rates based on DSM-IV criteria. Twenty-two
studies were identified that assessed ASD within
a month of trauma exposure and determined the
relationship between ASD and subsequent PTSD;[22–43]
19 with adults and 3 with children (a detailed report of
this analysis is reported elsewhere[44]).
Depression and Anxiety
4
Bryant et al.
The rates of full ASD ranged from 7 to 28%, with a
mean rate of 13%. It is noteworthy that a proportion of
these studies have also reported the prevalence of
subsyndromal ASD, which is typically defined as
satisfying at least three (typically not requiring the
dissociative criterion) of the symptom clusters. Including
subsyndromal cases of ASD, together with cases that
meet full ASD criteria results in markedly higher rates
of identified cases, with the range from 10–32% and a
mean of 23%. Across some of these studies, the rates of
trauma survivors displaying acute stress are increased
primarily by not requiring the dissociative criteria to be
met. Bryant’s review of these studies reported that the
positive predictive power of studies was moderately
strong, with most studies of adults indicating that at
least half of those trauma survivors with ASD meeting
criteria for subsequent PTSD.[44] These studies suggest
that people who do meet criteria for ASD are at higher
risk for persistent PTSD. In contrast, the sensitivity
across most studies was poor, indicating that the
majority of trauma survivors who eventually developed
PTSD did not meet the full criteria for ASD.
Specifically, the 22 identified studies included followup assessments of 3,335 individuals. Although 497 of
these trauma survivors met criteria for PTSD at the
follow-up assessment, only 238 (48%) of these
individuals had met criteria for ASD in the month
following trauma exposure. This suggests that if a
major goal of ASD is to predict people who will
subsequently develop PTSD, it is failing to identify half
of those who will meet criteria for PTSD at some later
time. In some cases, this may be because significant
psychopathology simply does not emerge until later. In
other cases the ASD criteria may lack sufficient
sensitivity.
Some studies reported data that permitted calculation of the predictive capacity of subsyndromal ASD,
defined as satisfying only three of the ASD symptom
clusters (often by omitting the requirement that
dissociation be present). Overall, these analyses indicate that the sensitivity is generally better when one
adopts a subsyndromal approach. That is, by focusing
on general posttraumatic stress symptoms rather than
emphasizing dissociation, more people who eventually
develop PTSD can be identified in the acute phase.
This pattern may be explained, in part, by the fact that
the re-experiencing, avoidance, and arousal clusters in
ASD match onto the same symptom clusters in PTSD,
whereas dissociation encompasses a different symptom
set that is not as strongly represented in the PTSD
diagnostic criteria. This approach resulted in significant proportions of trauma survivors who developed
PTSD not being identified in the acute phase. It should
also be noted that the relationship of ASD to other
later occurring psychiatric disorders has been less
studied (e.g., depression, alcohol use, prolonged grief).
Recent research developments have identified a range
of peritraumatic markers of high risk for PTSD that
may facilitate identification of acutely trauma-exposed
Depression and Anxiety
people who may benefit from early intervention. This
research has extended beyond the ASD diagnosis in
different ways to improve prediction of subsequent
PTSD. Numerous studies indicate that people who
develop PTSD have higher heart rates immediately
after trauma than those who do not develop PTSD.[45]
A comparable pattern has been found for acute
respiration rate.[46] Numerous studies have found that
maladaptive appraisals about oneself and one’s environment is strongly predictive of subsequent PTSD.[47–49]
Other approaches have adopted a broader framework
and assessed pretrauma, peritraumatic, and posttrauma
risk factors that potentially predict PTSD. For example,
one screening measure was developed that included
items about psychiatric history, perceived level of
threat, and access to social support, and was found to
usefully predict PTSD.[50] These developments underscore the point that there are potentially better means
to identify people at risk for PTSD development than
an acute diagnosis. However, although such biomarkers
are of considerable research interest, they have not been
employed successfully to correlate or predict diagnosis.
ROLE OF DISSOCIATION IN ASD
A cornerstone of the current ASD diagnosis is that
the survivor has at least three dissociative symptoms.
From a theoretical perspective, this position was
influenced by the view that dissociative responses in
the wake of trauma may impede access to affect and
memories about their traumatic experience, which may
limit emotional processing and recovery.[51] From an
empirical perspective, there is much evidence that
peritraumatic dissociative reactions are very common,
including emotional numbing, altered time sense,
reduction in awareness of one’s surroundings, depersonalization, and amnesia.[10,13] Furthermore, there are
many studies indicating that peritraumatic dissociation
is predictive of subsequent PTSD.[35,52–55] Despite this
evidence, there are several issues that raise significant
concerns about the emphasis currently placed on
dissociation in ASD.
First, some recent meta-analyses have suggested that,
in the majority of longitudinal studies, peritraumatic
dissociation has not emerged as an independent
predictor of PTSD,[56,57] although others show that it
does.[58] Second, a major reason for ASD not predicting
PTSD adequately is the requirement that dissociative
symptoms be present, which precludes many high-risk
people from being identified.[59] Third, there is much
evidence that dissociative responses are common under
conditions of high stress and may not necessarily be
associated with psychopathology.[60,61] It should be
noted, however, that this argument may also be made
in reference to arousal, which may be normal in many
cases but does not predict later PTSD.
There are several possible mechanisms that may
account for the mixed findings about peritraumatic
dissociation and subsequent PTSD. First, dissociation
Review: Acute Stress Disorder
may play a role in PTSD development in some
individuals but not others. Diathesis stress models of
dissociative disorders suggest that only people who
possess dissociative tendencies respond to trauma with
dissociative reactions.[62,63] This notion is supported by
evidence that higher levels of hypnotizability have been
reported in people with ASD compared to those who
have a comparable ASR, but lack dissociative symptoms.[64] Although both groups may have high risk for
developing PTSD, only the subset of people who
possess dissociative tendencies seem to respond with
acute dissociative symptoms. Second, it is also possible
that peritraumatic dissociation is associated with
subsequent PTSD because it is associated with other
known risk factors for PTSD development. For
example, there is documented relationship between a
history of childhood trauma and subsequent dissociation tendencies.[65] Moreover, childhood trauma is a
known risk factor for adult PTSD.[66] It is possible that
peritraumatic dissociation may be linked to PTSD
because of its association with childhood trauma.[67]
Third, another potential role of dissociation is its
association with hyperarousal and extreme anxiety in
the acute phase after trauma exposure. Peritraumatic
dissociation may be a consequence of elevated arousal
that occurs during trauma.[68] This interpretation is
consistent with evidence that the relationship between
peritraumatic dissociation and acute stress depends on
levels of peritraumatic panic.[69,70]
Another problem with the DSM-IV definition of
dissociation was that it stated that dissociation in ASD
could occur ‘‘either during or after experiencing the
distressing event.’’ The ambiguity concerning the time
frame for dissociation is problematic because transient
dissociation (peritraumatic dissociation) and persistent
dissociation can lead to contrary predictions concerning
outcome. Cognitive models of trauma would predict
that persistent dissociation would be maladaptive and
would be associated with subsequent PTSD, because it
impedes retrieval of emotional memories that are
required for adaption.[71] In contrast, transient dissociation at the time of the trauma could be a normal
response under stress, and even protective because it
may limit the encoding of trauma of experiences. In
this context, it is worth noting that persistent dissociation is more predictive of ASD[72] and subsequent
PTSD[73] than dissociation that only occurs at the time
of the traumatic experience. Similarly, a recent
prospective study found that persistent dissociation
was a stronger predictor of subsequent PTSD than
dissociation occurring immediately after the trauma.[35]
This pattern accords with evidence that much peritraumatic dissociation is transient. For example, one study
found that the vast majority of trauma survivors who
experienced dissociative reactions at the time of the
trauma did not develop pathology, and their dissociative symptoms did not persist beyond the trauma.[74]
However, the DSM-IV definition of ASD provides
some protection against this problem by requiring that
5
symptoms ‘‘last for a minimum of 2 days’’ (p. 472).
There is some ambiguity about the transient and
persistent nature of dissociation in ASD because
whereas it does require a minimum of 2 days, it also
states that dissociation may occur during or after the
trauma. There seems to be inadequate specification of
the exact dissociative symptoms described in ASD.
Although studies of chronic samples have found that
dissociative symptoms load onto distinct clusters,[75]
this has not been found to be the case in recently
trauma-exposed individuals. For example, one study of
acute dissociative responses reported that 85% of
individuals who reported lack of awareness of their
surroundings also report derealization.[76] Furthermore, it seems repetitive to conclude that a traumatized
individual suffers amnesia if that same individual did
not adequately encode an event because of reduced
awareness. However, although failure of encoding
could result in amnesia, it is a different problem than
retrieval difficulty, especially if amnesia is potentially
reversible. Dissociation in the acute phase may function differently than more chronic dissociation. These
issues suggest that the DSM-IV definition of dissociation may require greater delineation of dissociative
symptoms to ensure that strongly overlapping constructs are not being assessed multiple times. In this
context, it is worth noting that a recent factor analysis
of the Peritraumatic Dissociative Experiences Questionnaire[68] found two distinct factors: Lack of
Awareness and Derealization.[77] Whereas Lack of
Awareness was not associated with ASRs, Derealization
was. This pattern suggests that dissociative symptoms
involving altered perceptions of self or one’s surrounds
may reflect more severe stress reactions than alterations
in attention.
Taken together, these data suggest that dissociation
does not warrant the central place in ASD that it has
held. Nonetheless, it is clearly an important emotional
response that many people do experience, and it can
often be associated with severe reactions. The major
problems with its prominence in ASD seems to be (a)
mandating its presence in order to diagnose ASD,
although as noted above, dissociative responses may be
frequent in the acute aftermath of trauma, (b) using it
to predict subsequent PTSD, which is a secondary and
not a primary purpose of the disorder, (c) artificially
distinguishing between overlapping dissociative
responses, and (d) loosely combining dissociation
occurring at the time of the trauma and in the
following period. Future iterations of acute dissociative
definitions need to more specifically operationalize the
phenomena and specify the timeframe of the defined
dissociative response. Despite these criticisms of
current definitions of peritraumatic dissociation, the
limitations of acute dissociation in predicting subsequent PTSD are also applicable to other ASD
symptoms. As noted above, even when the dissociation
cluster is not required, subsyndromal ASD is still only a
modest predictor of PTSD. Nonetheless, the presence
Depression and Anxiety
6
Bryant et al.
of dissociative symptoms in many trauma survivors
suggests these reactions need to be considered among
the constellation of acute stress responses.
DOES THE ASD FACILITATE
EARLY INTERVENTION
AFTER TRAUMA?
One of the potential benefits of early identification
of people at high risk for PTSD development is the
opportunity to provide secondary prevention to limit
the subsequent disorder. There is now considerable
evidence that abridged forms of cognitive behaviour
therapy (CBT) for people with ASD is efficacious in
reducing subsequent PTSD.[78–83] This evidence is
consistent with findings that providing CBT survivors
with PTSD in the first few months after trauma
exposure prevents longer term problems.[84–86] These
findings do indicate that identifying people shortly
after trauma with ASD/acute PTSD and providing
them with trauma-focused CBT is beneficial in
reducing subsequent PTSD. Two implications of this
evidence emerge for DSM-V. First, this evidence
highlights the utility of having a means of identifying
people who are high risk for PTSD and can be treated
in the acute phase; the ability to identify people who
are high risk, however, may not necessarily require a
diagnostic category. There may be other means to
optimally identify people who can benefit from early
intervention (reviewed above). Second, these successful
studies have focused on exposure-based interventions
for people who suffer anxiety. In the context of the
consideration that ASD may be broadened to consider
other emotional reactions (e.g., depression, anger,
guilt, somatic reactions, dissociation, etc.), there are
significant issues for treatment planning for these acute
presentations. For example, whereas there is strong
evidence for CBT to target anxiety-based presentations, there is no equivalent evidence for managing
early responses to trauma that primarily involves other
responses, such as anger, depression, or guilt reactions.
Although evidence-based interventions exist for these
various posttraumatic reactions, they may not require
or respond well to exposure-based treatments that are
the hallmark of treating ASD/PTSD. This is an issue
that needs to be recognized if considering an expansion
of the fear/anxiety focus of the current definition of
ASD.
ROLE OF SUBJECTIVE
EMOTIONAL STRESSORS (A2)
IN ASD
As with PTSD, in order to meet the Stressor (A)
Criterion in the current definition of ASD, individuals
exposed to threatening events (A1) must also experience
an intense subjective (A2) reaction characterized as
Depression and Anxiety
‘‘fear, helplessness, or horror.’’ In DSM-IV, A2 was
included in ASD (and PTSD) to more specifically
identify people who had a negative reaction to a
traumatic event rather than simply survive the experience. The utility of the A2 criterion has been
challenged by a series of studies focusing on PTSD
but have relevance to the role of A2 in ASD. One study
found that intense levels of acute postexposure fear,
helplessness, and horror were weakly predictive of
PTSD 6 months later.[87] Furthermore, other posttraumatic emotional reactions (such as anger or shame)
also predicted PTSD. Finally, a small number of people
who denied postexposure A2 emotions also met PTSD
criteria at 6 months. Another study reported that
within a sample of A1 exposed individuals who went on
to meet PTSD B-F criteria, a substantial minority
(23%) failed to receive a PTSD diagnosis because of
the absence of A2.[88] Furthermore, there were no
differences with regard to B-F symptom severity or
impairment between the A2 positive and A2 negative
cohorts examined among a community sample of 6,104
adults with a history of trauma exposure, and a
substantial minority (24% males and 19% females)
failed to meet criterion A2.[89] There is also evidence
that only helplessness, but not fear or horror, is
correlated with posttraumatic symptomatology.[90]
Supporting this accumulating evidence are reports
that the presence of A2 had no effect on PTSD
prevalence in the DSM-IV Field Trials[91] and in a
sample of older male military veterans.[92] These results
were replicated by Karam et al. (submitted, under
review), regarding data from 102,846 respondents to
the WHO World Mental Health Survey Initiative, who
found that only 1.4% of respondents who met all other
PTSD criteria failed to meet the A2 criterion. Based on
such findings, a number of investigators have called for
the elimination of the A2 criterion because of its poor
positive predictive value, and it does not enhance
identification of people who will develop PTSD. On
the other hand, a consistent finding from three studies
is the negative predictive value of A2.[87,91,92] In other
words, people who do not exhibit an intense posttraumatic emotional reaction are unlikely to develop
PTSD.
It has been suggested that the context for acute
posttraumatic reactions should be expanded beyond the
‘‘fear, helplessness and horror’’ derived from a fear
conditioning model of PTSD. For example, panic
attacks have been one distinctive reaction that has
received attention. In the DSM-IV Field Trials, the
predominant indicator of posttraumatic distress was a
panic reaction consisting of two components: ‘‘panicphysiological arousal’’ and ‘‘other panic symptoms’’
(such as trembling, shaking, tachycardia, and fear of
dying), rather than A2 symptoms.[91] This accords with
proposals that the physical and cognitive symptoms of
panic mediate dissociation, as well as (A2) fear,
helplessness, and horror. Other investigators have
suggested that the focus should be on the positive
Review: Acute Stress Disorder
predictive value of a fright reaction[93] or peritraumatic autonomic activation.[94]
As noted earlier, a number of investigators[95] have
argued that other strong peritraumatic emotions are
also associated with PTSD, such as sadness, grief,
anger, guilt, shame, and disgust. The DSM-IV Field
Trial observed that people could deny A2 (fear, helplessness, or horror), as well as a panic reaction but
report confusion, negative affect, embarrassment, and
disgust.[91] Such a wide spectrum of posttraumatic
reactions calls into question the utility of limiting A2 to
the fear conditioning model. The DSM-IV does not
consider other emotions, such as anger and rage, which
are experienced during fight rather than flight
responses, shame if an individual is made to endure
degradation or guilt if there is a violation of one’s moral
beliefs or ethical standards (Resick and Miller, in press).
Finally, military and emergency medical personnel (for
example) often recall that they did not experience any
acute A2 or other emotional reaction in the immediate
aftermath of trauma exposure. Rather, they report that
their ‘‘training kicked in’’ as they carried out their
responsibilities.[96,97]
CROSS-CULTURAL
CONSIDERATIONS FOR ASD
Whereas there is some evidence on cross-cultural
manifestations of ASRs, there is very little crosscultural work on the predictive power of ASD.
Comparing prevalence across studies for ASD is
difficult because (a) studies have used different assessment tools (ranging from validated measures to existing
instruments for PTSD that have been amended to
index ASD criteria but have not been validated), and (b)
assessed patients from very different types of trauma.
Generally speaking, most Western studies have focused
on survivors of traumatic injury because these are
convenient samples that can be located in hospitals;
several studies from non-Western settings have measured reactions to war or massive natural disaster.
These different contexts result in major confounds
between culture and severity and type of traumatic
experience. A further possible confound in comparing
settings is the timeframe adopted by DSM-IV. ASD is
defined as the satisfaction of the symptom criteria
between 2 days and 4 weeks after the traumatic event.
This definition implicitly presumes that the traumatic
event has a discrete onset and offset, such as a motor
vehicle accident or assault. In contrast, in the case of
people responding to a massive natural disaster, civil
conflict, or refugee relocation, these traumatic experiences can be experienced for months or years—in these
cases it is problematic to compare prevalence rates in a
setting where the traumatic event is discrete compared
to ongoing. Highlighting the variability of ASD
prevalence across trauma events is evidence that
ASD has been identified variably following terrorist
7
attacks after 9/11 (9%),[98] motor vehicle accident
(13–25%),[59] and witnesses to drive-by shooting
(33%).[99] In terms of comparing prevalence, it is most
useful to compare estimates across the same type of
event. The most studied type of event is traumatic
injury. In this context, prevalence does vary across
settings. For example, prevalence of ASD following
traumatic injury has been reported in populations in
Australia (1,[100] 6,[101] 13,[22] and 14%[23]), United
Kingdom (21[25] and 10%[35]), Switzerland (4%[102]),
Germany (6%[43]), and Japan (9%[31]). These variable
rates highlight that the prevalence rates do vary across
settings, even when the trauma type is held constant,
although there is inevitable variability across studies in
stressor severity. Although most injury studies converge
on prevalence rates between 6–10%, there are significant outliers (i.e., 1% in Australia and 4% in
Switzerland, and up to 21% in the United Kingdom).
This variability suggests that even among Western
settings, the ASD prevalence varies.
In terms of prediction, across most studies there is
convergence that whereas the majority of people who
meet criteria for ASD subsequently develop PTSD,
most people who develop PTSD do not initially meet
criteria for ASD.[59] This pattern is reflected across
cultures insofar as ASD has modest sensitivity in
identifying people who will subsequently develop
PTSD in Australia, the United Kingdom, the United
States, Switzerland, Germany, Norway, and Japan.
The hallmark feature of ASD is dissociation. The
role of dissociation is complicated in cross-cultural
contexts because dissociative states have different
connotations in different cultures. It is worth noting
that general dissociation levels differ across racial
groups; for example, African-Americans and AsianAmericans report higher rates of dissociation than
white Americans.[103] Whereas DSM-IV presumes that
peritraumatic dissociation is a maladaptive response
that is a precursor to psychopathology, many different
cultures perceive dissociative states in spiritual or
religious frameworks that may be considered adaptive.[104] For example, the Candomblé religion in Brazil
perceives dissociative states as part of a spiritual life
history that needs to be narrated for self-healing.[105]
Several studies have reported that depersonalization is
reported more often in Western settings than Columbia[106]
and Peru.[107] It has been suggested that, whereas in
individualistic societies (i.e., Western societies) having
a detached view of oneself is regarded as aberrant, in
collectivist societies it is common to view oneself in
relation to one’s surrounds. Although dissociative states
can reflect maladaptive reactions across many cultures,
it is premature to presume that dissociative states after
trauma are indicative of poor adjustment in different
cultures until appropriate studies are conducted. It
should be noted that the same could be said of fear,
anxiety, sadness, and other responses that can be
appropriate and adaptive in some contexts and maladaptive in others.
Depression and Anxiety
8
Bryant et al.
From a cross-cultural perspective, there may also
problems in presuming a fear circuitry perspective with
ASD that focuses on a narrow definition of the
manifestation of anxiety. Some societies have reported
acute reactions that extend beyond these responses. For
example, a study in the collectivist society of Micronesia found that the majority of youth exposed to
traumatic stress responded with s’a’aw, which is a term
that fuses anger with fear and reflects the negative
emotional reaction to youth feeling that their standing
in their local community was threatened.[108] Focusing
specifically on fear in the sense of fear circuitry
described could omit this ASR.
There is considerable evidence that ASRs are
characterized by panic attacks.[109] Although there is
not a significant body of evidence pertaining to panic in
the acute phase after trauma, there is much evidence
concerning the nature of panic attacks in different cultures.
It is reasonable to conclude that these culturally
specific features of panic attacks in different cultures
will apply to posttraumatic panic in the acute phase.
For example, ataques de nervios have been documented
in Dominican and Puerto Rican people, which has
shown that although this constructs overlaps with
panic, it is not identical.[110] Whereas ataques de nervios
shares features with panic attacks (fear of losing
control, dizziness, fear of dying), there are other
features not included in DSM-IV definitions of panic
attacks (e.g., screaming, hitting out). Much work on
ataques de nervios has focused on the overlap with
ataques de nervios and panic disorder; however, the
relevant issue for ASD is the breadth of description of
acute reactions of panic attacks that does not limit it to
strict panic disorder-type panic attacks. Similarly, there
has been much work done on panic attacks in
Cambodians (khyaˆl), which involves a perception that
a ‘‘wind’’ can enter the body in the diaphragm and rise
through the body and cause a range of symptoms,
including shortness of breath, tinnitus, dizziness,
soreness in the neck, and catastrophic fears for ones
well-being.[111,112] Rather than specify the exact nature
of the panic symptoms associated with Cambodian
reactions, it is sufficient to note that culturally specific
descriptions of panic should be applied to ASD because
panic attacks (as distinct from panic disorder) are
common in the acute phase after trauma.
ASD currently requires marked avoidance to satisfy
criteria. This has conceptual overlap with emotional
numbing, which is described as one of the dissociative
symptoms. It is important to note that certain Asian
cultures explicitly discourage emotional displays
because it may lead to adverse consequences. For
example, people from Bali may adopt a ‘‘smooth’’
facade following trauma to mask emotional distress;
this is adopted to avoid personal illness, harm to others,
and harm to the spirits of lost loved ones.[113] Similarly,
the Toraja in Indonesia avoid strong emotions which is
commonly regarded as adaptive, because intense
emotions are considered to be linked to poor health
Depression and Anxiety
outcomes.[114] To presume that all forms of avoidance
are markers of impaired emotional processing and are
maladaptive may ignore important local cultural
standards of coping. At this point, there is insufficient
data to inform us about the extent to which different
forms of avoidance are adaptive or maladptive across
cultural settings.
TIMEFRAME FOR ASD
In DSM-IV, ASD could be diagnosed between 2 days
and 4 weeks following the traumatic event. There was
no sound evidence for the minimum 2-day delay
between the onset of the traumatic event and the
symptoms. There is concern that diagnosing people
within 48 hr of an event may identify many trauma
survivors who are experiencing transient stress reactions that will subsequently abate in the following days.
In terms of the timeframe, the available evidence points
to extending the current minimum delay from 2 days to
a longer timeframe, to reduce the likelihood of false
positive diagnoses; that is, by classifying people as
satisfying ASD who may no longer display these
symptoms 1 or 2 days later. There is a need to find a
balance between introducing the diagnosis too early,
yet ensuring that highly distressed people can receive
diagnosis and treatment as soon as they need it. There
is very little evidence to guide DSM-5 regarding the
optimal timeframe, because this decision is largely
influenced by situationally specific factors and by the
posttrauma environment. It is highly probable that
ASD reactions will be higher when current threat is
still present relative to when the threat has passed. For
example, one study reported PTSD prevalence of 99%
in Sierra Leone; however, this study was conducted in
the context of active civil unrest and direct threat to
respondents.[115] It would be useful to study datasets
that have assessed ASRs at different timeframes
within the initial weeks following trauma exposure,
because this can shed light on the trajectory of acute
symptoms over the initial weeks, which would provide
some guidance on the optimal number of symptoms
required to identify people who are likely to be suffering
acute symptoms beyond the immediate period.
DISTINCTION BETWEEN NORMAL
AND PATHOLOGICAL STRESS REACTIONS
One of the major challenges for a diagnosis of ASD is
to operationally distinguish between normal and
pathological stress responses to a traumatic event. As
noted above, acute posttraumatic stress reactions are
very common and the majority of these subside within
a short timeframe. If the new aim of ASD is to identify
severe ASRs, how does one quantify severe or
pathological stress reactions? It is important to note
that the underpinning rationale of this proposed
diagnosis is not to predict longer term disorder, but
to facilitate treatment for those suffering significant
distress and whose response suggests that this distress
Review: Acute Stress Disorder
9
may be interfering sufficiently or distressing the person
excessively such that treatment may facilitate recovery.
In this sense, it may be more appropriate to describe
the ASD construct in terms of severity of ASR rather
than ‘‘psychopathology.’’ Several criteria may be
considered in operationalizing the distinction between
severe and less severe reactions. Is a requisite number
of acute symptoms associated with impairment of
functioning, as described in most DSM disorders?
This is a problematic criterion to apply to ASD because
it is often difficult to measure functioning in the weeks
after trauma, because social disruptions, environmental
upheaval, or changing medical status may limit scope
for measuring functioning accurately. Another possibility is to define the required number of symptoms in
order to identify a limited proportion of individuals. It
would be erroneous to define ASD in a liberal fashion
such that the majority of trauma survivors met criteria,
because it is well documented that the majority of
trauma survivors, even in the acute phase, adapt in the
weeks from the severe levels of distress that may be
initially experienced. Ultimately, the number of symptoms that should be required to meet an ASD diagnosis
needs to be empirically determined to ensure that (a)
only the minority of trauma survivors are captured by
the diagnosis, and (b) the symptoms are likely to
describe marked stress responses that will be assisted by
targeted intervention.
reactions? A major rationale for the inclusion of ASD is
to provide health care rebates for US citizens. At a
service delivery level, this has been identified as
important in order for many people, who suffer distress
in the acute phase after a trauma, to receive a formal
diagnosis that will allow them to receive compensable
health care services for mental health problems. The
timeframe of 1 month before PTSD may preclude
some survivors from receiving diagnosis and healthcare. In this context, we need to acknowledge that
DSM-5 is not driven simply by theoretical or scientific
priorities but also by practical issues faced by US
practitioners. In this sense, DSM-5 is primarily a
US product that at times may have less relevance to
international communities that may not share the same
health care financing problems as the United States.
A cogent argument could be made that if there was no
need for a diagnosis to facilitate access to appropriate
mental health services in the United States, the need
for the ASD diagnosis would markedly diminish.
Nonetheless, the expectation is that the proposed
definition of ASD will provide clinicians anywhere to
identify the more severely distressed survivors of
trauma who may benefit from early interventions that
have proven efficacy with ASD patients.
UTILITY OF AN ASD DIAGNOSIS
In considering the utility for the ASD diagnosis, it is
important to consider the conceptual, empirical, and
practical bases for such a diagnostic category. In terms
of conceptual issues, the ASD diagnosis has been
criticized because the major distinguishing factor ASD
and PTSD is the duration of symptoms.[4,5] One
rationale for retaining the ASD diagnosis is to formally
describe the severe posttraumatic stress that some
people experience in the initial month after trauma
exposure without labeling them as suffering PTSD.
There has justifiably been a reluctance to describe
severe stress reactions as PTSD because many cases in
the acute phase will be transient. The issue that has to
be resolved is the manner in which these reactions are
described. Distinguishing syndromes according to a
timeframe can be justified by acknowledging that
different assumptions underlie this distinction: whereas
PTSD is presumed to be a disorder that persists
beyond a timeframe when the majority of people will
experience remission of severe reactions,[6,7] ASD is
recognized as a potentially transient disorder that may
or may not develop into PTSD. Although the
argument could be made that PTSD could include an
‘‘acute’’ subtype or specifier, this could artificially
elevate the prevalence rates of PTSD.
The question may be asked: why do we need the
ASD diagnosis? If the predictive role of ASD is
abandoned, what is the need to describe severe acute
1. There is strong evidence that ASD does not identify
the majority of people who subsequently develop
PTSD, in that it is sensitive but not specific.
Accordingly, the definition of the diagnosis should
not necessarily be shaped by factors that are
predictive of PTSD. Current studies indicate that
variations of the ASD diagnosis do not adequately
address the poor predictive capacity of ASD.
Accordingly, it is useful to distinguish between the
goals of describing acute reactions from predicting
people who will develop chronic disorder. In light of
the reviewed evidence that ASD does not adequately
identify the majority of people who subsequently
develop PTSD, it is proposed that it’s predictive role
be abandoned. To that end, it is more appropriate to
conceptualize the ASD diagnosis as a means to
describe ASRs that are severe enough to warrant
clinical attention. In many health systems (especially
in the United States), receiving a diagnosis can
facilitate access to mental health services, and so the
ASD diagnosis may allow people in need of mental
health services to receive adequate care. In this
sense, ASD may be conceptualized as severe distress
in the acute phase but without the presumption that
it will develop into subsequent disorder. This can
actually be conceptualized as a benefit of this
diagnostic category.
2. An outstanding issue is the extent to which ASD
should focus on fear/anxiety/posttraumatic stress
LITERATURE-INFORMED
RECOMMENDATIONS
Depression and Anxiety
10
Bryant et al.
responses or should be broadened (in a manner
similar to ASR) to encompass the range of emotional
problems trauma survivors can experience in the
acute phase. The attraction of the latter option is
that it will be applicable to most people who suffer
anxiety, depression, grief, guilt, anger, or somatic
reactions. The problem with this option is that it
does not necessarily translate to specific treatment
options, which the current ASD formula does.
A parsimonious solution seems to describe people
in the acute phase who present with predominant
posttraumatic stress responses as ASD (and who will
then receive targeted intervention), and Adjustment
Disorder can be applied to other trauma survivors
who display other forms of distress.
3. If the goal is to describe acute anxiety reactions
following trauma, the remaining question is to
define the requisite symptoms. At present, the ASD
criteria are clustered into four categories (dissociation, re-experiencing, avoidance, and arousal), and
one needs to display reactions in each category to
meet criteria. The current data suggest that there is
considerable heterogeneity in acute responses and
being prescriptive about the specific symptoms may
be difficult and exclude those who report anxietyrelated distress in different forms. In this sense, it
needs to be recognized that, whereas chronic forms
of PTSD symptoms do tend to cluster into
recognized groupings of symptoms, this may develop
as time progresses. Accordingly, it may be more
appropriate to describe ASD in terms of people
suffering a requisite number of symptoms, but not
prescribing that they need to be from specific
clusters. For example, current data indicate that
many patients respond in the acute phase with
dissociative responses, but comparable numbers
display acute stress in the absence of dissociation.
Requiring a requisite number of symptoms may
increase identification of acute distressed people
who may benefit from treatment by acknowledging
the diversity of acute posttraumatic stress responses.
4. The remaining question is what potential symptoms
should comprise the ASD diagnosis and how many of
these should be required to meet diagnostic threshold.
Across studies, there is convergent evidence that
re-experiencing symptoms are a hallmark feature of
anxiety responses in the initial period after trauma.
This observation is consistent with fear conditioning
models of trauma response,[116] and accords with the
finding that the absence of these symptoms in the
acute phase is highly predictive of the absence of
acute or chronic anxiety.[58] Reviewing the literature,
however, indicates that re-experiencing needs to be
defined somewhat broadly because it does not always
involve repeated intrusive memories. Re-experiencing
following the trauma may include memories, nightmares, flashbacks, or psychological, somatic, or
behavioral reactivity to reminders. This is ultimately
an empirical question that requires calculating the
Depression and Anxiety
requisite symptoms reported by patients in the acute
phase to capture those patients who also display
marked distress on independent measures. There is a
need to limit overdiagnosis, insofar as it would be a
mistake to describe all stress reactions in the acute
phase as ASD when they may be short-term transient
responses that may not require mental health intervention. In this context, it is worth emphasizing that
the ASD diagnosis in DSM-V may be conceptualized
as an acute response that may be temporary and
resolve rapidly or may continue to PTSD. Unlike
many other disorders, ASD is not likely to be tested in
the context of field trials because it requires identifying people who have very recently been exposed to
traumatic events. On the other hand, there are many
datasets in the field in which researchers have assessed
ASD symptoms. A potential means to clarify the
optimal number of symptoms would be to calculate
the requisite number of symptoms that patients report
in the acute phase that optimally captures those
patients who are reporting significant distress (as
measured by independent measures of posttraumatic
stress/anxiety).
5. In terms of timeframe, it will be important in text to
highlight that clinicians need to evaluate the extent
of the disorder in relation to which the threat has
passed or if the person is still living in a highly
stressful environment (e.g., soldier in combat or
survivor of massive disaster). At this point in time, it
may be appropriate to extend the delay from 2 to 3
days to not only minimize overdiagnosis, but also to
ensure that distressed people are able to be assessed
and treated in the acute phase.
PROPOSED ASD DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA
FOR DSM-V
The rationale for the proposed diagnostic criteria is
to identify trauma survivors in the initial month
following trauma who are displaying marked fear/
anxiety responses and may benefit from mental health
services. The focus of the diagnosis is on acute
posttraumatic stress, because the disorder is conceptualized as a diagnosis of early PTSD and because there
is a strong evidence base for intervening in cases that
are characterized by fear/anxiety (rather than more
diverse emotional responses that can be described by
Adjustment Disorder).
This revision is a radical shift from the DSM-IV
definition. This shift is justified by that (a) the ASD
diagnosis was new to DSM-IV and at the time there
was very little evidence to substantiate the defining
symptoms, (b) there is now strong evidence that ASD is
not the optimal means to identify trauma survivors who
are high risk for PTSD, and (c) ASD is better
conceptualized as a description of severe ASRs. The
proposed revision will comprise the major changes to
the DSM-IV definition of ASD: (a) modifying the
Stressor definition to eliminate A2, and (b) requiring a
Review: Acute Stress Disorder
minimum number of symptoms to present, but these
do not need to be from prescribed clusters. The
specific proposal is:
1. Criterion A1 (stressor) will probably not change
substantially. The language of A1 has been revised
to emphasize that qualifying events must involve
direct exposure to actual or threatened death,
serious injury, or a threat to the physical integrity
of others. The most controversial aspect of the
DSM-IV A1 Criterion is having been ‘‘confronted
by’’ traumatic events. The proposed revision limits
such ‘‘confrontation’’ to learning about the traumatic exposure of a close friend or loved one or
learning about aversive details of unnatural deaths,
serious injury or serious assault to others. This
includes learning about the homicide of a family
member, learning about the gruesome death or
grotesque details of rape, genocide, or other
abusive violence to others.
2. Criterion A2 (subjective sense of fear, helplessness,
or horror) will be eliminated because there is little
evidence to support it.
3. B1 (sense of numbing) is largely unchanged but
moved to B5.
4. B2 (reduction in awareness) is deleted because
earlier studies indicate that it overlaps largely with
other dissociative symptoms.
5. B3 (derealization) and B4 (depersonalization) are
combined into B6 because of evidence that depersonalization and derealization are strongly overlapping.
6. B5 (dissociative amnesia) is largely unchanged and
becomes B7.
7. The re-experiencing symptoms (previously Cluster
C) are now delineated as specific symptoms in
accordance with considerable evidence concerning
ASRs. These symptoms are intrusive memories (B1),
nightmares (B2), flashbacks (B3), and psychological
distress/physiological reactivity on reminders (B4).
8. The avoidance symptoms (previously Cluster D) are
now delineated as two separate symptoms: avoidance of thoughts, feelings, and conversations (B8)
and avoidance of situations (B9). This will refine the
earlier description of ‘‘marked’’ avoidance.
9. The arousal symptoms (previously Cluster E) are
now delineated as five separate symptoms to refine
the earlier description of ‘‘marked’’ arousal: sleep
disturbance (B10), hypervigilence (B11), irritability
(B12), startle reaction (B13), and agitation (B14).
10. The major revision is that rather than requiring
endorsement of each of the four clusters, the new
criteria recognize that there is heterogeneity in
acute stress responses, and thus it requires a
specific number of symptoms without requiring
any particular symptoms to be present. At this
stage, the evidence indicates that requiring people
to satisfy specific clusters may not be appropriate
in the acute phase because current evidence that
11
satisfying the four ASD clusters is restrictive of
people satisfying acute stress.[4,7] It is premature to
definitively conclude that there should not be a
requirement that specific symptoms/clusters be
present in order to satisfy ASD diagnosis because
the available datasets have not been exhaustively
studied. There is a theoretical reason that
re-experiencing may be mandatory because fear
conditioning models posit that these symptoms
will drive all other reactions; however, this has yet
to be empirically validated across datasets.
The proposal described here is tentative and is
dependent on comprehensive analyses of at least
12 existing datasets that have collected ASD responses
from numerous countries (including datasets from the
United Kingdom, the United States, Israel, Japan, The
Netherlands, Norway, and Australia). The eventual
criteria need to be empirically validated across these
datasets to ensure that the structure and number of
symptoms is consistent across settings and accurately
identifies the minority of people with severe stress
reactions that would indicate early intervention.
At this point, it is tentatively proposed that 8 out of
the 14 symptoms be present to make an ASD diagnosis.
The tentative proposal of 8 from 14 symptoms was
derived from two steps. The 14 potential symptoms
were derived from the current list of ASD symptoms,
with the combination of several symptoms that have
been shown in earlier studies to be strongly overlapping; for example, reduced awareness was deleted
because of strong overlap with other symptoms,
and derealization was combined with depersonalization because of the theoretical and documented
overlap between these symptoms.[8] Three large-scale
datasets (one unpublished dataset from Israel and two
published datasets from the United Kingdom[9] and
Australia[10,11]) were analyzed to determine an optimal
cut-off to identify 20% of recently trauma-exposed
people. We emphasize that this decision–rule was
adopted only to calculate an approximate number of
symptoms that could be tested (and modified) in more
comprehensive analyses of more datasets. On this basis,
it was noted that 8 of the potential 14 symptoms tended
to capture approximately 20% of the samples. Accordingly, we propose that this requisite number of
symptoms should be tested across datasets.
PROPOSED ASD/PTSD SUBTYPE
OF ADJUSTMENT DISORDERS
A new ASD/PTSD subtype of adjustment disorder
has been proposed for DSM-5. It is much more specific
than other adjustment disorder subtypes and would
provide a subsyndromal diagnostic option for individuals who exhibit many posttraumatic ASD or PTSD
symptoms but who fail to meet full ASD/PTSD
diagnostic criteria. With respect to ASD, for example,
Depression and Anxiety
12
Bryant et al.
it would provide a much more specific diagnostic niche
for individuals who present with less than the requisite
eight ASD symptoms but who are clearly exhibiting a
clinically significant assortment of posttraumatic
re-experiencing, dissociative, avoidance and/or arousal
symptoms (Table 2).
The ASD/PTSD subtype will make it possible to
identify people with subsyndromal ASD (and PTSD),
TABLE 2. New diagnostic criteria for acute stress disorder for DSM-5
Criterion A. The person was exposed to one or more of the following situations
Experienced an event or events that involved a threat of death, actual or threatened serious injury, or actual or threatened physical or sexual
violation of himself or herself
Personally witnessed an event or events that involved the actual or threatened death, serious injury, or physical or sexual violation of others
Learned of such harm coming to a close relative or close friend
Or underwent repeated or extreme exposure to aversive details of unnatural death, serious injury, or serious assault or sexual violation of
others
Witnessed exposure or exposure to aversive details does not include events that are witnessed only in electronic media, television, video games, movies,
or pictures
Criterion B. Eight (or more) of the following symptoms are currently present that were not present before the traumatic event or have worsened
since
(1) A subjective sense of numbing, detachment from others, or reduced responsiveness to events that would normally elicit an emotional
response
(2) An altered sense of the reality of one’s surroundings or oneself (e.g., seeing oneself from another’s perspective, being in a daze, time slowing)
(3) Inability to remember at least one important aspect of the traumatic event that was probably encoded (i.e. not due to head injury, alcohol,
drugs)
(4) Spontaneous or cued recurrent, involuntary and intrusive distressing memories of the event
(5) Recurrent distressing dreams related to the event
(6) Dissociative reactions in which the individual feels or acts as if the traumatic event were recurring
(7) Intense or prolonged psychological distress or physiological reactivity at exposure to internal or external cues that symbolize or resemble an
aspect of the traumatic event
(8) Persistent and effortful avoidance of thoughts, conversations, or feelings that arouse recollections of the trauma
(9) Persistent and effortful avoidance of activities, places, or physical reminders that arouse recollections of the trauma
(10) Sleep disturbance (e.g., difficulty in falling asleep, restless sleep, or staying asleep)
(11) Hypervigilence
(12) Irritable, angry or aggressive behavior
(13) Exaggerated startle response
(14) Agitation or restlessness
C. Duration of the disturbance (symptoms described in Criterion B) occurs for 3 or more days and less than 1 month after the traumatic event
D. The disturbance causes clinically significant distress or impairment in social, occupational, or other important areas of functioning
E. The disturbance is not due to the direct physiological effects of a substance (e.g., medication or alcohol) or a general medical condition (e.g.,
traumatic brain injury, coma), and is not better accounted for by brief psychotic disorder
1. Shephard B. A War of Nerves: Soldiers and Psychiatrists in the Twentieth Century. London: Johnathan Cape; 2000.
2. Isserlin L, Zerach G, Solomon Z. Acute stress responses: a review and synthesis of ASD, ASR, and CSR. Am J Orthopsychiatry
2008;78:423–429.
3. The dexamethasone suppression test: an overview of its current status in psychiatry. The APA task force on laboratory tests in psychiatry. Am J
Psychiatry 1987;144:1253–1262.
4. Arana GW, Baldessarini RJ, Ornsteen M. The dexamethasone suppression test for diagnosis and prognosis in psychiatry. Commentary and
review. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1985;42:1193–1204.
5. Ozer EJ, Best SR, Lipsey TL, Weiss DS. Predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder and symptoms in adults: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull
2003;129:52–73.
6. Harvey AG, Bryant RA. Acute stress disorder: a synthesis and critique. Psychol Bull 2002;128:886–902.
7. Marshall RD, Spitzer R, Liebowitz MR. Review and critique of the new DSM-IV diagnosis of acute stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry
1999;156:1677–1685.
8. Bryant RA, Harvey AG. Acute stress disorder: a critical review of diagnostic issues. Clin Psychol Rev 1997;17:757–773.
9. Bryant RA. Early predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2003;53:789–795.
10. Harvey AG, Bryant RA. Dissociative symptoms in acute stress disorder. J Trauma Stress 1999;12:673–680.
11. Brewin CR, Andrews B, Rose S, Kirk M. Acute stress disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder in victims of violent crime. Am J Psychiatry
1999;156:360–366.
12. Bryant RA, Creamer M, O’Donnell ML, Silove D, McFarlane AC. A multisite study of the capacity of acute stress disorder diagnosis to predict
posttraumatic stress disorder. J Clin Psychiatry 2008;69:923–929.
13. Bryant RA, Creamer M, O’Donnell ML, Silove D, McFarlane AC. A multisite study of the capacity of acute stress disorder diagnosis to predict
posttraumatic stress disorder. J Clin Psychiatry 2008;e1–e7.
Depression and Anxiety
Review: Acute Stress Disorder
to assess their symptom severity using standard ASD
(or PTSD) assessment instruments, and to assess
treatment outcome utilizing interventions shown to
be effective for ASD (and PTSD).[117]
FINAL REMARKS
The proposed ASD criteria for DSM-V are tentative
at this stage. It is proposed that the tentative criteria be
tested across at least ten large-scale existing datasets
from different countries. The eventual criteria will
need to be an empirically determined set of symptoms
that distinguish severe from moderate acute anxiety
reactions, which will need to be determined by
measuring different symptom constellations against
independent measures of distress or functioning. The
possibility that a required cluster be mandatory for the
diagnosis (e.g., re-experiencing) will also need to be
tested. It is expected that by comparing the endorsement of variable numbers and structure of symptoms,
and by evaluating this response in relation to scores on
measures of distress, a criteria set will be derived that
permits identification of the minority of people in the
month after trauma that can benefit from traumafocused interventions.
REFERENCES
1. Koopman C, Classen C, Cardena E, Spiegel D. When disaster
strikes, acute stress disorder may follow. J Trauma Stress 1995;
8:29–46.
2. Spiegel D, Koopmen C, Cardena C, Classen C. Dissociative
symptoms in the diagnosis of acute stress disorder. In:
Michelson LK, Ray WJ, editors. Handbook of Dissociation.
New York: Plenum Press; 1996:367–380.
3. Bryant RA, Harvey AG. Acute stress disorder: a critical review
of diagnostic issues. Clin Psychol Rev 1997;17:757–773.
4. Koren D, Arnon I, Lavie P, Klein E. Sleep complaints as early
predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder: a 1-year prospective
study of injured survivors of motor vehicle accidents. Am J
Psychiatry 2002;159:855–857.
5. Isserlin L, Zerach G, Solomon Z. Acute stress responses: a
review and synthesis of ASD, ASR, and CSR. Am J Orthopsychiatry 2008;78:423–429.
6. Lichtenthal WG, Cruess DG, Prigerson HG. A case for
establishing complicated grief as a distinct mental disorder in
DSM-V. Clin Psychol Rev 2004;24:637–662.
7. Bonanno GA, Wortman CB, Lehman DR, et al. Resilience to
loss and chronic grief: a prospective study from preloss to
18-months postloss. J Pers Soc Psychol 2002;83:1150–1164.
8. Bryant RA, Harvey AG. Initial posttraumatic stress responses
following motor vehicle accidents. J Trauma Stress 1996;9:
223–234.
9. Shalev AY. Posttraumatic stress disorder among injured
survivors of a terrorist attack: predictive value of early intrusion
and avoidance symptoms. J Nerv Ment Dis 1992;180:
505–509.
10. Cardena E, Spiegel D. Dissociative reactions to the San
Francisco Bay Area earthquake of 1989. Am J Psychiatry
1993;150:474–478.
13
11. North CS, Smith EM, McCool RE, Lightcap PE. Acute
postdisaster coping and adjustment. J Trauma Stress 1989;
2:353–360.
12. Sloan P. Post-traumatic stress in survivors of an airplane crashlanding: a clinical and exploratory research intervention.
J Trauma Stress 1988;1:211–229.
13. Feinstein A. Posttraumatic stress disorder: a descriptive study
supporting DSM-III—R criteria. Am J Psychiatry 1989;146:
665–666.
14. World Health Organization. The ICD-10 Classification of
Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions
and Diagnostic Guidelines. 1995 ed. Geneva: World Health
Organization; 1995.
15. Marshall RD, Spitzer R, Liebowitz MR. Review and critique of
the new DSM-IV diagnosis of acute stress disorder. Am J
Psychiatry 1999;156:1677–1685.
16. Yitzhaki T, Solomon Z, Kotler M. The clinical picture of acute
combat stress reaction among Israeli soldiers in the 1982
Lebanon War. Mil Med 1991;156:193–197.
17. Shalev AY, Freedman A, Peri T, et al. Prospective study of
posttraumatic stress disorder and depression following trauma.
Am J Psychiatry 1998;155:630–637.
18. Bergiannaki JD, Psarros C, Varsou E, Paparrigopoulos T,
Soldatos CR. Protracted acute stress reaction following an
earthquake. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2003;107:18–24.
19. Soldatos CR, Paparrigopoulos TJ, Pappa DA, Christodoulou GN.
Early post-traumatic stress disorder in relation to acute stress
reaction: an ICD-10 study among help seekers following an
earthquake. Psychiatry Res 2006;143:245–253.
20. Solomon Z, Mikulincer M. Trajectories of PTSD: a 20-year
longitudinal study. Am J Psychiatry 2006;163:659–666.
21. Andreasen NC, Wasek P. Adjustment disorders in adolescents
and adults. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1980;37:1166–1170.
22. Harvey AG, Bryant RA. The relationship between acute stress
disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder: a prospective evaluation
of motor vehicle accident survivors. J Consult Clin Psychol
1998;66:507–512.
23. Bryant RA, Harvey AG. Relationship between acute stress
disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder following mild
traumatic brain injury. Am J Psychiatry 1998;155:625–629.
24. Brewin CR, Andrews B, Rose S, Kirk M. Acute stress disorder
and posttraumatic stress disorder in victims of violent crime.
Am J Psychiatry 1999;156:360–366.
25. Holeva V, Tarrier N, Wells A. Prevalence and predictors of
acute stress disorder and PTSD following road traffic accidents:
thought control strategies and social support. Behav Ther
2001;32:65–83.
26. Staab JP, Grieger TA, Fullerton CS, Ursano RJ. Acute stress
disorder, subsequent posttraumatic stress disorder and depression after a series of typhoons. Anxiety 1996;2:219–225.
27. Kangas M, Henry JL, Bryant RA. The relationship between
acute stress disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder following
cancer. J Consult Clin Psychol 2005;73:360–364.
28. Harvey AG, Bryant RA. The relationship between acute
stress disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder: a 2-year
prospective evaluation. J Consult Clin Psychol 1999;67:
985–988.
29. Harvey AG, Bryant RA. Two-year prospective evaluation of the
relationship between acute stress disorder and posttraumatic
stress disorder following mild traumatic brain injury. Am J
Psychiatry 2000;157:626–628.
30. Elklit A, Brink O. Acute stress disorder as a predictor of posttraumatic stress disorder in physical assault victims. J Interpers
Violence 2004;19:709–726.
Depression and Anxiety
14
Bryant et al.
31. Hamanaka S, Asukai N, Kamijo Y, Hatta K, Kishimoto J,
Miyaoka H. Acute stress disorder and posttraumatic stress
disorder symptoms among patients severely injured in motor
vehicle accidents in Japan. Gen Hosp Psychiatry 2006;28:
234–241.
32. Bryant RA, Creamer M, O’Donnell ML, Silove D, McFarlane AC.
A multisite study of the capacity of acute stress disorder diagnosis to
predict posttraumatic stress disorder. J Clin Psychiatry 2008;69:
923–929.
33. Ginzburg K, Solomon Z, Dekel R, Bleich A. Longitudinal study
of acute stress disorder, posttraumatic stress disorder and
dissociation following myocardial infarction. J Nerv Ment Dis
2006;194:945–950.
34. Fullerton CS, Ursano RJ, Wang LM. Acute stress disorder
posttraumatic stress disorder, and depression in disaster or
rescue workers. Am J Psychiatry 2004;161:1370–1376.
35. Murray J, Ehlers A, Mayou RA. Dissociation and posttraumatic stress disorder: two prospective studies of road traffic
accident survivors. Br J Psychiatry 2002;180:363–368.
36. Dalgleish T, Meiser-Stedman R, Kassam-Adams N, et al.
Predictive validity of acute stress disorder in children and
adolescents. Br J Psychiatry 2008;192:392–393.
37. Kassam-Adams N, Winston FK. Predicting child PTSD: the
relationship between acute stress disorder and PTSD in injured
children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2004;43:403–411.
38. Bryant RA, Salmon K, Sinclair E, Davidson P. The relationship
between acute stress disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder
in injured children. J Trauma Stress 2007;20:1075–1079.
39. Fuglsang AK, Moergeli H, Schnyder U. Does acute stress
disorder predict post-traumatic stress disorder in traffic
accident victims? Analysis of a self-report inventory. Nord J
Psychiatry 2004;58:223–229.
40. Elklit A, Christiansen D. ASD and PTSD in rape victims.
J Interpers Violence 2010, in press.
41. Kassam-Adams NFC, Winston FK. Acute stress disorder and
posttraumatic stress disorder in parents of injured children.
J Trauma Stress 2009;22:294–302.
42. Balluffi AK-AN, Kazak A, Tucker M, Dominguez T, Helfaer M.
Traumatic stress in parents of children admitted to the pediatric
intensive care unit. Pediatr Crit Care Med 2004;5:547–553.
43. Kuhn M, Ehlert U, Rumpf HJ, Backhaus J, Hohagen F,
Broocks A. Onset and maintenance of psychiatric disorders after
serious accidents. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2006;256:
497–503.
44. Bryant RA. Acute stress disorder as a predictor of posttraumatic
stress disorder: a systematic review. J Clin Psychiatry 2010, in press.
45. Bryant RA. Longitudinal psychophysiological studies of heart
rate: mediating effects and implications for treatment. Ann N Y
Acad Sci 2006;1071:19–26.
46. Bryant RA, Creamer M, O’Donnell M, Silove D, McFarlane AC.
A multisite study of initial respiration rate and heart rate as
predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder. J Clin Psychiatry
2008;69:1694–1701.
47. Ehlers A, Mayou RA, Bryant B. Cognitive predictors of
posttraumatic stress disorder in children: results of a prospective
longitudinal study. Behav Res Ther 2003;41:1–10.
48. Mayou RA, Bryant B, Ehlers A. Prediction of psychological
outcomes one year after a motor vehicle accident. Am J
Psychiatry 2001;158:1231–1238.
49. Bryant RA, Salmon K, Sinclair E, Davidson P. A prospective
study of appraisals in childhood posttraumatic stress disorder.
Behav Res Ther 2007;45:2502–2507.
50. O’Donnell ML, Creamer MC, Parslow R, et al. A predictive
screening index for posttraumatic stress disorder and depression
Depression and Anxiety
51.
52.
53.
54.
55.
56.
57.
58.
59.
60.
61.
62.
63.
64.
65.
66.
67.
68.
69.
70.
71.
following traumatic injury. J Consult Clin Psychol 2008;76:
923–932.
Spiegel D, Koopman C, Classen C. Acute stress disorder and
dissociation. Aust J Clin Exp Hypn 1994;22:11–23.
Ehlers A, Mayou RA, Bryant B. Psychological predictors of
chronic posttraumatic stress disorder after motor vehicle
accidents. J Abnorm Psychol 1998;107:508–519.
Koopman C, Classen C, Spiegel DA. Predictors of posttraumatic stress symptoms among survivors of the Oakland/
Berkeley firestorm. Am J Psychiatry 1994;151:888–894.
Ozer EJ, Best SR, Lipsey TL, Weiss DS. Predictors of
posttraumatic stress disorder and symptoms in adults: a metaanalysis. Psychol Bull 2003;129:52–73.
Shalev AY, Freedman S, Peri T, Brandes D, Sahar T. Predicting
PTSD in trauma survivors: prospective evaluation of self-report
and clinician-administered instruments. Br J Psychiatry 1997;
170:558–564.
van der Velden PG, Kleber RJ, Christiaanse B, et al. The
independent predictive value of peritraumatic dissociation for
postdisaster intrusions, avoidance reactions, and PTSD symptom severity: a 4-year prospective study. J Trauma Stress 2006;
19:493–506.
Breh DC, Seidler GH. Is peritraumatic dissociation a risk factor
for PTSD? J Trauma Dissociation 2007;8:53–69.
Harvey AG, Bryant RA. Acute stress disorder: a synthesis and
critique. Psychol Bull 2002;128:886–902.
Bryant RA. Early predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder.
Biol Psychiatry 2003;53:789–795.
Sterlini GL, Bryant RA. Hyperarousal and dissociation: a study
of novice skydivers. Behav Res Ther 2002;40:431–437.
Bryant RA. Does dissociation further our understanding of
PTSD? J Anxiety Disord 2007;21:183–191.
Butler LD, Duran REF, Jasiukaitis P, et al. Hypnotizability and
traumatic experience: a diathesis-stress model of dissociative
symptomatology. Am J Psychiatry 1996;153:42–63.
Kihlstrom JF, Glisky ML, Angiulo MJ. Dissociative tendencies
and dissociative disorders. J Abnorm Psychol 1994;103:
117–124.
Bryant RA, Guthrie RM, Moulds ML. Hypnotizability in acute
stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2001;158:600–604.
Spiegel D, Cardena E. Disintegrated experience: the dissociative disorders revisited. J Abnorm Psychol 1991;100:366–378.
Brewin CR, Andrews B, Valentine JD. Meta-analysis of risk
factors for posttraumatic stress disorder in trauma-exposed
adults. J Consult Clin Psychol 2000;68:748–766.
Keane TM, Kaufman ML, Kimble MO. Peritraumatic dissociative symptoms, acute stress disorder, and the development
of posttraumatic stress disorder: causation, correlation or
epiphenomena. In: Sanchez-Planell L, Diez-Quevedo C,
editors. Dissociative States. Barcelona: Springer; 2001:21–43.
Marmar CR, Weiss DS, Metzler TJ. The peritraumatic
dissociative experiences questionnaire. In: Wilson JP, Keane TM,
editors. Assessing Psychological Trauma and PTSD. New York:
Guilford Press; 1997:412–428.
Fikretoglu D, Brunet A, Best S, et al. The relationship between
peritraumatic distress and peritraumatic dissociation. J Nerv
Ment Dis 2006;194:853–858.
Fikretoglu D, Brunet A, Best SR, et al. Peritraumatic
fear, helplessness and horror and peritraumatic dissociation:
do physical and cognitive symptoms of panic mediate the
relationship between the two? Behav Res Ther 2007;45:
39–47.
Foa EB, Kozak MJ. Emotional processing of fear: exposure to
corrective information. Psychol Bull 1986;99:20–35.
Review: Acute Stress Disorder
72. Panasetis P, Bryant RA. Peritraumatic versus persistent dissociation in acute stress disorder. J Trauma Stress 2003;16:
563–566.
73. Briere J, Scott C, Weathers F. Peritraumatic and persistent
dissociation in the presumed etiology of PTSD. Am J
Psychiatry 2005;162:2295–2301.
74. Noyes Jr R, Kletti R. Depersonilization in response to lifethreatening danger. Compr Psychiatry 1977;18:375–384.
75. Simeon D, Kozin DS, Segal K, Lerch B, Dujour R,
Giesbrecht T. De-constructing depersonalization: further evidence for symptom clusters. Psychiatry Res 2008;157:303–306.
76. Harvey AG, Bryant RA. Dissociative symptoms in acute stress
disorder. J Trauma Stress 1999;12:673–680.
77. Brooks R, Bryant RA, Silove D, et al. The latent structure of the
Peritraumatic Dissociative Experiences Questionnaire. J Trauma
Stress 2009;22:153–157.
78. Bryant RA, Moulds ML, Guthrie RM, Nixon RD. The additive
benefit of hypnosis and cognitive-behavioral therapy in treating
acute stress disorder. J Consult Clin Psychol 2005;73:334–340.
79. Bryant RA, Moulds ML, Nixon RV. Cognitive behaviour
therapy of acute stress disorder: a four-year follow-up. Behav
Res Ther 2003;41:489–494.
80. Bryant RA, Sackville T, Dang ST, Moulds M, Guthrie R.
Treating acute stress disorder: an evaluation of cognitive
behavior therapy and supporting counselling techniques. Am J
Psychiatry 1999;156:1780–1786.
81. Bryant RA, Harvey AG, Dang ST, Sackville T, Basten C.
Treatment of acute stress disorder: a comparison of cognitivebehavioral therapy and supportive counseling. J Consult Clin
Psychol 1998;66:862–866.
82. Bryant RA, Mastrodomenico J, Felmingham KL, et al. Treatment of acute stress disorder: a randomized controlled trial.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2008;65:659–667.
83. Bryant RA, Moulds M, Guthrie R, Nixon RD. Treating acute
stress disorder following mild traumatic brain injury. Am J
Psychiatry 2003;160:585–587.
84. Bisson JI, Shepherd JP, Joy D, Probert R, Newcombe RG. Early
cognitive-behavioural therapy for post-traumatic stress symptoms after physical injury. Randomised controlled trial. Br J
Psychiatry 2004;184:63–69.
85. Sijbrandij M, Olff M, Reitsma JB, Carlier IVE, de Vries MH,
Gersons BPR. Treatment of acute posttraumatic stress disorder
with brief cognitive behavioral therapy: a randomized controlled trial. Am J Psychiatry 2007;164:82–90.
86. Foa EB, Zoellner LA, Feeny NC. An evaluation of three brief
programs for facilitating recovery after assault. J Trauma Stress
2006;19:29–43.
87. Brewin CR, Andrews B, Rose S. Fear, helplessness, and
horror in posttraumatic stress disorder: investigating DSM-IV
criterion A2 in victims of violent crime. J Trauma Stress 2000;13:
499–509.
88. O’Donnell ML, Creamer M, McFarlane A, Silove D, Bryant RA.
Should A2 be a diagnostic requirement for posttraumatic stress
disorder in DSM-V? Psychiatry Res 2010;176:257–260.
89. Creamer MC, McFarlane AC, Burgess P. Psychopathology
following trauma: the role of subjective experience. J Affect
Disord 2005;86:175–182.
90. Roemer L, Orsillo SM, Borkovec TD, Litz BT. Emotional
response at the time of a potentially traumatizing event and
PTSD symptomatology: a preliminary retrospective analysis of
the DSM-IV criterion A-2. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry
1998;29:123–130.
91. Kilpatrick DG, Resnick HS, Freedy JR, et al. The posttraumatic
stress disorder field trial: evaluation of the PTSD construct: criteria
92.
93.
94.
95.
96.
97.
98.
99.
100.
101.
102.
103.
104.
105.
106.
107.
108.
109.
110.
15
A through E. In: Widiger TA, Frances AJ, Pincus HA, First MB,
Ross R, Davis W, editors. DSM-IV Sourcebook (Volume IV).
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press; 1997.
Schnurr PP, Spiro A, Vielhauer MJ, Findler MN, Hamblen JL.
Trauma in the lives of older men: findings from the normative
aging study. J Clin Geropsychol 2002;8:175–187.
Vaiva G, Brunet A, Lebigot F, et al. Fright (effroi) and other
peritraumatic responses after a serious motor vehicle accident:
prospective influence on acute PTSD development. Can J
Psychiatry 2003;48:395–401.
Bracha HS, Williams AE, Haynes SN, Kubany ES, Ralston TC,
Yamashita JM. The STRS (shortness of breath, tremulousness,
racing heart, and sweating): a brief checklist for acute distress
with panic-like autonomic indicators; development and factor
structure. Ann Gen Hosp Psychiatry 2004;3:8.
Mezey G, Evans C, Hobdell K. Families of homicide victims:
Psychiatric responses and help-seeking. Psychol Psychother
Theory Res Pract 2002;75:65–75.
Adler AB, Wright KM, Bliese PD, Eckford R, Hoge CW. A2
diagnostic criterion for combat-related posttraumatic stress
disorder. J Trauma Stress 2008;21:301–308.
Breslau N, Kessler RC. The stressor criterion in DSM-IV
posttraumatic stress disorder: an empirical investigation. Biol
Psychiatry 2001;50:699–704.
Galea S, Resnick H, Ahern J, et al. Posttraumatic stress disorder
in Manhattan, New York City, after the September 11th
terrorist attacks. J Urban Health 2002;79:340–353.
Classen C, Koopman C, Hales R, Spiegel D. Acute stress
disorder as a predictor of posttraumatic stress symptoms. Am J
Psychiatry 1998;155:620–624.
Creamer M, O’Donnell ML, Pattison P. The relationship between
acute stress disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder in severely
injured trauma survivors. Behav Res Ther 2004;42:315–328.
Bryant RA, Creamer M, O’Donnell ML, Silove D, McFarlane AC.
A multisite study of the capacity of acute stress disorder
diagnosis to predict posttraumatic stress disorder. J Clin
Psychiatry 2008;69:e1–e7.
Schnyder U, Wittmann L, Friedrich-Perez J, Hepp U,
Moergeli H. Posttraumatic stress disorder following accidental
injury: rule or exception in Switzerland? Psychother Psychosom
2008;77:111–118.
Douglas AN. Racial and ethnic differences in dissociation: an
examination of the dissociative experiences scale in a nonclinical
population. J Trauma Dissociation 2009;10:24–37.
Seligman R, Kirmayer LJ. Dissociative experience and cultural
neuroscience: narrative, metaphor and mechanism. Cult Med
Psychiatry 2008;32:31–64.
Seligman R, Kirmayer LJ. Distress, dissociation, and embodied
experience: reconsidering the pathways to mediumship and
mental health. Ethos 2005;33:71–79.
Sierra M, Gomez J, Molina JJ, Luque R, Munoz JF, David AS.
Depersonalization in psychiatric patients: a transcultural study.
J Nerv Ment Dis 2006;194:356–361.
Mezzich JE, Raab ES. Depressive symptomatology across the
Americas. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1980;37:818–823.
Lowe ED. Identity, activity, and the well-being of adolescents
and youths: lessons from youg people in a Micronesia society.
Cult Med Psychiatry 2003;27:187–219.
Nixon RD, Bryant RA. Peritraumatic and persistent panic attacks
in acute stress disorder. Behav Res Ther 2003;41:1237–1242.
Lewis-Fernandez R, Guarnaccia PJ, Martinez IE, Salman E,
Schmidt A, Liebowitz M. Comparative phenomenology of
ataques de nervios, panic attacks, and panic disorder. Cult Med
Psychiatry 2002;26:199–223.
Depression and Anxiety
16
Bryant et al.
111. Hinton DE, Chhean D, Pich V, Hofmann SG, Barlow DH.
Tinnitus among Cambodian refugees: relationship to PTSD
severity. J Trauma Stress 2006;19:541–546.
112. Hinton DE, Chhean D, Pich V, Um K, Fama JM, Pollack MH.
Neck-focused panic attacks among Cambodian refugees; a
logistic and linear regression analysis. J Anxiety Disord
2006;20:119–138.
113. Wikan U. Cross-cultural features of women’s place in society.
Genus 1990;46:1–16.
114. Wellencamp J. Notions of grief and catharsis among the Toraja.
Am Ethnol 1988;15:486–500.
Depression and Anxiety
115. de Jong K, Mulhern M, Ford N, van der Kam S, Kleber R. The
trauma of war in Sierra Leone. Lancet 2000;355:2067–2068.
116. Rauch SL, Drevets WC. Neuroimaging and neuroanatomy of
stress-induced and fear circuitry disorders. In: Andrews G,
Charney DS, Sirovatka PJ, Regier DA, editors. Stress-Induced
and Fear Circuitry Disorders: Refining the Research Agenda for
DSM-V. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association;
2009:215–254.
117. Friedman MJ. PTSD, ASD & AD: Looking Towards DSM-5.
Atlanta: Presented at American Psychiatric Association’s Annual
Meeting; 2010.
VOLUME 24/ NO. 1 • ISSN: 1050 -1835 • 2013
Research Quarterly
a d va n c i n g s c i e n c e a n d p r o m o t i n g u n d e r s t a n d i n g o f t r a u m a t i c s t r e s s
Published by:
National Center for PTSD
VA Medical Center (116D)
215 North Main Street
White River Junction
Vermont 05009-0001 USA
(802) 296-5132
FAX (802) 296-5135
Email: ncptsd@va.gov
All issues of the PTSD Research
Quarterly are available online at:
www.ptsd.va.gov
Editorial Members:
Editorial Director
Matthew J. Friedman, MD, PhD
Scientific Editor
Fran H. Norris, PhD
Managing Editor
Heather Smith, BA Ed
National Center Divisions:
Executive
White River Jct VT
Behavioral Science
Boston MA
Dissemination and Training
Menlo Park CA
Clinical Neurosciences
West Haven CT
Evaluation
West Haven CT
Pacific Islands
Honolulu HI
Women’s Health Sciences
Boston MA
An Update of
Acute Stress Disorder
Richard A. Bryant, PhD
School of Psychology
University of New South Wales
It is nearly 20 years since DSM-IV introduced the
diagnosis of acute stress disorder (ASD). At the time
there was relatively little research substantiating the
role or definition of diagnosis, but its introduction
promoted an unprecedented amount of research
into acute stress reactions after trauma. As we
approach the introduction of DSM-5, it is timely to
review what we now know about acute traumatic
stress and how this is influencing the new definition
of the ASD diagnosis.
overall the ASD diagnosis is sensitive in predicting
PTSD; that is, the majority of individuals with a
diagnosis of ASD do subsequently develop PTSD
(Bryant, 2011). In contrast, the ASD diagnosis has
low specificity; that is, most people who eventually
experience PTSD do not initially display ASD. These
studies suggest that although trauma survivors with
ASD do experience stress reactions that are likely
to persist, the current criteria is overly narrow in
identifying people at risk.
ASD was initially introduced for two reasons:
(a) to describe severe acute stress reactions that
occur in the initial month after a trauma that could
not be described as PTSD, which can only be
diagnosed after a month has transpired since the
trauma; and (b) to identify acutely traumatized
people who will subsequently develop PTSD as
opposed to experiencing a transient stress reaction
(Spiegel et al., 1996). To satisfy criteria for an ASD
diagnosis in DSM-IV, one needed to experience a
traumatic event and respond with fear, horror, or
helplessness (Criterion A), and similarly to PTSD,
needed to satisfy re-experiencing (Criterion C),
avoidance (Criterion D), and arousal (Criterion E)
symptom clusters. ASD was markedly differentiated
from PTSD by a strong emphasis on acute
dissociation, such that one needed to have at least
three of the following symptoms: emotional
numbing, derealization, depersonalization, reduced
awareness of surroundings, or dissociative amnesia
(Criterion B). This emphasis on dissociation was
based largely on the perspective that dissociative
responses to trauma are pivotal in longer-term
psychopathology (Harvey and Bryant, 2002).
What is the reason for the poor predictive ability of
ASD? Some studies suggest that the emphasis on
dissociation may be overly limiting because many
people at risk may not display acute dissociative
responses (Bryant, Creamer, O’Donnell, Silove,
and McFarlane, 2008; Dalgleish et al., 2008; Harvey
and Bryant, 1998; Kassam-Adams and Winston,
2004). This finding is consistent with other evidence
concerning the relationship between acute
dissociation and subsequent PTSD. Although there
are numerous studies attesting to the predictive
capacity of acute dissociation and subsequent
PTSD (Murray, Ehlers, and Mayou, 2002), other
analyses suggest that peritraumatic dissociation
is not an independent predictor of PTSD (Breh and
Seidler, 2007; van der Velden et al., 2006). One study
found that dissociation mediates the relationship
between acute arousal and subsequent PTSD
(Bryant, Brooks, et al., 2011), suggesting that it may
be the arousal rather than dissociation at the time of
trauma that is pivotal in PTSD development.
Longitudinal Evidence
for Acute Stress Disorder
A series of longitudinal studies have assessed the
relationship between ASD and subsequent PTSD.
One recent review of 22 studies concluded that
There are only a few studies of the relationship
between ASD and PTSD in children (Bryant, Salmon,
Sinclair, and Davidson, 2007; Dalgleish et al., 2008;
Kassam-Adams and Winston, 2004). Although limited
by the small number of studies, and the focus of all
of them on traumatic injury, these studies indicate
that the ASD diagnosis has poor capacity to predict
PTSD in injured children. We know much less about
the trajectories of trauma response in children than
U.S. Department of Veterans Affairs
Continued on page 2
Author’s Address: Richard A. Bryant, PhD is affiliated with the School of Psychology, University of New South Wales
Sydney, NSW, Australia, 2052. Email Address: r.bryant@unsw.edu.au.
Continued from cover
we do in adults, but it is possible that children experience distinctive
trajectories of posttraumatic adjustment as a result of developmental
and environmental factors, and so the definition of ASD may be less
successful in predicting PTSD.
Arguably the more compelling reason why ASD is not accurately
predicting longer-term PTSD is that evidence has now accrued that
the relationship between acute trauma response and longer-term
PTSD is complex and non-linear. One recent large longitudinal study
that assessed more than 1,000 traumatic injury survivors at four time
points after injury found that people followed complex courses over
time; only half of those who met PTSD at any time point satisfied
PTSD criteria at subsequent time points (Bryant, O’Donnell, Creamer,
McFarlane, and Silove, in press). A more sensitive approach has
been to use latent growth mixture modeling (LGMM), which classifies
groups in a population to identify classes of individual variation over
time, thereby allowing distinct trajectories to be identified over time
after trauma. Several studies have noted four major trajectories
following traumatic experiences: (a) resilient class with few PTSD
symptoms, (b) recovery class with initial distress then gradual
remission over time, (c) delayed reaction class with initially low
symptom levels but increasing symptoms over time, and (d) chronic
distress with consistently high PTSD levels. These trajectories have
been documented in survivors of traumatic injury, Severe Acute
Respiratory Syndrome (SARS) infection, women diagnosed with
breast cancer, and military personnel deployed to the Middle East.
These complex and often fluctuating courses of posttraumatic
adjustment represent a marked challenge for any attempt to use
acute symptoms as a marker for trauma survivors who will develop
chronic PTSD: for a review, see Bonanno and Mancini, 2012.
As a result of this accumulating evidence, DSM-5 has markedly
modified the goals and criteria for ASD. The diagnosis no longer
attempts to predict chronic PTSD, but rather identifies those
survivors who are suffering severe acute stress reactions in the
period prior to when a diagnosis of PTSD can be made (i.e., 1 month).
In recognition of the heterogeneity of acute stress responses and the
finding that dissociation may be overemphasized in DSM-IV, the new
definition now requires at least 9 out of possible 14 symptoms
without regard to any particular clusters (Bryant, Friedman, Spiegel,
Ursano, and Strain, 2011). That is, trauma survivors can experience
a range of traumatic stress responses that may or may not include
dissociative symptoms. It is important to note that a major driver of
the ASD diagnosis is that in many regions, especially in the U.S.,
receiving a formal diagnosis can facilitate reimbursed health care
and this is an important consideration in ensuring that the diagnosis
can effectively describe the most distressed people before a
diagnosis of PTSD can be implemented.
The diagnosis requires some form of re-experiencing and/or
avoidance, which maintains the relevance of the treatment protocols
that have been validated with the DSM-IV definition of ASD.
Numerous trials have found that exposure-based cognitive behavior
therapy reduces levels of subsequent PTSD symptoms relative to
other counseling techniques (Bryant, Mastrodomenico et al., 2008;
Shalev et al., 2012). Thus screening trauma survivors with the DSM-5
definition of ASD should continue to offer benefits because cognitive
behavior therapy techniques directly target the fundamental
re-experiencing and avoidance problems.
PAGE 2
Assessment of Acute Stress Disorder
Although the definition of ASD has changed, the existing measures
of ASD are still useful because the symptom composition is largely
unchanged. Scoring of the measures would need to be modified
because the new definition no longer requires that each cluster of
symptoms are satisfied; instead, each measure would need to
determine the presence of ASD on the basis of 9 symptoms being
present. There are three measures of ASD to choose from. The first
measure developed for ASD was the Stanford Acute Stress Reaction
Questionnaire, which is a self-report measure that indexes 30 possible
symptoms of ASD (Cardena, Koopman, Classen, Waelde, and
Spiegel, 2000). An alternate measure is the Acute Stress Disorder
Interview (Bryant, Harvey, Dang, and Sackville, 1998), which possesses
good sensitivity (92%), and specificity (93%) relative to independent
clinical diagnosis. The Acute Stress Disorder Scale (Bryant, Moulds,
and Guthrie, 2000) is a self-report version of the Acute Stress
Disorder Interview, which also has sound psychometric properties.
Alternate Options for Assessing
High-risk Trauma Survivors
The modest predictive accuracy of the ASD diagnosis raises questions
concerning alternate means to identify recently traumatized people
who will progress to PTSD. In short, we currently have little knowledge
in terms of acute markers that we can rely on to predict who will
develop the disorder. In light of the previously reviewed complexity
of the trajectories that we see following trauma, it is perhaps not
surprising that acute markers do not perform very well as predictors.
This qualification notwithstanding, several acute biological markers
have been shown to have a statistical relationship with longer-term
PTSD severity. These include elevated resting heart rate, elevated
respiration rate, elevated cortisol, low Gamma-Amino Butyric Acid
(GABA) plasma levels,...
Purchase answer to see full
attachment