reading review

User Generated

Zvn007

Business Finance

Description

After reading ModelingResidentSpendingBehavior, discuss how the authors presented their study, from the questions and hypotheses to the findings.

Identify and discuss

  1. Research method
  2. Data collection 

Unformatted Attachment Preview

Journal of Sport Management, 2018, 32, 473-485 https://doi.org/10.1123/jsm.2017-0207 © 2018 Human Kinetics, Inc. ARTICLE Modeling Resident Spending Behavior During Sport Events: Do Residents Contribute to Economic Impact? Nola Agha University of San Francisco Marijke Taks University of Ottawa The role of residents in the calculation of economic impact remains a point of contention. It is unclear if changes in resident spending caused by an event contribute positively, negatively, or not at all. Building on previous theory, we develop a comprehensive model that explains all 72 possible behaviors of residents based on changes in (a) spending, (b) multiplier, (c) timing of expenditures, and (d) geographic location of spending. Applying the model to Super Bowl 50 indicates that few residents were affected and positive and negative effects were relatively equivalent; thus, their overall impact is negligible. This leaves practitioners the option to engage in the challenging process of gathering data on all four variables on all residents or to revert back to the old model of entirely excluding residents from economic impact. From a theoretical perspective, there is a pressing need to properly conceptualize the time variable in economic impact studies. Keywords: cost benefit analysis, crowding out, mega event, mega sport event Positive economic impacts of large scale sport events, as well as the methods for measuring economic impact have come under scrutiny (e.g., Késenne, 2012). Nevertheless, sport event managers, local organizations, and public authorities still rely on economic impact studies to justify the public spending which is often required to cover the high cost of organizing events. Therefore, it is imperative that they can rely on trustworthy economic impact studies. One question that continues to arise is whether residents should be included in economic impact. To answer this question, we do not conduct an economic impact study. Instead, we examine the theory behind economic impact, build a model, and apply it to a large event to answer a critical question about the methods currently used to conduct these studies. Traditionally, resident spending was not considered in the calculation of economic impact (Crompton, 1995; Getz, 1991). Over time, researchers identified, categorized, and labeled exceptions to this rule and excluded or included residents if they were home stayers, runaways, changers, or exhibited other forms of nontraditional behavior (e.g., Coates & Depken, 2009; Cobb & Olberding, 2007; Preuss, 2005). These previous studies mainly focused on event-affected residents who were surveyed at events. Yet, as Matheson and Baade (2006) stated, “A basic shortcoming of typical economic impact studies, in general, pertains not to information on spending by those included in a direct expenditure survey, but rather to the lack of information on the spending behavior for those who are not” (p. 356). In other words, we have some understanding of the role event-affected residents play in the calculation of economic impact (e.g., Kwiatkowski, 2016), but we do not understand the role of residents that are not involved in the event but may still be affected by it. Previous research has indeed indicated that the residents who do not engage with the event may change their spending by staying home (e.g., Coates & Depken, 2009), going out, or otherwise altering their behaviors (e.g., Crompton & Howard, 2013, Preuss, 2005; Taks, Girginov, & Boucher, 2006). Ultimately, the current conceptualizations of “other” types of residents are incomplete. There is no model that encompasses the universe of possible behavioral and spending changes incurred by residents who are affected by events (e.g., disrupted, stimulated, diverted), which then lead to either a positive, negative, or neutral charge to the total calculation of economic impact. Therefore, the primary purpose of this article is to develop a model that explains all of the possible ways residents’ changes in behavior can affect impact. Furthermore, primary data collected during a Type B event (Gratton & Taylor, 2000) are used to illustrate an application of the model and to determine if the overall effect of residents is positive, negative, or neutral. Type B events are defined as “Major spectator events generating significant economic activity, media interest and part of an annual cycle of sport events” (Gratton & Taylor, 2000, p. 190). This contribution clarifies a major point of contention, namely whether or not to include resident spending in economic impact studies based on a direct expenditure approach (DEA; Davies, Coleman, & Ramchandani, 2013) or a cost benefit analysis approach (CBA; Késenne, 2012; Taks, Késenne, Chalip, Green, & Martyn, 2011). The question of whether and how residents affect economic impact is also important for event managers, local organizations, and public authorities, who continually read, conduct, and evaluate survey-based economic impact studies and use them as a currency to justify their public spending. Residents in Economic Impact Studies Agha is with the University of San Francisco, San Francisco, CA. Taks is with the School of Human Kinetics, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada. Address author correspondence to Nola Agha at nagha@usfca.edu. Economic impact studies based on a DEA engage in a series of steps to measure new spending in a local economy due to an event. 473 474 Agha and Taks Table 1 Previous Categories of Residents Affected by Events Previous Cases Description Economic Outcome Variables Affected by the Event Home stayers (Preuss, 2005) Staycation (Getz, 1991) Vacationing at home (Cobb & Olberding, 2007) Residents forgo a vacation to stay in the region to partake in the event Expenditures are an economic benefit as they would not have occurred locally without the event Geographic location of the spending Timing Leavers (Crompton & Howard, 2013) Runaways (Preuss, 2005) Skedaddle effect (Coates & Depken, 2009) Residents specifically leave the area to avoid the event Expenditures are out of the area and generate an economic cost Geographic location of the spending Changers (Preuss, 2005) Residents replace a preexisting vacation with one that allows them to avoid the event No cost or benefit to the region as the vacation would have occurred regardless Timing This includes surveying spectators and/or participants and asking a series of questions regarding their status as a visitor or as a local resident, how much they spent, how long they are visiting, etc. Traditional methods excluded all residents’ spending due to the idea that these expenditures would occur locally regardless of the presence of an event and that these expenditures simply substitute for others (e.g., Crompton, 1995). Over time, ad hoc attempts identified the ways residents might have a nonneutral effect on impact (see Table 1) and authors, such as Gelan (2003), advocated for including resident spending, although the mistake in this approach is that it only analyzed residents who were event spectators, not all residents. CBA, on the other hand, is based on welfare economics and views each dollar as a cost or benefit and results in a calculation of net economic benefits (Késenne, 2012). In a framework developed by Agha and Taks (2015), residents have the ability to add to impact (e.g., residents tapping into their savings because of the event) or to take away from it (e.g., event is crowding out residents or crowding out local businesses). A CBA has some similarities to a DEA in that it includes a survey-based approach to measure specific gains (or losses) to the local economy because a researcher must know how locals are changing their behaviors and spending to determine if it generates a benefit or a cost. Table 1 provides an overview of research that has labeled various categories of residents that should be accounted for in economic impact studies. The types of changes in behaviors described in these studies are rather limited, and various names have been given to describe the same behavior. Note that the conceptualizations of resident impact in Table 1 define eventrelated changes, or shifts, in two different variables, the timing (changers and home stayers) and geographic location of the money spent (home stayers and runaways). These studies do not take into consideration possible shifts in the amount or businesses where the money is spent. In the “Model Development” section, we propose a model using four variables that captures many more possible ways in which residents may shift their spending behaviors because of an event. In doing so, we demonstrate that these previous categorizations are insufficient to capture the real economic outcome of changes in resident spending behavior due to event hosting. Model Development a reallocation of funds and do not generate benefits or costs (Table 2). From this simplistic definition, we see the amount spent and the geographic location of the spending are the first of four necessary variables that need to be considered. We also see that economic impact implicitly considers two cases that capture a shift—the actual spending and the alternate spending that would have occurred without the event. In Table 2, these two variables and the two cases are interacted: the geographic area where the money is normally spent in the absence of the event (i.e., “origination of expenditures”) and where the money is actually spent because of the event (i.e., to the “location of expenditures”). The third variable that needs attention is the business industry in which spending occurred to derive a multiplier that will estimate the indirect and induced effects of the initial spending (Crompton, 1995). The fourth variable, the timing of the expenditure, is not nearly as straight forward as the first three. This is due to competing frameworks, limited research in this area, and ad hoc operationalization of the variable. For instance, there is no consensus which time frame should be considered. In the context of the Olympics, tourism spending can change several years before an event (Solberg & Preuss, 2007). With no sense of time scale, direct survey questions ask, “Did you ‘reduce spending in the past’ or ‘reduce spending in the future,’ or will you ‘respend at a later date’” although questions designed to find home stayers ask, “Did you forgo another vacation (trip) in order to attend the (event)?” (Preuss, Kurscheidt, & Schütte, 2009). In short, there is very little alignment between theoretical models (conceptualizations) of time shifted expenditures and the survey questions that are designed to identify those shifts. Our solution is three pronged: (a) we acknowledge time as a theoretical necessity in calculating economic impact and build it into our model, (b) we rely on current survey-based questions to identify time-shifted behaviors (home stayers and Table 2 Economic Impact Location of Expenditures Origination of expenditures Outside the area Economic impact is new spending in a local economy less any expenditures that have left the local economy due to the event in question. At a basic level, expenditures made locally from residents who would have otherwise made those expenditures are considered Inside the area Outside the Area Inside the Area Not related to economic impact Economic cost Economic benefit Zero economic impact Note. Adapted from “The economic impact of visitors at major multi-sport events,” by H. Preuss, 2005, European Sport Management Quarterly, 5, pp. 281–301. JSM Vol. 32, No. 5, 2018 Modeling Resident Spending During Sport Events changers) and test those against an improved four variable conceptualization of shifted spending, and (c) we collect qualitative data on time shifting to see if it aligns with multiple-choice questions. In summary, there are four necessary variables related to economic impact: the amount spent, the geographic location of spending, the business industry in which it was spent (to derive the multiplier), and the timing of the expenditure. To calculate impact, one must capture the shift in these variables. For example, a resident can spend more, less, or the same amount of money in the presence of an event in the host region. A resident can spend within the geographic area of impact or outside of it. Spending can shift to a business with a higher multiplier, same multiplier, or lower multiplier. Finally, spending can occur as normally planned or a resident may shift the timing of their expenditures to before or after the event.1 Given these four variables and their associated shifts, Table 3 illustrates that there are 18 potential behaviors for the case in which a resident intended to, and did, spend their money locally (i.e., In-In). For example, an event can cause a resident to spend the same amount but at a lower multiplier business, which will have a negative effect on impact. An event can cause a resident to spend more at a higher multiplier business, which will lead to economic benefits regardless of whether the spending is time shifted or not. From Tables 2 and 3, it is apparent that the geographic variable has multiple dimensions, which generates four distinct cases for residents: (a) In-In: Spending that would have been spent in the area of impact and stayed in the area (the cases illustrated in Table 3). (b) In-Out: Spending that would have been spent in the area of impact but shifted out. All of these 18 cases are negative and are analogous to the economic cost in Table 2. Runaways would be classified here but so would a resident who intended to go to the local golf course, but it was booked for a preevent tournament and instead drove to a nearby course that was outside of the area of impact. (c) Out-In: Spending that would have been spent outside the region but shifted in. These 18 cases are all positive and correspond to the economic benefit in Table 2. A home stayer is a special case of this. (d) Out-Out: Spending that would have been outside the region and stayed outside the region. These 18 cases are not related to economic impact. They include changers, but also residents who planned to take a day trip to visit family in a nearby metropolitan area and did, in fact, take that planned trip. The outcomes based on shifts in geography related to In-Out, Out-In, and Out-Out are straight forward, but the shifts in behaviors within the specific area, In-In, need to be accurately modeled and estimated. Moreover, the cases in Table 3 illustrate three important points. (a) Economic impact can be affected regardless if residents are positively or negatively affected by the event (e.g., a resident spending more than usual on public transit to attend the event, or a resident forced to spend more on public transit because of event-related traffic). As illustrated here, it is possible, although not necessary, for positive and negative engagement to have the same effect (in this case, higher spending on public transportation). (b) The changes in behavior can begin with any of the four variables. For example, a resident may stay away from downtown because of traffic (geography), a resident may 475 Table 3 Theoretical Model of Resident Effects on Economic Impact Spending Multiplier Time Shift Geography In-In In-Out Out-In Out-Out More Higher Same Lower Same Higher Same Lower Less Higher Same Lower Yes No Yes No Yes No Yes No Yes No Yes No Yes No Yes No Yes No + + 0 + ? ? + + 0 0 − − ? ? 0 − − − − − − − − − − − − − − − − − − − − − + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + Note. In-In is spending that would have occurred within the area of impact and did, in fact, occur in the area of impact. In-Out is spending that would have occurred within the area of impact but instead shifted out of the area because of the event. Out-In is spending that would have occurred outside the area of impact but instead shifted into the area because of the event. Out-Out is spending that would have occurred outside of the area of impact and did, in fact, occur outside the area of impact. + means positive effect, − means negative effect, 0 means no effect, and blank cells in Out-Out indicate that these resident spending cases are irrelevant because they do not relate to economic impact. purchase tickets for the event (spending), a resident may go grocery shopping on Thursday to avoid weekend crowds (timing), or a resident may buy lunch from a grocery store instead of going to her normal deli for lunch (business). (c) Regardless of where it begins, the initial disruption can (but does not necessarily have to) affect the other three variables. We provide two examples of how multiple variables can be affected and illustrate which variables shift. For instance, a resident may purchase tickets for the event, which could be more than she normally spends on entertainment and in a different location. She makes up for it by not going out for movies the following week. This is a shift in location, timing, business multiplier, and spending. Or, a resident may go grocery shopping on Thursday instead of his normal Sunday shopping, but he spends more because he went to a different store in a different city (within the area of impact) on his drive home from work. This is a shift in timing and spending, but the location and business multiplier are the same. Of the 18 possible behaviors for the In-In group, five outcomes are positive, five are negative, four are neutral (no effect), and four are indeterminate. The cases of higher spending at lower multiplier businesses and lower spending at higher multiplier businesses make it clear that all four variables are necessary to calculate the final impact as these are indeterminate, ex ante, and require the actual values on a case by case basis to determine their effect. JSM Vol. 32, No. 5, 2018 476 Agha and Taks Based on the framework presented in Table 3, we can see there are 72 possible behavioral combinations of which 22 have no effect on economic impact, 23 are positive, 23 are negative, and four are indeterminate. Thus, to determine the overall impact of residents we must know (a) the shift in spending, business, time, and geography; and (b) the proportion of residents in each of these categories. Because mega events generally assume a large area of impact, we hypothesize most resident geographic spending shifts will be within the host region (i.e., In-In) and thus subject to the variable impacts presented in Table 3. Study Context Super Bowl 50, the 2016 championship game for the National Football League (NFL), was hosted in the San Francisco Bay area and allowed for applying the model to different groups of residents from three distinct geographical perspectives (see Figure 1). The Super Bowl is traditionally the largest 1-day sporting event in the United States in terms of viewership with 111.9 million viewers in 2016 (Nielsen, 2016). The Super Bowl 50 Host Committee defined the area of impact (i.e., geography variable) as the 6,900 square mile, nine-county San Francisco Bay Area (population 7.15 million). This nine-county Bay Area as whole was the first geographic area we delineate for this study. The region is comprised of three major cities, San Francisco (population 805,235), Oakland (population 390,724), and San Jose (population 945,942), and 98 smaller municipalities (Bay Area Census, 2016). Host Committee consultants reported 1.9 million residents and 300,000 out-of-area visitors attended a Super Bowl related event (Repucom, 2016) although the game itself was played in front of only 70,000 fans at Levi’s Stadium in Santa Clara. Seven miles away from Levi’s Stadium, the city of San Jose hosted several community events as well as the NFL Opening Night at SAP Center where 7,000 fans paid to watch the media interview players (Davidson, 2016). Santa Clara County, which includes the cities of San Jose and Santa Clara, was the second geographic area taken into consideration. San Francisco, 45 miles away from Levi’s Stadium, hosted the majority of the lodging and hundreds of hospitality events located primarily in the downtown central business district. Two main fan festivals were also in San Francisco. Super Bowl City was a free, Figure 1 — Maps of the study area. 9-day fan festival featuring 64 free performances with attendance estimates ranging from 5,000 per day (Lee, 2016) to a total of 900,000 (“Super Bowl,” 2016). The NFL Experience was a paid fan experience that reported 150,000 attendees over 9 days (Controller’s Office, 2016) and was located at the Moscone Center; ticket prices ranged from $25 to $60. Super Bowl City was located above the region’s busiest public transit station and required the closure of over 14 streets, the rerouting of 20 bus lines, and the closure of one streetcar line for a total of 21 days (SFMTA, 2016). The NFL Experience was located less than a mile away from Super Bowl City, and both events were located within one mile of all six of San Francisco’s Fortune 500 companies, two of which were asked to have their employees work from home during Super Bowl City (Raymos, 2016). Similarly, the city of San Francisco encouraged residents to, “Work remotely, stagger your work hours or take that vacation you deserve” (SFMTA, 2016).2 In short, the bulk of the activity and disruption (nongame related events) were held in a small portion of downtown San Francisco, for multiple days. We expected the vast majority of resident disruption to be related to San Francisco and not Santa Clara where the game was played. San Francisco County was the third geographic area under consideration. Super Bowl 50 was somewhat different from other Super Bowls in that it was geographically dispersed in a warm weather city with a developed public transportation system. On the other hand, Super Bowl 50 is highly comparable with other Super Bowls and with many other large events on other event features such as high levels of security, a multitude of hospitality events, crowding out of nonevent visitors (it occurred during Chinese New Year), fan events, the closing of the central business district, altering public transportation, residents asked to stay home, geographic dispersion (similar to the Olympics or the Commonwealth Games), thousands of visitors, public subsidies, etc. Thus, Super Bowl 50 has event features that make it generalizable to many other Type B events (Gratton & Taylor, 2000) throughout the world. Method Survey Instrument We used a survey method (using the Qualtrics platform, Provo, UT) similar to traditional DEA impact studies to collect the data on residents’ spending behavior. The survey was pretested multiple times to develop the clearest questions for respondents. We integrated the standard questions to identify the three categories of home stayers, changers, and runaways and added questions to capture all of the 72 categories. A screening question first identified whether the respondent was a Bay Area resident and then recorded their zip code. NonBay Area residents were exited from the survey. The zip code determined to which of the three geographic areas the resident belonged. Next, a series of questions determined whether the respondent was aware of the Super Bowl, if they were attending, and if so, how much they were spending on the event. Similarly, respondents were asked if they were aware of the fan-related events, if they were attending, and if so, how much they were spending. The following section captured spending information. Specifically, respondents were asked to record their actual spending all day “yesterday” using three variables: the amount of spending, the business, and the city in which each expenditure took place. Next, they were asked if the Super Bowl or its related events caused them JSM Vol. 32, No. 5, 2018 Modeling Resident Spending During Sport Events to change the amount and/or location of their spending yesterday. If they believed their spending yesterday was affected by the Super Bowl they recorded the amount, the business, and the city of each of what their expenditures would have been in the absence of the event, allowing for the capture of changes in dollar amounts spent, multipliers, and geography. In imagining what their behaviors would have been in the absence of the event, respondents relied or either known or hypothetical information. For example, in some cases, the alternate activity was known (traffic was terrible so someone took public transportation instead of Uber), sometimes it was partially known (someone planned to go out to dinner but the restaurant was near a busy event zone so they stayed home and ate dinner—the cost of staying home exists but is generally not acknowledged), and was sometimes completely unknown and responses were hypothetical (someone definitely spent money to visit a fan festival but without it they honestly did not know what they would have done or how much they would have spent that Saturday afternoon). To determine timing changes, all respondents were asked the screening question, “Because of the Super Bowl or its related events, was your total spending amount yesterday: the same as usual, more than usual, or less than usual.” The three responses were randomized so as not to lead respondents in any direction. If a respondent spent more or less than usual, they were asked the amount and then received a follow-up question on the timing of their expenditures. Those who spent more than usual were asked the source of their additional funds: their savings, borrowed money (e.g., a credit card), reducing spending in the past, or reducing spending in the future. Those who spent less than usual were asked if they would respend at a later date in the Bay Area, respend at a later date outside the Bay Area, or save. To identify home stayers, runaways, and changers, all respondents (not just those who had indicated they were affected) were asked if they were taking a vacation away from the Bay Area between January 30 and February 7. Those who responded positively were asked follow-up questions to determine if they were leaving because of the Super Bowl (runaways) or foregoing another vacation at a different time to take a vacation during the Super Bowl (changers). In addition, they were asked if they were renting out their home on Airbnb or a similar service and how much they were earning. Those who responded they were not taking a vacation during the Super Bowl period were asked if they were foregoing a vacation (at a different time) to stay and attend the Super Bowl or its associated events (home stayers). All respondents were asked if they lived or worked near four primary event zones: Moscone Center in San Francisco, Justin Herman Plaza/Ferry Building in San Francisco, Levi’s Stadium in Santa Clara, and SAP Center in San Jose. Demographic questions included gender identity, age (in years), and annual household income (15 categories). To capture a respondent’s attitude toward the Super Bowl, data were collected with the Sport Involvement Inventory based on Shank and Beasley (1998), using a seven-point, eight-item sematic differential (e.g., boring vs. exciting, uninteresting vs. interesting, etc.). Finally, respondents were asked to share any additional comments regarding the amount, timing, and location of their spending changes due to the Super Bowl and its associated events. Data Collection and Participants When survey populations are geographically and demographically diverse Yun and Trumbo (2000) recommend using 477 multimode techniques to improve sample representativeness. For that reason, we collected data throughout the 6,900 square mile area in a variety of ways. First, 32 graduate students enrolled in a research methods course were enlisted to probabilistically sample (Jones, 2015) the nine-county Bay Area in person for the 9 days surrounding the Super Bowl by intercepting subjects at six similar, predetermined locations in each county (public transit station, low-price-point grocery store, high-price-point grocery store, coffee shop, laundromat, and a strip mall or a busy shopping street in a big city). To achieve a geographically stratified sample, counties were sampled in approximation to their overall portion of the Bay Area population (see the percentages in Figure 1). Second, given the complexity of surveying such a geographically distributed area, during the 9-day collection period, 33 graduate students enrolled in a different research methods class distributed the survey online through 33 different, hyperlocal, digital news publications that covered news related to Super Bowl 50. Third, to counter concern that we would oversample residents who were familiar with or who were interested in the event, we targeted residents in the Bay Area who would take our questionnaire when shared on various convenient digital platforms. Finally, a sampling concern was related to obtaining accurate spending information given that the survey only captured a single day of behavior and given that respondents are often hesitant to respond to questions about money (Swan & Epley, 1981). To address these concerns, we utilized a longitudinal convenience panel (also stratified by county population) that was tracked over the 9-day data collection period using an electronic diary method known to provide more accurate data than onsite surveying (Breen, Bull, & Walo, 2001). Each panel respondent received the full online survey on the first day and a shortened online version that collected only spending information on subsequent days. There were 1,227 surveys taken of which only 790 were completed.3 A further 151 surveys were discarded because the reported spending information (in dollar amounts) was not complete or was not in alignment with the follow-up question of whether they spent more, the same, or less. Table 4 indicates the descriptive statistics of the samples for each geographic sample—the ninecounty Bay Area with respondents from all samples (n = 572),4 San Francisco County (n = 127), and Santa Clara County (n = 141) in terms of gender, household income, attitude, age, and awareness of the event. The samples were highly representative of the region regarding age and income distribution (https:// censusreporter.org/profiles/). At a 95% confidence level, the confidence intervals were 4.1% for the Bay Area sample, 8.7% for San Francisco, and 8.3% for Santa Clara (Griffiths, Hill, & Judge, 1993). Coding and Data Analysis The survey collected shift data for the time variable through multiple choice questions. For the spending, business, and geographic location variables, actual and alternate values were collected to generate the shifts. First, the location of the business was translated into a county code, and business variables were assigned an output multiplier5 from IMPLAN6 based on the appropriate industry.7 Next, variables were coded into their respective shifts; spending more, less, or the same was determined by evaluating actual and alternate spending; a business multiplier that was higher, lower, or the same was determined by the actual and alternate businesses; spending that was In-In, In-Out, Out-In, or Out-Out was determined by evaluating the county codes for the location of JSM Vol. 32, No. 5, 2018 478 Agha and Taks Table 4 Descriptive Statistics Bay Area San Francisco Santa Clara (n = 572) (n = 127) (n = 141) Gender Male 45% 54% 57% Female 55% 46% 43% Household income under $50,000 15% 16% 12% $50,000–$99,999 30% 27% 39% $100,000–$149,999 22% 23% 23% $150,000+ 32% 34% 26% Average attitude 4.0 3.1 4.3 (7 is highest) Average age 43.4 38.7 42.3 Awareness Aware of Super Bowl 96% 98% 99% Attended Super Bowl 1% 1% 2% Average spent to $1,300 (n = 5) $3,500 (n = 1) $1,000 (n = 2) attend Super Bowla Aware of fan festivals 90% 98% 88% Attended fan festivals 22% 35% 11% Average spent to $39.07 $28.52 $29.62 attend fan festivalsa a Average based on those who attended only. the actual and alternate spending. All of this coding was performed separately for the three different areas of impact, and observations were assigned to these areas according to their zip code. Results We present the results for each of the geographic areas of interest, for individual variables, two variables, three variables, and all four variables. Spending Depending on the geography, 76–84% reported their spending amounts to be unaffected by the Super Bowl (see Table 5). Of the remainder, more respondents reported spending more than spending less except in San Francisco. The average amount spent more and spent less varied across geographic samples. Business Overall, the vast majority of respondents spent at businesses with the same multiplier (see Table 5). Although the average multiplier in the geographic samples was roughly the same, the shifts in multipliers were negative in the Bay Area and Santa Clara samples. The positive change in multiplier in San Francisco stems from the behavioral shift toward public transportation which has one of the highest multipliers. Time Residents classified as changers are those whose behaviors have shifted on only the dimension of time. The results show that they are less than 1% of the Bay Area population. Similarly, in the openended feedback, very few respondents reported shifting the timing of their expenditures on the day in question or on other days during the Super Bowl period (see the lines labeled “Qualitative” in Table 5). The most common descriptions of time shifts were for residents changing the timing of doctor appointments and other meetings to avoid traffic and crowds. In the overall framework of economic impact, changing the timing of an appointment is an example of a behavioral disruption that begins with time (and does not affect amount, business, or geography), whereas traditionally, the timing variable is intended to capture deeper shifts in expenditure timing (e.g., spending more now on tickets to events and less later on local leisure consumption). The qualitative feedback listing only these time-disrupted activities begs the questions of whether the right questions are being asked and if people are even capable of truly answering time shifting questions. Certainly, people have a general sense that they are being affected by these different variables, but it is not clear that the data can capture these shifts. Geography The majority of residents in all geographic areas spent their money within the area of impact and had intended to do so regardless of the event (Table 5). In addition, In-Out and Out-In behavior is quite uncommon. Open-ended comments revealed that residents perceived their spending to shift away from the area more often than reported in their spending data. Generally, more people reported shifting spending away from event-related areas than toward them. Spending × Business Whereas the values in Table 5 reflect the difference of how much more or less was spent, the values hereafter represent the more precise case of the actual spending times the multiplier less the alternate spending times a multiplier for all reported transactions, and then averaged across respondents. The interaction of the spending shift and the multiplier shift in Table 6 reinforce the results from the spending section. The most important finding is the overall negative effect in San Francisco where more people are spending less than spending more and the amount less is greater than the amount more. In the other areas, the reverse is true, and a greater number of residents are spending more. Note that when less is spent in higher multiplier businesses, the overall effect remains negative. Given the much higher spending values in the spent more category, we point to research showing that we remember larger expenditures (Neter & Waksberg, 1964), whereas the alternate case, spending less, is both hypothetical and harder to remember. This suggests that the spent less values may be underestimated. Spending × Time A single question was asked to identify if those who spent more, time shifted their behaviors. Of those who spent more than normal, the proportion of time shifting ranged from 50% in the Bay Area sample to 22% in the San Francisco sample (see Table 6). These time-shifted expenditures should be disregarded for economic impact (unless the multiplier of the business is taken into account, however, as can be seen from the multiplier analysis (Table 5) there is no substantial shift in the actual and JSM Vol. 32, No. 5, 2018 Modeling Resident Spending During Sport Events Table 5 479 Single Variable Shifts Bay Area n Spending Spent more (%) Reported how much more ($)a Spent same (%) Spent less (%) Perception how much less ($)a Business Higher multiplier industry (%) Same multiplier industry (%) Lower multiplier industry (%) Average actual multiplierb Average alternate multiplierc Average difference of actual − alternate multiplier Time Changers Qualitative: shifted time this day Qualitative: shifted time a different day Geographyd In-In In-Out Out-In Out-Out Runaways (a specific form of In-Out) 58 San Francisco n Santa Clara n 10.39% $93.21 84.23% 5.38% −$30.08 11 11 93 18 18 9.02% $62.39 76.23% 14.75% −$63.07 22 22 105 11 11 15.94% $48.18 76.09% 7.97% −$32.73 19 489 18 485 72 526 3.32% 85.49% 3.15% 1.550 1.560 −0.001 8 98 8 112 25 114 6.30% 77.17% 6.30% 1.550 1.533 0.008 3 115 7 109 27 125 2.13% 81.56% 4.96% 1.518 1.554 −0.004 3 1 7 0.52% 0.17% 1.22% 2 0 1 1.57% 0.00% 0.79% 0 0 3 0.00% 0.00% 2.13% 554 1 0 30 8 96.85% 0.17% 0.00% 5.25% 1.40% 109 0 1 27 7 85.83% 0.00% 0.79% 21.26% 5.51% 114 8 7 17 3 80.85% 5.67% 4.97% 12.06% 2.13% 470 30 Note. The sum of the n’s may be lower than the total n’s due to missing values. a Average. bThis reflects the respondents who actually had transactions “yesterday.” Some reported zero spending “yesterday” and thus have no actual multiplier. cThis reflects respondents whose business locations shifted and is comprised of some who had actual spending the prior day and some who had zero spending the prior day. dThe sum of percentages can be more than 100% because a single respondent can have multiple types of geographically shifted expenditures on a single day. alternate multiplier). In contemplating the reliability of these results, we do wonder, do respondents really know what their time shifts will be? Given that spending more in the past or future requires pondering one’s budget and expenditures, we are not confident that the majority of people know or track this kind of behavior. Spending × Geography Table 6 indicates that adding geographical shifts to net changes in Spending × Multiplier provides different estimates than in Table 5, which reports only spending. In the geographic samples, the amount spent less and the amount spent more have higher average values once geographic shifts are considered. We use this to highlight the importance of including all variables because an estimate of economic impact based on spending without geographic location would have been incorrect as the In-Out and Out-Out expenditures would have been erroneously included as positive gain in the calculations. Time × Geography The effect of home stayers on impact is positive, but there are more runaways (negative) in Table 5 than home stayers (Table 6). Note, however, that the numbers are low confirming that these behaviors do not greatly impact shifts in economic impact. Spending × Business × Geography The interaction of three variables in Table 7 paints a different picture than does the analysis of one or two variables. Both In-Outs and OutIns are negligible in size perhaps suggesting that surveyors should not spend time capturing runaways and home stayers or other forms of geographic shifts. Santa Clara County does appear to have more InOuts and Out-Ins than the other areas. Note that the average In-Out shift of $106.70 in Santa Clara County is a loss as this money was shifted out of the area. On the other hand, Out-Ins represent money that was shifted in and generate a positive impact. In the case of Santa Clara County, the Out-Ins spent $17.19 less than usual, but this was still a gain to the county (without time shifting) and is still positive, as expected. Spending × Time × Geography Of those who spent less than normal, a single question determined their time and geographic shifts. Few plan to respend later outside the Bay Area suggesting that reductions in spending are mostly retained locally (Table 7). Spending × Business × Time × Geography When time shifting is included to analyze all four variables simultaneously, the total responses drop because time shifting does not matter for those whose spending behavior was the JSM Vol. 32, No. 5, 2018 480 Agha and Taks Table 6 Net Change in Average Spending × Multiplier for Two Variable Shifts Bay Area n Spending × Businessa Spent more Higher multiplier business Same multiplier business Lower multiplier business Spent same Higher multiplier business Same multiplier business Lower multiplier business Spent less Higher multiplier business Same multiplier business Lower multiplier business Average for full sample Spending × Time Because you spent more than normal did you Reduce spending past Reduce spending future Spending × Geographyb In-In Spent more Spent same Spent less In-Out Spent more Spent same Spent less Out-In Spent more Spent same Spent less Out-Out Spent more Spent same Spent less Time × Geography Home stayers (a specific form of Out-In) San Francisco n 15 10 15 $134.12 $34.94 $239.05 4 0 3 0 470 0 $0.00 0 93 0 4 9 3 558 −$37.72 −$31.87 −$5.35 $11.58 63 7 24 11.11% 38.10% 540 56 454 30 1 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 28 1 27 0 $11.90 $139.68 $0.00 −$46.56 $30.63 $30.63 5 0.87% $0.16 $4.60 $0.00 Santa Clara n $64.02 3 6 7 $92.38 $47.92 $98.17 $0.00 0 105 0 $0.00 4 5 5 122 −$63.46 −$45.57 −$28.15 −$6.59 0 4 0 138 9 1 1 11.11% 11.11% 21 3 5 $50.16 104 10 76 18 0 0 0 0 1 1 0 0 26 2 24 0 −$9.41 $77.13 $0.00 −$97.22 1 0.79% $3.72 $3.72 $9.69 $126.00 $0.00 −$36.38 $7.68 14.29% 23.81% 112 14 94 4 6 6 0 0 7 1 0 6 17 1 14 2 $6.02 $63.02 $0.00 −$59.62 $106.70 $106.70 −$28.32 −$7.81 $43.39 $0.00 −$88.08 1 0.71% −$17.19 $49.59 a The sum of the n’s may be lower than the total n’s due to missing values. bn’s are different from the geography results in Table 4 because some cases of missing spending or missing multipliers. We remind readers that in the calculation of overall impact In-Out values are negative and Out-Out are irrelevant, as in Table 3. same (which ranged from 59% to 83% of the samples in Table 8). Only respondents who reported spending more or less provided information on their time shifting behaviors and of these respondents, few answered the time shifting question. From this, we derive two important conclusions. First, in Table 8, only a small portion of residents engage in behavior that leads to a change in impact. In the case of the entire Bay Area, only 0.2% of respondents engaged in In-Out behavior and 7% engaged in In-In behavior that affected impact. In San Francisco, the geographic area most impacted by the event, the results are similar: 0.8% were Out-In and 11.2% were In-In with behaviors that affected impact. Second, when the fourth variable for time is brought in, the average values for each type of behavior shift from the values in Spending × Business × Geography analysis in Table 7. This further reinforces the point that any estimate of economic impact performed with values from one, two, or three variables will be incorrect. Discussion Using the model based on shifts in four spending dimensions, we found In-In residents exhibited all 18 forms of behavioral shifts in JSM Vol. 32, No. 5, 2018 Modeling Resident Spending During Sport Events Table 7 481 Net Change in Average Spending × Multiplier for Three Variable Shifts Bay Area San Francisco n Spending × Business × Geography In-In Spent more Higher multiplier business Same multiplier business Lower multiplier business Spent same Higher multiplier business Same multiplier business Lower multiplier business Spent less Higher multiplier business Same multiplier business Lower multiplier business In-Out Out-In Out-Out Spending × Time × Geography Because you spent less than normal will you Respend later in the Bay Area Respend later outside the Bay Area n Santa Clara n 540 $11.90 104 −$9.41 112 $6.02 15 9 14 $134.12 $35.42 $255.80 4 0 3 $64.02 1 4 3 $71.99 $49.68 $83.11 $0.00 0 76 0 0 94 0 $0.00 0 454 0 4 9 3 1 0 28 26 8 1 −$37.72 −$31.87 −$5.35 $30.63 $0.16 4 5 5 0 1 26 30.77% 3.85% 18 3 1 $50.16 $0.00 −$63.46 −$45.57 −$28.15 $3.72 $9.69 0 2 0 6 7 17 $106.70 −$17.19 −$7.81 16.67% 5.56% 8 1 1 12.50% 12.50% −$45.96 Note. Spending and multiplier shifts are truncated for In-Out, Out-In, and Out-Out. We remind readers that in the calculation of overall impact In-Out values are negative and Out-Out values are irrelevant, as in Table 3. spending, multiplier, and time. On the other hand, we found In-Out and Out-In behavior to be exceedingly rare, except in the case of Santa Clara County. We find this unsurprising since the county is a smaller subset of a major metropolitan area whose county lines are indistinguishable in the physical landscape leading to higher rates of cross-border transactions.8 The application of the model to an event allowed us to demonstrate that using one, two, or three variables resulted in incorrect estimates of resident impact. Moreover, we illustrated that respondents were unwilling or unable to answer questions on time shifts and qualitative responses indicated that respondents viewed time shifting differently (short term) than academic conceptualizations of the variable (to pre-event or post-event periods). Finally, the model included four indeterminate categories of In-In residents (in Table 3). We found the multiplier effect was not stronger than the spending shift in the cases of higher spending + lower multiplier or lower spending + higher multiplier. Thus, although these situations are hypothetically indeterminate, the model can be simplified by assuming that the higher spending + lower multiplier has a positive effect and the lower spending + higher multiplier has a negative effect. This means that of the 72 possible behavioral combinations, 22 have no effect on economic impact, 25 are positive, and 25 are negative. Are Resident Effects Positive, Negative, or Neutral? To determine if the overall effect of residents is positive, negative, or neutral, we note that the value of impact is a function of the definition of the area of impact. In this exercise, we looked at three geographic areas: the entire metropolitan area, the county where the game was hosted, and the county where the vast majority of the Super Bowl week activities were held. Those counties, Santa Clara and San Francisco, saw the highest percentages of residents who were affected and who reported shifts in behaviors. San Francisco had more residents spend less than more. The decline in spending from those spending less was larger in magnitude than the increased spending from those spending more, resulting in an overall net decrease. Santa Clara saw an increase in In-Out behavior of residents shifting their spending outside the county; thus, resident behavior decreased economic activity. As illustrated in Table 9, we found San Francisco, the event area with the most disruption and activity, to be most negatively affected. Note that the three areas under investigation represent three different event size and city size contexts (e.g., Agha & Taks, 2015): a multiday event concentrated in a central business district (San Francisco County), a single day event in a suburban city (Santa Clara County), and an annual, week-long mega event in a large metropolitan area (Bay Area). From this perspective, a large event in a small area of impact had a more negative impact (10% of San Francisco residents leading to negative impact) compared with a large event in a large area of impact (2% of Bay Area residents leading to negative impact). Although the purpose of this research is not to conduct an economic impact analysis, the natural inclination of a researcher is to extrapolate the values in Table 8 to the entire area to generate an overall effect of residents. This would be incorrect because if we applied the percentages in Table 9 to the entire population, we would be suggesting that every person (including babies, children, seniors, and the unemployed) engaged in these spending behaviors, clearly leading to an overestimation. Using the number JSM Vol. 32, No. 5, 2018 482 Agha and Taks Table 8 Outcome of Resident Effects on Economic Impact Samples Theoretical Outcome Bay Area (n = 528) Geography Spending Multiplier Time Shift In-In More Higher Yes No Yes No Yes No Yes No Yes No Yes No Yes + + 0 + ? ? + + 0 0 − − ? 1.1% 1.2% 0.7% 0.5% 0.9% 1.1% 0.0% 0.0% 79.4% 0.0% 0.0% 0.0% 0.4% No ? 0.4% Yes 0 0.4% No − 0.9% Yes − 0.0% No − − + 0.5% 0.2% 0.0% Same Lower Same Higher Same Lower Less Higher Same Lower In-Out Out-In All cases All cases Out-Out All cases 4.9% Table 9 Percentages of Sample Leading to Positive, Negative, and Neutral Effects Effect on Impact Bay Area San Francisco Santa Clara Positive Neutral Negative Not related to impact 5% 87% 2% 5% 3% 65% 10% 22% 11% 80% 7% 2% $174.53 $128.93 $35.21 $51.66 $93.82 $481.90 $0.00 − $26.44 − $49.00 − $15.32 − $41.91 San Francisco (n = 118) 0.0% 0.8% 0.0% 0.0% 0.8% 0.0% 0.0% 0.0% 59.8% 0.0% 0.0% 0.0% 0.0% $85.21 $0.00 0.0% 7.0% 0.7% 2.1% 0.7% 0.7% 0.0% 0.0% 67.4% 0.0% 0.0% 0.0% 0.0% 3.2% −$63.46 0.0% 0.8% −$7.66 0.7% 2.4% − $196.53 − $118.11 −$5.65 0.7% 0.8% −$5.35 $30.63 $114.51 Santa Clara (n = 121) 3.2% 0% 0.8% $3.72 20.5% $71.99 $43.76 $51.66 $144.40 $79.09 $0.00 − $22.98 − $68.93 0.0% 0.0% 5.7% 5.0% $106.47 − $25.94 1.4% the sports economics literature. Possible explanations for the negative or neutral effects of the Super Bowl and other large events (e.g., Baade, Baumann, & Matheson, 2008; Matheson & Baade, 2006) are crowding out of both visitors and locals. The neutralizing behaviors of residents confirm that these nonpositive ex post results are unlikely to derive from local crowding out of residents. Limitations and Future Research of households in each geographic region could lead to similar inaccurate results because a single household could include one member who spent more, one who spent less, and one who was unaffected. Even without an exact value for resident impact for this event, the results from the application of the model clearly support the proposition that some local residents are crowded out during an event (Késenne, 2012). We also found evidence of retained expenditures. Most importantly, we found that they are roughly equivalent with slight differences based on the area of impact, essentially neutralizing the overall impact. Although our research is framed around the DEA and CBA survey-based approaches to impact, the quantitative results indicate important implications for the ex post approach that is common in Although it is theoretically possible and conceptually simple to gather information on all residents to compute an impact, Wilton and Nickerson (2006) agree “the actual collection of such information is extremely difficult” (p. 17). For instance, capturing all shifts in all four variables was very challenging, and we acknowledge that there are known imperfections in collecting survey data (Ritchie, 1984). Despite testing multiple variations of our instrument, there were several indications that it did not precisely capture all behaviors. For example, the open-ended qualitative responses indicated that respondents are better able to remember or identify higher expenditures despite a perception of a shift to lower spending. We also found evidence that humans are hesitant to share information pertaining to money (Furnham & Argyle, 1998). Capturing residents’ actual activity on a previous day, as well as any activity that was different from what would have occurred, JSM Vol. 32, No. 5, 2018 Modeling Resident Spending During Sport Events allowed us to identify intertemporal effects. There was a high nonresponse rate when asking respondents if changes in spending (reduced or increased) is at the benefit/expense of the past or future or if they have saved/spent or plan to respend or save the money in the future. Respondents struggled to know, understand, or properly evaluate time shifting behavior. There is an important need for future research on time shifting—clearly defining it, deciding what time period matters, and finding ways to ask appropriate questions, so respondents can both understand and correctly answer. Journaling expenses over a certain period of time could be an alternative way to capture this. To gather the required information with a large enough number of responses, we used a variety of data collection techniques. Based on the number of Super Bowl game attendees, it appears we oversampled people who purchased Super Bowl tickets. Based solely on the definition of runaways and hunkerdowns, these residents were not physically present in the region or were at home. To overcome this inherent difficulty in sampling a resident who is not present, it was necessary to utilize online sampling (to reach those at home) and a lengthy data collection period (9 days) to capture some runaways before they left. In both cases, it is still possible we undersampled, which relates back to the concern of Matheson and Baade (2006) that to calculate the most precise estimate of resident impact with survey techniques, it is necessary to sample residents who are not physically present. 483 Third, in the case of this Type B event, changes in residents’ spending behavior had a negligible effect on impact although it varied between positive and negative depending on the area of impact. Thus, practitioners have the option to engage in the challenging process of gathering data on all four variables on all residents (including those who do not attend the event) or to revert back to the old model of entirely excluding residents from economic impact (e.g., Crompton, 1995, 2006; Wilton & Nickerson, 2006). The findings from the case of the Super Bowl that the gains and losses are roughly equivalent in all three geographic areas suggest that studies would result in a relatively small error in the overall impact estimation when entirely excluding residents from the calculation of economic impact. However, it is advised that researchers apply the model to other events to determine if these relative equivalencies hold true for multiple event types, especially given the recent focus on smaller events and impact (e.g., Agha & Taks, 2015; Rascher & Goldman, 2015). Either way, sport event managers, local organizations, and public authorities need to accurately understand the implications of including or excluding residents in the calculations. Notes 1 Whereas a shift in amount, business, or geography will have an impact, a shift in just timing of residents does not have to have an impact if the other three variables are constant. 2 Conclusions To date, the largest problem in including residents in impact has been that researchers have named, and thus attempted to capture through surveys, only a few of the possible behaviors of residents. To solve this problem, we utilized the core principles of economic impact to build a model with four variables that captured all 72 possible ways residents can affect impact. Next, the model was applied, and primary data were collected in the context of Super Bowl 50 to determine the extent to which residents’ spending was affected by the event. We analyzed shifts in their spending behavior because of the event (in the four variables spending, business, time, and geography) but also asked what their behavior would have been in the absence of the event. We found support for the model in determining the effect of changes in resident spending on economic impact for any event and highlight three findings. First, economic impact studies capturing only a few categories of residents (such as home stayers or runaways) using only one, two, or three variables are incomplete, resulting in incorrect estimates of resident impact. Second, we have illustrated that what must be done (gathering data on four variables from residents who are mostly not at an event) is extremely challenging because of the nature of the data being collected (sometimes hypothetical and the reluctance to share monetary information), nearly always cost prohibitive (because of the necessity to find respondents who are geographically dispersed and not in attendance at the event), and researchers have yet to develop a sufficient method to gather the required information for one of the variables (time). Although time is a core variable in economic impact (e.g., historically measured through visitors as time switchers or casuals and through residents as runaways or home stayers), it has been poorly operationalized by academics, and it is very difficult for survey respondents to report accurately. There is a pressing need for considerable academic attention on this aspect of economic impact. Reduced productivity is an important issue in large scale events. Mills and Rosentraub (2013) examine this issue in detail. Our method focuses on the DEA and as such we do not investigate indirect costs. 3 Of the 437 incomplete surveys, 50% exited the survey once they reached the questions about individual spending data which reinforces our statement about the difficulty in collecting spending information. An additional 28% were not located in the nine-county Bay Area and were thus not our targeted sample. The remaining 22% opened the survey but answered zero questions. These reasons for elimination do not raise concerns for a nonresponse bias. 4 Although there were 639 useable responses in the nine-county Bay Area sample, we oversampled in San Francisco and thus used a random number generator to drop 55 responses from San Francisco so that the Bay Area sample achieved the objective stratified sample resulting in 572. 5 We agree with Crompton (1995) that income multipliers are more useful for a resident to understand the true value of an event to their personal gain or to their elected leaders to make policy decisions to fund events (Crompton, 2006). This study seeks to accomplish neither of these. We analyze how residents shift their spending between industries. The sales, or output multipliers, allow us to calculate the economic impact of this shift (e.g., negative economic impact if a resident shifts behavior from a business with a higher multiplier to a business with a lower multiplier). 6 IMPLAN (http://implan.com/company/) is one of three companies that provide multipliers based on input-output tables from the U.S. Department of Commerce. It is a common tool used in U.S.-based economic impact. See Davies et al. (2013) for more information on input-output and other methods of impact estimation. 7 Of the over 400 industries tracked by IMPLAN, respondents spent in 44 different industries ranging from auto repair to wineries. For the indirect and induced effects, the mean = 0.62 (SD = 0.23), minimum = 0.27 (gasoline stations), and maximum = 1.55 (state and local government passenger transit). The only other industry with a multiplier over one is performing arts companies. JSM Vol. 32, No. 5, 2018 484 Agha and Taks 8 Although San Francisco is also part of the major metropolitan area, it is surrounded on three sides by water and bridges, making individual expenditures in adjacent areas less common. Manhattan is likely an analogous region. Although there are common flows of business goods and services in the region, residents are less likely to leave the area to make purchases. References Agha, N., & Taks, M.A. (2015). A theoretical comparison of the economic impact of large and small events. International Journal of Sport Finance, 10(3), 199–216. Baade, R.A., Baumann, R.W., & Matheson, V.A. (2008). Selling the game: Estimating the economic impact of professional sports through taxable sales. Southern Economic Journal, 74(3), 794–810. Bay Area Census. (2016). Retrieved from http://www.bayareacensus. ca.gov/ Breen, H., Bull, A., & Walo, M. (2001). A comparison of survey methods to estimate visitor expenditure at a local event. Tourism Management, 22(5), 473–479. doi:10.1016/S0261-5177(01)00005-X Coates, D., & Depken, C.A. (2009). The impact of college football games on local sales tax revenue: Evidence from four cities in Texas. Eastern Economic Journal, 35(4), 531–547. doi:10.1057/eej.2009.29 Cobb, S., & Olberding, D. (2007). The importance of import substitution in marathon economic impact analysis. International Journal of Sport Finance, 2(2), 108–118. Controller’s Office. (2016, May 9). Super Bowl 50: City budget impact report. Office of the Controller, City and County of San Francisco. Retrieved from http://sfcontroller.org/sites/default/files/SB%2050% 20May%209%202016.pdf Crompton, J.L. (1995). Economic impact analysis of sports facilities and events: Eleven sources of misapplication. Journal of Sport Management, 9(1), 14–35. doi:10.1123/jsm.9.1.14 Crompton, J.L. (2006). Economic impact studies: Instruments for political shenanigans? Journal of Travel Research, 45, 67–82. doi:10.1177/ 0047287506288870 Crompton, L.J., & Howard, D.R. (2013). Costs: The rest of the economic impact story. Journal of Sport Management, 27(5), 379–392. doi:10. 1123/jsm.27.5.379 Davidson, J. (2016, February 1). Super Bowl opening night features players, media and characters. The Sacramento Bee. Retrieved from http://www. sacbee.com/sports/nfl/super-bowl/article57855723.html Davies, L., Coleman, R., & Ramchandani, G. (2013). Evaluating event economic impact: Rigour versus reality? International Journal of Event and Festival Management, 4(1), 31–42. doi:10.1108/ 17582951311307494 Furnham, A., & Argyle, M. (1998). The psychology of money. New York, NY: Routledge. Gelan, A. (2003). Local economic impacts: The British Open. Annals of Tourism Research, 30(2), 406–425. doi:10.1016/S0160-7383(02) 00098-1 Getz, D. (1991). Festivals, special events, and tourism. New York, NY: Van Nostrand Reinhold. Gratton, C., & Taylor, P. (2000). Economics of sport and recreation. London, UK: Spon. Griffiths, W., Hill, R.C., & Judge, G.G. (1993). Learning and practicing econometrics. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons. Jones, I. (2015). Research methods for sports studies. 3rd ed. London, UK: Routledge. Késenne, S. (2012). The economic impact, costs and benefits of the FIFA World Cup and the Olympic Games: Who wins, who loses? In W. Maennig & A.S. Zimbalist (Eds.), International handbook on the economics of mega sporting events (pp. 270–278). Cheltenham, UK: Edward Elgar. Kwiatkowski, G. (2016). Economic impact of event attendees’ spending on a host region: A review of the research. Event Management, 20(4), 501–515. doi:10.3727/152599516X14745497664398 Lee, S. (2016, February 2). 10 ways San Francisco has fumbled its Super Bowl festivities (so far). Newsweek. Retrieved from http://www. newsweek.com/san-francisco-super-bowl-super-bowl-50-super-bowlcity-421857 Matheson, V.A., & Baade, R.A. (2006). Padding required: Assessing the economic impact of the Super Bowl. European Sport Management Quarterly, 6(4), 353–374. doi:10.1080/16184740601154490 Mills, B.M., & Rosentraub, M.S. (2013). Hosting mega-events: A guide to the evaluation of development effects in integrated metropolitan regions. Tourism Management, 34, 238–246. doi:10.1016/j. tourman.2012.03.011 Neter, J., & Waksberg, J. (1964). A study of response errors in expenditures data from household interviews. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 59(305), 18–55. doi:10.1080/01621459.1964. 10480699 Nielsen. (2016, February 8). Super Bowl 50 Draws 111.9 Million TV Viewers. 16.9 Million Tweets. Retrieved from http://www.nielsen. com/us/en/insights/news/2016/super-bowl-50-draws-111-9-milliontv-viewers-and-16-9-million-tweets.html Preuss, H. (2005). The economic impact of visitors at major multi-sport events. European Sport Management Quarterly, 5, 281–301. doi:10. 1080/16184740500190710 Preuss, H., Kurscheidt, M., & Schütte, N. (2009). Ökonomie des Tourismus durch Sportgroßveranstaltungen: Eine empirische Analyse zur Fußball-Weltmeisterschaft 2006. Wiesbaden, Germany: Springer Gabler Verlag. Rascher, D.A., & Goldman, M.M. (2015). Tracking the dollars: How economic impact studies can actually benefit managerial decision making. Sport & Entertainment Review, 1(1), 15–19. Raymos, J. (2016, January 25). PG&E telling downtown SF employees to work from home, but some businesses left in Super Bowl cold. CBS SF Bay Area. Retrieved from http://sanfrancisco.cbslocal.com/2016/ 01/25/pge-telling-downtown-sf-employees-to-work-from-home-butsome-businesses-left-in-super-bowl-cold/ Repucom. (2016). Super bowl 50 host committee community impact study highlights positive impact on San Francisco bay area. Retrieved from http://repucom.net/super-bowl-50-host-committee-community-impactstudy/ Ritchie, B. (1984). Assessing the impact of hallmark events: Conceptual and research issues. Journal of Travel Research, 23(1), 2–11. doi:10. 1177/004728758402300101 San Francisco Municipal Transportation Agency. (2016). Getting around during super bowl 50. Retrieved from https://www.sfmta.com/sites/ default/files/pdfs/2016/SB50-General-Ltr_1.21.16.pdf Shank, M.D., & Beasley, F.M. (1998). Fan or fanatic: Refining a measure of sports involvement. Journal of Sport Behavior, 21(4), 435–444. Solberg, H.A., & Preuss, H. (2007). Major sport events and long-term tourism impacts. Journal of Sport Management, 21(2), 213–234. doi:10.1123/jsm.21.2.213 Super Bowl 50 Host Committee. (2016). Super bowl insight. Retrieved from http://www.cipherbsc.com/superbowlinsight/superbowl50insight/ reader.html?contextId=P4&isExternal=_external Swan, J.E., & Epley, D.E. (1981). Completion and response rates for different forms of income questions in a mail survey. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 52, 219–222. JSM Vol. 32, No. 5, 2018 Modeling Resident Spending During Sport Events Taks, M., Girginov, V., & Boucher, B. (2006). The outcomes of coattailmarketing: The case of Windsor, Ontario and Super Bowl XL. Sport Marketing Quarterly, 15(4), 232–242. Taks, M., Késenne, S., Chalip, L., Green, B.C., & Martyn, S. (2011). Economic impact analysis versus cost benefit analysis: The case of a medium-sized sport event. International Journal of Sport Finance, 6(3), 187–203. 485 Wilton, J.J., & Nickerson, N.P. (2006). Collecting and using visitor spending data. Journal of Travel Research, 45(1), 17–25. doi:10. 1177/0047287506288875 Yun, G.W., & Trumbo, C.W. (2000). Comparative response to a survey executed by post, e-mail, & web form. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 6(1). doi:10.1111/j.1083-6101.2000.tb00112.x JSM Vol. 32, No. 5, 2018 Copyright of Journal of Sport Management is the property of Human Kinetics Publishers, Inc. and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.
Purchase answer to see full attachment
Explanation & Answer:
2 pages
User generated content is uploaded by users for the purposes of learning and should be used following Studypool's honor code & terms of service.

Explanation & Answer

Please view explanation and answer below.

Reading review outline
Thesi...

Related Tags