13
Culture and Human Inference
Perspectives from Three Traditions
Copyright © 2001. Oxford University Press. All rights reserved.
KAIPING PENG, DANIEL R. AMES, & ERIC D. KNOWLES
Human inference—that is, the ability to make judgments—is a basic psychological
process that cuts across areas of study in psychology, from perception and cognition
through social behavior and reasoning. It is an area that has been well studied not
only in mainstream psychology in the United States, but also across many other cultures of the world, by psychologists, anthropologists, and philosophers alike.
In this chapter, Peng, Ames, and Knowles provide a comprehensive review of the
cross-cultural literature on human inference. They first describe three major perspectives that have provided much of the impetus for the work conducted to date—the
value, self, and theory tradition. In particular, they describe the basic tenets of each
tradition and highlight their contributions to the conduct of research and theoretical
understanding of human inference.
A major part of their chapter presents a state-of-the-art review of the cross-cultural
research in specific domains of inference, including domains in inductive reasoning,
as well as deductive and formal reasoning. Their review of these areas is superb and
draws attention to the major findings in the field, which cut across cognitive, personality, and social psychology.
The major contribution of the work of Peng et al., however, is their attempt to integrate the three major perspectives into a single, synthesized model of cultural influence on human inference. They point out, and correctly so, that mainstream psychology is too quick to latch on to single viewpoints, examining psychological processes
solely from those single viewpoints to the exclusion of other views of the same process. One is quickly reminded from their points here about the story of different people viewing a different part of the same elephant or of the Humpty Dumpty story as
described in a previous chapter in this book. Peng et al. also point out that such an
exclusive focus on single perspectives of psychological phenomena runs the danger
of becoming fragmented into more pieces, further shattering the whole into more unidentifiable parts.
Instead, Peng et al. distill an integrated theory of cultural influences on human inference, suggesting the ways in which the value, self, and theory traditions may all
be true and all work together in a collectively efficient manner to influence human inference. While they describe for the reader different traditions of thought among Western linear thought, logical determinism, and Eastern holism and contradiction and
245
Matsumoto, D. (Ed.). (2001). The handbook of culture and psychology. Retrieved from http://ebookcentral.proquest.com
Created from waldenu on 2018-03-27 06:39:15.
246
CULTURE AND PERSONALITY
Copyright © 2001. Oxford University Press. All rights reserved.
how these influence human inference, at the same time they provide an important lesson about cultural influences on theory construction as they employ a framework of
holism and synthesis to a field that is fragmented. In this way, there are two lessons
to be learned from their approach, one concerning the content of their work (i.e.,
about their model of human inference) and the other concerning their method of theory construction.
Given their synthesized view of human inference, Peng et al. suggest clearly that
the most important challenge for future study in this area concerns the need for methodologies that are meaningful at both psychological and cultural levels, that are both
tractable and precise, while at the same time nuanced and sensitive. They call for a
rapprochement between the methods and theories of cross-cultural and cultural psychology, at the same time espousing the guiding methodological principles of mainstream research, including concerns for objectivity, validity, generalization, and
causal explanation. Their call for rapprochement and integration among theories,
methods, and approaches, including quantitative and qualitative ones, strikes a chord
similar to the message provided by many other authors in this volume and is necessary if cross-cultural psychology will be able to continue to evolve in the creation of
universal models of psychological processes, as Peng et al. have proposed, and to test
them adequately.
Two decades ago, American social psychologists Nisbett and Ross (1980) published their
now classic book, Human Inference, a broad
survey of how judgments, particularly about
the social world, unfold from evidence and reasoning. D’Andrade, a notable cognitive anthropologist, read the book and pronounced it a
“good ethnography.” The authors were dismayed: They thought they had written a universal account of inference and cognition, describing social judgment processes in a relatively
timeless and culture-free way. Most of their
colleagues at the time agreed. However, in the
ensuing 20 years, cultural psychology has blossomed, some of it pursued by Nisbett and Ross.
The accumulating evidence on cultural differences in inference is clear, and Nisbett and Ross
now agree that their original work amounts to
something of an ethnographic study of inference in a single culture, the United States (see
Nisbett, Peng, Choi, & Norenzayan, in press).
What has happened in the past two decades
that changed the minds of these and other psychologists studying human judgment, leading
them to believe previous efforts on human inference are useful, but culturally bound? What
has cultural psychology revealed about the role
of culture in human inference? In this chapter,
we review an ongoing revolution that examines
the cultural nature of judgment and thinking.
Evidence suggests that so-called basic processes such as attribution and categorization
do not play out in the same ways among all
human groups—and the differences go beyond
superficial variety in content. A variety of empirical studies on culture and inference, mostly
works completed in the past decade, is utilized
to illustrate the cultural characteristics of
human inference. But, before reviewing this evidence, we examine three prominent psychological approaches to studying culture: the
well-established value and self traditions and
the emerging theory tradition. Each has a distinct way of conceiving culture and makes different kinds of claims about the relationship
between culture and inference. Our brief introduction of these traditions lays the groundwork
for an overview of psychological research findings. After recounting the findings, we return
to the traditions and propose an integrated way
for thinking about the rich and wide-ranging
connections between culture and human inference.
Perspectives on Culture and
Human Inference
It is no great exaggeration to suggest that the
biggest challenge to culture-oriented scholars
of human inference is the issue of the independent variable, “What is culture?” Wide-ranging
answers are available from a host of disciplines,
variously including shared meaning systems,
cultural personality or ethos, practices and habits, institutions and social structures, artifacts
and tools, and everything that takes place in
human psychological life and interaction be-
Matsumoto, D. (Ed.). (2001). The handbook of culture and psychology. Retrieved from http://ebookcentral.proquest.com
Created from waldenu on 2018-03-27 06:39:15.
CULTURE AND HUMAN INFERENCE
yond what is dictated by our genes. However,
for psychological researchers to gain traction
on the issue of human inference, they must
“unpack” culture and adopt a position on how
to define and reflect culture in their work (Ames
& Peng, 1999b; Betancourt & Lopez, 1993; Rohner, 1984; Whiting, 1976).
There is likely no single best definition of
culture or way of studying the effects of culture.
Rather, research psychologists highlight various aspects of culture, adopting inevitably imperfect, but workable, assumptions about what
culture is. In psychological work related to human inference, there have been two dominant
traditions over the last 20 years: one that arranges cultures by their distinct value systems
and one that contrasts cultures in terms of their
conceptions of selfhood. More recently, another tradition has emerged that describes cultures in terms of a variety of widely shared
implicit folk theories. We review each of these
perspectives in turn, but note that they have
many assumptions and techniques in common;
individual scholars—and even individual studies—may draw on several or all of these approaches.
Copyright © 2001. Oxford University Press. All rights reserved.
Value Tradition
Many people who have traveled or lived outside their home country have a sense that people in other cultures possess values different
from their own. In some way, these values
could be taken as defining culture itself, and
systematic differences in values—especially in
a small collection of “core” values—could be
seen as providing some structure for thinking
about cultural differences. This, broadly, is the
approach advocated by a large number of cultural psychologists (e.g., P. B. Smith & Bond,
1999, p. 69).
A pioneering figure from these ranks is Hofstede (1980), who some 20 years ago compiled
an almost unparalleled data set: He administered a survey of values to nearly 120,000 IBM
employees in 40 countries. Hofstede factor analyzed the data at the country level (as a proxy
for culture) and found four dimensions, which
he labeled power distance (willingness to tolerate differences in power and authority), individualism (versus collectivism; orientation toward individual or group), masculinity (versus
femininity; the former stressing achievement
and material success, the latter stressing harmony and caring), and uncertainty avoidance
(willingness to tolerate ambiguity). Hofstede’s
247
approach has been pursued by a number of other
scholars, including Schwartz (1991; Schwartz
and Sagiv, 1995), who argues that 10 important
values (such as tradition, security, power, and
stimulation) form a universal structure across
two dimensions: openness to change/conservation and self-transcendence/self-enhancement.
According to Schwartz, any given culture has
an identifiable position in this value space that
allows it to be compared with other cultures.
A number of scholars have examined crosscultural differences in inference and judgment
by focusing on particular value dimensions.
Shweder (1995), for instance, explored the
value of spiritual purity among Hindu Indians.
Meanwhile, Leung (1997) examined how East
Asian harmony values affect justice perceptions and decisions, such as reward allocation.
However, the most widespread research program in the value tradition has focused on one
of the dimensions identified by Hofstede: individualism-collectivism. This dimension reflects
an orientation toward one’s own needs and impulses (individualism) or toward the needs and
dictates of one’s social groups, such as families
and communities (collectivism).
Individualism-collectivism has drawn a great
deal of attention from cross-cultural researchers, and some observers see it as the most overarching theory of cultural psychology (Triandis, 1995). Scholars have made this dimension
operational at both the country level (assigning
“individualism scores” to countries) and at the
individual level (with studies gauging individual participants’ values). Most often, East
Asians are seen as more collectivist, while
North Americans and Europeans are viewed as
individualist.
How does the value tradition prepare us to
think about cultural differences in inference?
Three main points emerge. First, in frequently
highlighting individualism-collectivism as a
central dimension, the value approach draws
our attention to inferences that concern judgments about groups and about how individuals
relate to groups. If a main source of cultural
differences occurs in the attitudes of members
about their groups and group relations, we
would expect to find considerable accompanying cultural variance in inferences related to
groups and membership.
Second, and more broadly, the value tradition underscores the importance of prescriptive
stances in construal and judgment. Scholars
in this tradition do not simply make causal
claims about values that affect other values and
Matsumoto, D. (Ed.). (2001). The handbook of culture and psychology. Retrieved from http://ebookcentral.proquest.com
Created from waldenu on 2018-03-27 06:39:15.
248
CULTURE AND PERSONALITY
choices (such as claims about a general stance
of individualism that affects a narrow attitude
toward wanting to take credit for some good
outcomes). Rather, claims are made that connect values to inferences and resulting beliefs
(e.g., between individualism and the belief that
a single person is the cause for a good performance). What is the connection between these
prescriptive and descriptive stances? How do
norms shape inferences from evidence? The
value tradition draws attention to such questions.
A third, and related, consideration prompted
by the values approach is a pragmatic or functionalist one: What are the consequences of certain inferences in, for example, a collectivist
culture? If collectivism describes a system of
norms, those norms comprise an important part
of the environment in which inferences must be
“lived out.” Thus, the value approach prompts
consideration of how inferences are shaped by
the consequences they might entail in particular cultural contexts. We return to this issue of
consequences, as well as to the issue of prescription-description, in our concluding analysis of the three traditions.
Copyright © 2001. Oxford University Press. All rights reserved.
Self Tradition
Beginning a century ago with James (1890), the
construct of “self” has been widely regarded by
scholars as playing a key role in much psychological functioning (see Markus & Cross, 1990,
for a review). Although James and many of his
Western intellectual heirs have voiced the caveat that the self may be experienced differently
in various cultural systems, there has been little
psychological research on this issue until recently. Is self a cultural concept? A chorus of
researchers answers “Yes” and suggests it is
perhaps the most important cultural concept.
Contemporary thinking about culture and
self has been led by Markus and Kitayama
(1991), who suggest not only that the psychology of self varies across cultures, but also that
self-conceptions may be at the very heart of
what culture is. Markus, Kitayama, and others
have described culturally driven ways of “being” a self, focusing specifically on two types
of self: independent and interdependent. An
independent construal of self, prevalent in the
West, is characterized by a sense of autonomy,
of being relatively distinct from others. In contrast, the interdependent construal of self, prevalent in Asia, is characterized by an emphasis
on the interrelatedness of the individual to oth-
ers; self-identity is more diffused socially across
important others rather than strictly bounded
with the individual. There is an obvious similarity between these self-concepts and individualism-collectivism. However, it is worth noting the descriptive, as well as the prescriptive,
nature of these positions. We might crudely
characterize the slogan of collectivism as “My
in-group is important,” while an interdependent self might be described as “My in-group
is who I am.”
A host of research by Markus and Kitayama
(e.g., 1991), Heine and Lehman (e.g., 1995, 1997),
Singelis (e.g., 1994), and others has explored this
cultural dimension of selfhood. Other scholarship on culture and self has emerged as well,
including Shweder’s (1995) description of divinity in selfhood among Hindu Indians; in this
case, self is not so much distributed socially
across other persons (as with an interdependent
self), but distributed spiritually across reincarnations and all living things.
What guidance does the self tradition provide regarding cultural differences in human
inference? Two major considerations emerge.
First, understanding the social network that
could potentially be implicated in a perceiver’s
self-concept becomes critical. A perceiver’s attention to others in this network may be driven
by his or her self-concept; the self-concept
would likely also affect how others in this network are treated in judgments. Second, highlighting the self-concept encourages us to expand our view of the domains of inference in
which self-construal matters. In other words,
the impact of self-concept can be found in domains beyond self-judgment. Cognitive dissonance, for instance, might seem unrelated to
the self, but Heine and Lehman (1997) argue
that Japanese experience less dissonance than
Canadians because of how they understand social contexts and the self.
Theory Tradition
The traditions of value and self have attracted
many cultural psychologists over the last 20
years. These perspectives have revealed many
insights and continue to do so. However, an
increasing amount of cultural scholarship is
not based on a person’s notions of self or value
systems, but on various folk theories and beliefs
shared by a culture’s members. There is no single theme of content (like “individualism”) that
unifies this emerging tradition, but rather there
is an inclination to identify and measure im-
Matsumoto, D. (Ed.). (2001). The handbook of culture and psychology. Retrieved from http://ebookcentral.proquest.com
Created from waldenu on 2018-03-27 06:39:15.
Copyright © 2001. Oxford University Press. All rights reserved.
CULTURE AND HUMAN INFERENCE
plicit folk theories at a rather specific level—a
level, moreover, that ties directly into inference
and judgment. Work in this tradition does not
attempt to measure culture in its entirety, but
rather selects particular domains and attempts
to describe judgments by culturally driven beliefs.
Members of a culture share a variety of widespread stances that scholars have described in
terms of cultural models (Holland & Quinn,
1987); cosmologies (Douglas, 1982); social representations (Moscovici, 1984; Wagner, 1997);
cultural representations (Boyer, 1993; Sperber,
1990); naive ontologies and epistemologies (Ames
& Peng, 1999b; Peng & Nisbett, 1999); and folk
psychologies, biologies, sociologies, and physics
(e.g., Ames, 1999; Atran, 1990; Fiske, 1992; Lillard, 1998; Peng & Knowles, 2000; Vosniadou,
1994).
One way of describing these stances is to
consider them implicit theories (Dweck, Chiu,
& Hong, 1995; Vallacher & Wegner, 1987) about
the world that persons in particular communities have in common to varying degrees. The
particular theories studied vary widely. Chiu,
Hong, and Dweck (1997), for instance, examined implicit theories about personality change:
Americans tend to assume considerable constancy in personality, while Hong Kong Chinese often expect malleability. Elsewhere, Ames
and Peng (1999a) showed that, compared to
Americans, Chinese are guided by more holistic
theories of impression evidence in getting to
know a target person. The research of Menon,
Morris, Chiu, and Hong (1999) suggests that
Americans’ theories of groups lead them to assign less causality and responsibility at the
group level, while Chinese, led by different theories, are more willing to make such attributions.
Given this variety of topics, the theory approach to culture is identifiable by a common
set of assumptions and methodologies—stances
largely shared with the emerging implicit theories perspective in psychology in general (Dweck,
1996; Wegner & Vallacher, 1977). Some of these
assumptions are shared with scholarship in the
self and value traditions, but the theory tradition seems distinct in its specificity of constructs and variety of domains.
What distinct insights does the theory tradition offer regarding culture and the psychology
of inference? Two novel points emerge. First,
the implicit theory approach offers compelling
ways for describing variance across persons
and groups and change across time. Personality
psychologists are increasingly using implicit
249
theories to capture the differences between individuals (e.g., Dweck, 1996); some developmental psychologists, meanwhile, describe the
course of cognitive development in terms of
theory adoption and use (e.g., Gopnik & Meltzoff, 1997). At the culture level, the theory approach offers dynamic models for describing
how widespread beliefs are transmitted, flourish, and fade—and for what kinds of beliefs
might be likely to prosper (Boyer, 1993; Moscovici, 1984; Sperber, 1990; Strauss & Quinn,
1997). The implicit theory approach also lends
itself readily to cultural differences within a
given geographic region, such as within a
country.
A second contribution is that the theory approach points toward comparatively precise
models of culturally influenced psychological
process. Culture is made operational at a representational level as a knowledge structure (a
folk theory) that supports and guides inference.
As Ames and Peng (1999b) note, implicit theories can play a direct role in inferences, with
perceivers invoking a theory to go beyond the
information given (e.g., use of a stereotype in
which a target’s gender yields an inference
about their aggressive tendencies). Theories
can also play a management role in inference,
guiding how evidence and other theories are
recruited and used (Ames & Peng, 1999b). For
instance, an epistemological theory about the
value of contextual evidence does not itself
yield a conclusion, but can guide a perceiver’s
attention toward certain aspects of the environment. The effect of culture no doubt can be
seen working at both these levels.
Cultural Research on
Human Inference
These three traditions take different approaches
to studying culture and human inference. What
does each reveal about cultural differences in
inference? Can they be integrated into some
overarching framework? Are they somehow at
odds with one another—and is one more preferable than the others? Before we answer these
questions, we proceed first by reviewing evidence for cultural differences. We organize our
review around two major domains of inference:
induction and deduction. Here, we are concerned with findings rather than traditions, and
we include as much relevant empirical work
as possible. After this review of findings, we
return to the issue of the three traditions and,
Matsumoto, D. (Ed.). (2001). The handbook of culture and psychology. Retrieved from http://ebookcentral.proquest.com
Created from waldenu on 2018-03-27 06:39:15.
250
CULTURE AND PERSONALITY
based on the evidence, search for an overarching framework.
Inductive Reasoning
As a working definition, we take induction to
be the human ability to reach useful generalizations based on limited experience and information. These generalizations come in multiple
forms, ranging from the apprehension of correlations between phenomena in the environment to the attribution of causes for physical
and social events, and from the inference of
a target person’s personality traits and mental
states to the formation and use of categories. In
this section, we argue that culture plays a role
in each of these types of inductive inference.
Copyright © 2001. Oxford University Press. All rights reserved.
Covariation Judgment
Since the birth of behaviorism, psychologists
have viewed the ability to perceive covariations
between environmental stimuli accurately as a
fundamental type of human inference (Alloy &
Tabachnik, 1984). Perhaps because the processes that rely on covariation judgment (e.g.,
classical conditioning) are seen as so basic to
human cognition, psychologists have only recently begun investigating the influence of culture on people’s ability to detect and evaluate
associations. In one of the few studies to address this issue, Ji, Peng, and Nisbett (1999) examined covariation judgment among Chinese
and Americans. Reasoning that the dialectical
epistemology of Asians would make them especially sensitive to relations between stimuli,
these researchers predicted that Asians would
exceed Americans in their ability to evaluate
the magnitude of associations between stimuli.
Chinese and American participants were shown
pairs of arbitrary figures on a computer screen;
particular stimuli were correlated to varying
degrees, and participants were asked to judge
the degree of association.
The results provide preliminary evidence
for the influence of culture on covariation detection. Chinese were more confident than Americans about their covariation judgments, and
their confidence judgments were better calibrated with the actual degree of covariation between figures. In addition, American participants showed a strong primacy effect, making
predictions about future covariations that were
more influenced by the first pairings they had
seen than by the overall degree of covariation
to which they had been exposed. In contrast,
Chinese participants showed no primacy effect
at all and made predictions about future covariation that were based on the covariation they
had actually seen.
Causal Attribution
Covariation judgment is undeniably important
to human survival; very often, however, people
are not satisfied by merely estimating the magnitude of associations between environmental
phenomena. People typically go further, assigning phenomena to their presumed causes. Lay
causal analysis—or causal attribution—has
been one of the most thoroughly studied areas
in psychology. Below, we review evidence that,
in both the social and physical domains, people’s attributions are influenced by culture.
Social Domain The last two decades have seen
a growing acknowledgment that culture guides
people’s attributions for social phenomena (i.e.,
the social behavior of others). Prior to this recognition, however, psychologists often assumed
that the findings of studies conducted in Western settings would generalize across cultures.
One of most widely reported findings in (Western) attribution research is that people tend to
see behavior as a product of the actor’s dispositions, while ignoring important situational
causes of behavior.
In an early demonstration of dispositional
bias, Jones and Harris (1967) asked perceivers
to infer a target’s attitude on a controversial
political topic based on an essay written by the
target. Participants were also given information
about situational determinants of the target’s
behavior that suggested against the usefulness
of the speech in ascertaining the target’s true
attitude—specifically, that the target had been
required to write the essay by an authority figure. Despite having information about the
power of the situation, most participants were
willing to infer a behavior-correspondent attitude.
After this and other classic demonstrations,
confidence in the universality of dispositional
bias ran high—so high, in fact, that psychologists dubbed the bias the “fundamental attribution error” (Ross, 1977). The assumption of universality is reflected in theoretical accounts of
attribution, which portray dispositional inference as the product of gestalt (Heider, 1958;
Jones, 1990) or ecological (Baron & Misovich,
1993) perceptual processes presumed to be similar across cultures.
Work by Miller (1984) first suggested that
the fundamental attribution error might not
Matsumoto, D. (Ed.). (2001). The handbook of culture and psychology. Retrieved from http://ebookcentral.proquest.com
Created from waldenu on 2018-03-27 06:39:15.
Copyright © 2001. Oxford University Press. All rights reserved.
CULTURE AND HUMAN INFERENCE
be so fundamental. She found that, whereas
Americans explained the behavior of others
predominantly in terms of traits (e.g., recklessness or kindness), Hindu Indians explained
comparable behaviors in terms of social roles,
obligations, the physical environment, and other
contextual factors. This finding calls into question the universality of dispositional bias and,
by extension, attribution theories that link dispositional inference to universal perceptual
mechanisms. Miller’s work instead suggested
that attributions for social events are largely the
product of culturally instilled belief systems
that stress the importance of either dispositional or situational factors in producing social
behavior. Numerous researchers have extended
Miller’s (1984) basic finding—in which Asians
focus more on situational factors in explaining
behavior than do Westerners—to a wide range
of cultures and social phenomena. While we
cannot present an exhaustive review of crosscultural attribution research (see Choi, Nisbett,
& Norenzayan, 1999, for a more extensive treatment), we survey some representative studies
below.
American explanations for events such as
mass murders were shown by Morris and Peng
(1994; see also Morris, Nisbett, & Peng, 1995) to
focus almost entirely on the presumed mental
instability and other negative dispositions of
the murderers, whereas Chinese accounts of the
same events referred more to situational and
societal factors. The researchers then replicated
this cultural difference using visual stimuli that
depicted animal movements. Participants were
presented with cartoon displays of a target fish
moving relative to the school in a variety of
ways. Each pattern of movement was ambiguous in that the movement of the target fish could
be attributed to dispositional causes (e.g., the
fish is a leader) or situational causes (e.g., the
fish is being chased by the school). As expected,
Chinese participants were more likely to see
the behavior of the individual fish as being produced by situational factors than were Americans.
Other researchers have documented cultural
diversity in attributions for more mundane, everyday events. For instance, F. Lee, Hallahan,
and Herzog (1996) found that sports editorial
writers in Hong Kong focused on situational
explanations of sports events, whereas American sports writers were more likely to prefer
explanations that involved the dispositions of
individual team members. Choi et al. (1999)
found that Korean participants, unlike Ameri-
251
can participants, did not underutilize consensus information (i.e., information about the
behavior of other people) when making attributions—information that logically should be
used to gauge the power of situational factors.
Likewise, Norenzayan, Choi, and Nisbett (1999)
found that Korean participants were more responsive to contextual factors when making
predictions about how people in general would
behave in a given situation and, much more
than American participants, made use of their
beliefs about situational power when predicting the behavior of a particular individual.
Choi and Nisbett (1998) duplicated the basic
conditions of the Jones and Harris (1967) study,
adding a condition in which, before making
judgments about the target’s attitude, participants were required to write an essay and were
allowed no choice about which side to take. It
was made clear to participants that the target
had been through the same procedure the participants had. Participants were then asked to
judge the target’s true attitude. The American
participants in this condition made inferences
about the target’s attitude that were as strong
as those made by participants in the standard
no-choice condition. Korean participants, in
contrast, made much less extreme inferences.
Thus, Korean participants, presumably by virtue of seeing the role that the situation played
in their own behavior, recognized the power of
the context and made attributions about others
accordingly. Similar results were obtained by
Kitayama and Masuda (1997) in Japan. These
researchers duplicated the procedure of Gilbert and Jones (1986), in which participants
were paired with a confederate and told one of
them would be randomly assigned to read an
essay written by a third person. After the confederate was chosen to read the essay, American observers assumed that the target individual actually held the position advocated in the
speech. Although Masuda and Kitayama found
strong attitude inferences in line with the
speech for Japanese subjects in the standard nochoice condition, they found none at all when it
was made clear that the target individual was
simply reading an essay written by someone
else.
Physical Domain Unlike attributions for social phenomena, relatively little research has
examined the influence of culture on lay explanations for physical events. Nonetheless, there
is reason to believe that folk theories of physical
causality differ between Western and Asian
Matsumoto, D. (Ed.). (2001). The handbook of culture and psychology. Retrieved from http://ebookcentral.proquest.com
Created from waldenu on 2018-03-27 06:39:15.
252
CULTURE AND PERSONALITY
cultures, and that these differences may lead to
culturally divergent interpretations of physical
phenomena. Many scholars have argued that
Asian folk physics is relational and dialectical,
stressing conceptions of “field” and “force over
distance” (Capra, 1975; Needham, 1954, 1962;
Zukav, 1980). On the other hand, Western folk
physics is seen as preferring internal and dispositional causes, explaining physical phenomena in terms of “the nature of the object
concerned” rather than the relation of objects
to the environment (Lewin, 1935, p. 28). Peng
and Knowles (2000) presented evidence that
this difference in intellectual tradition may affect everyday interpretations of physical events.
These researchers presented Chinese and American individuals with physical interactions involving “force-over-distance” causality resembling hydrodynamic, aerodynamic, or magnetic
phenomena. In explaining these events, Chinese
participants were more likely to refer to the field,
whereas Americans were more likely to refer
solely to factors internal to the object. The researchers concluded that development within
Asian cultures instilled individuals with a relational, field-oriented folk physics, while Western cultures instilled their members with a more
dispositional, analytic folk physics (also see Peng
& Nisbett, 1996).
Copyright © 2001. Oxford University Press. All rights reserved.
Person Perception
Our inferences about other persons are crucial
to our everyday lives. We frequently and fluidly
make judgments about those around us: what
they’re like, how they’re feeling, what they
want. These judgments are certainly related to
attribution, but differ in an important way:
Whereas attribution concerns assigning cause
and responsibility to events, person perception
concerns assigning qualities to persons. For instance, if Beth’s new assistant Andrew acts
aggressively toward her, Beth might consider
attributing the behavior to Andrew’s dispositional aggression or to some other event that
made Andrew angry—a case of both attribution
and person perception. However, if Beth is deciding whether or not to ask Charles to be her
new assistant and gets letters of recommendation from Charles’ teachers and former employers and also interviews Charles, integrating this
information to form a judgment is more a case
of person perception (“solving for a person”)
than attribution (“solving for an event”).
Not surprisingly, person perception inferences take on different forms in different cultures. Here, we only briefly review selected
findings in two areas of person perception: impression formation and the inference of mental
states.
Impression Formation What kinds of impressions do we form about persons? One theme
that emerges is the willingness to see personal
qualities as fixed and enduring or malleable
and changing (Dweck, 1996). This difference
seems to map well onto the cultural dimension
of independent and interdependent selves: The
former notion features a more fixed self, the
latter describes a changeable, context-based self.
This connection was pursued by C. Y. Chiu,
Hong, and Dweck (1997) in their comparison
of dispositional judgments by American and
Hong Kong perceivers. As the literature on the
self would predict, these researchers found a
main effect of culture: American perceivers
were more willing than Hong Kong perceivers
to ascribe fixed, enduring traits to targets.
Dweck, Chiu, and Hong (1995) also measured
the individual theories of the perceivers about
the nature of dispositions: A high score on their
dispositionalism scale indicated a belief in
fixed, unchanging traits. There was a culture
difference in dispositionalism, with Americans
scoring higher. Following in the theory tradition, Dweck and colleagues demonstrated that
this implicit theory of dispositionalism mediated the effect of culture on perceivers’ trait
judgments.
It appears that perceivers in the East may
be less oriented toward making ascriptions of
dispositions to targets. Are there also differences in the kinds of evidence that are sought
out and used in forming impressions? Research
by Ames and Peng (1999a) suggests there are.
Following from cultural research on self, dispositionalism, and dialecticism, Ames and Peng
proposed that Americans would be more focused on evidence directly from or about a
target (e.g., a self-description), while Chinese
would be more focused on contextual evidence
(e.g., a description of the target by a friend,
a description of the target’s friend). Across a
variety of studies, just such a pattern emerged.
Americans expressed greater preference for target-focused evidence and made greater use of
target self-descriptions in their evaluations of
targets.
Inference of Mental States How
know what others are thinking,
wanting? Recent work suggests
state epistemologies may differ
Matsumoto, D. (Ed.). (2001). The handbook of culture and psychology. Retrieved from http://ebookcentral.proquest.com
Created from waldenu on 2018-03-27 06:39:15.
is it that we
feeling, and
that mental
by culture.
CULTURE AND HUMAN INFERENCE
Knowles and Ames (1999) suggested that Western cultures stress a “norm of authenticity”
such that a person’s external actions and displays should be consistent with their internal
attitudes. “Saying what’s on your mind” and
“straight talk” are sought-after qualities in the
West. Eastern cultures, meanwhile, may view
such displays as impolite and potentially bizarre. The role of hosts in many Asian countries, for instance, is to intuit a guest’s unspoken
needs, while guests are often expected to defer
and not betray self-centered desires.
Knowles and Ames (1999) collected initial
evidence documenting such an epistemic difference in the United States and China. When
asked how important various pieces of evidence are in determining what someone is
thinking, Americans, on average, rate “what
they say” as more important than “what they
do not say,” while Chinese show the reverse
preference. The same pattern holds for determining what someone is feeling or wanting.
Mental state epistemology in the West may be
as simple as listening: It is not uncommon to
wish targets disclosed less about their beliefs,
desires, intentions, and so forth. Reading minds
in the East, however, may take other routes,
such as nonverbal behavior.
Copyright © 2001. Oxford University Press. All rights reserved.
Categorization
Categorization is one of the most ubiquitous
and important human mental activities and
provides efficiency in memory and enables
communication. Moreover, categories aid survival by allowing us to make educated guesses
about the unseen properties of categorized objects (“That rustling behind the bush must be
Johnny’s new pit bull. I bet it has a bad temper.”). Categorization is one of the most wellstudied areas of psychology, as well as the
closely allied field of cognitive anthropology.
Researchers have distinguished between three
related questions. First, where do categories get
their structure (the question of category coherence)? Second, how and when do people use
categories to make inductions about unseen
properties of objects (the question of category
use)? Finally, how do individuals acquire new
categories (the question of category learning)?
There is growing evidence that culture is part
of the answer to each of these questions.
Category Coherence Of all the infinitely many
ways one could divide the world, why do people show a decided preference for some categories (e.g., “dog”) and not others (e.g., “apple
253
or prime number”)? In other words, what makes
some categories hang together or cohere? In her
recent review of category coherence research,
Malt (1995) noted a shift in psychologists’ thinking concerning the source of category coherence. Early psychological work tended to suppose that structure inherent in the environment
determines which categories people will form.
Most notably, Rosch and colleagues (Rosch &
Mervis, 1975; Rosch, Mervis, Gray, Johnson, &
Boyes-Braem, 1976) argued that perceptible
features in the world are not distributed randomly across entities, but rather occur in clusters—for instance, “fur,” “four legged,” and
“barks” tend to occur together. People take advantage of this environmental structure by
grouping entities that share clusters of features
into categories; for instance, entities in which
fur, four legged, and barks cooccur are grouped
into the category “dog.” While it is true that
the human perceptual system must place constraints on which feature correlations people
notice (Murphy & Medin, 1985), the work of
Rosch and colleagues emphasizes the role of
environmental structure in determining category coherence. A corollary of this view is that,
to the extent the human perceptual system is
the same everywhere, classification systems
will be relatively impervious to the influence
of higher level cognitive structures—such as
those instilled by culture.
More recent work, while not denying the
role of environmental structure in lending coherence to taxonomic categories like “dog” and
“fern,” points to the contribution of high-level
cognitive structures in determining coherence
of nontaxonomic categories. Barsalou (1983,
1985) drew attention to a class of categories that
could not exist simply by virtue of their
mapping onto environmental structures such
as correlations between perceptible features.
Specifically, “goal-based” categories are coherent because their members serve a common
goal; for example, pencils and calculators, despite sharing few features, could both be
grouped into the category “things used to take
to a math exam.” Goal-based categories are
highly susceptible to cultural influence since
cultures undeniably shape the goals adopted
by their members. To illustrate, things to take to
a math exam is a coherent category for Western
youths, but not for members of preliterate
societies, whose members lack the goal of taking math exams.
Work in cognitive anthropology and crosscultural psychology suggests that culture even
Matsumoto, D. (Ed.). (2001). The handbook of culture and psychology. Retrieved from http://ebookcentral.proquest.com
Created from waldenu on 2018-03-27 06:39:15.
Copyright © 2001. Oxford University Press. All rights reserved.
254
CULTURE AND PERSONALITY
plays a role in the coherence of taxonomic categories. Malt (1995) reviewed a number of ethnobiological studies that indicated that the degree to which a society subcategorizes a plant
or animal domain corresponds in part to the
cultural importance of that domain. Folk categorizers direct their attention disproportionately to domains of the most practical importance to their culture (e.g., edible plants and
domesticated or dangerous animals) and as a
result create narrower subordinate categories
within those domains. This finding parallels
psychological evidence that individuals with a
history of allocating a disproportionate amount
of attention to a particular domain—for instance, birdwatchers or dog aficionados—may
develop “expertise” in that domain. Experts
create more subdivisions within their domain
of expertise than do nonexperts and categorize
within the domain more quickly (Tanaka & Taylor, 1991). In sum, culture may affect the deployment of attention to different taxonomic
domains, lending coherence to increasingly
subordinate categories.
Other anthropological and psychological research suggests that culture influences category
coherence not only by directing attention, but
also by changing the kinds of features used to
bind categories together. López, Atran, Coley,
Medin, and Smith (1997) found that, while
Americans tended to categorize animals on the
basis of size and ferocity, Itzaj-Mayan animal
categories were based largely on relational—
specifically, ecological—features, such as habitat and food consumption. Likewise, Atran and
Medin (1997) found that Itzaj-Mayan informants grouped arboreal mammals partly according to the nature of their interactions with
plants.
Experimental research suggests that such a
relational style of categorization plays an important role in Chinese culture. In 1972, L.-H.
Chiu showed Chinese and American children
sets of three pictures drawn from various domains and asked them to pick the two that went
together. The dominant style of categorization
for Chinese children was “relational-contextual.” For instance, shown a picture of a man,
a woman, and a child, Chinese children were
likely to group the woman and child together
because “the mother takes care of the baby.” In
contrast, American children were more likely
to group objects on the basis on isolable properties, such as age (e.g., grouping the man and
woman together because “they are both grownups”).
Category-Based Induction In addition to organizing the world for purposes of memory and
communication, categories serve the vital function of allowing people to go “beyond the information given.” Once an object has been categorized, category membership may be used as the
basis for inferences about the object’s unseen
or invisible properties; this process is referred
to as category-based induction. For instance,
knowing that an animal is a mammal allows
one to infer that it probably bears live young
and regulates its own body temperature.
Work by Choi, Nisbett, and Smith (1997)
suggests that category representations are less
chronically accessible for Koreans than for
Americans and thus are less readily used in
category-based induction. In keeping with previous research on category-based induction (e.g.,
Osherson, Smith, Wilkie, López, and Shafir,
1990), Choi and colleagues made operationalized category-based induction using a premise-conclusion format. For instance, individuals might be presented with the following
argument:
Hippos have ulnar arteries.
Hamsters have ulnar arteries.
. . . . . . . . . . .
Dogs have ulnar arteries.
Participants are then asked the extent to which
they believe the conclusion given the premises.
In the above example, participants might use
the premises to infer that mammals have ulnar
arteries and thus place great confidence in the
conclusion. The researchers increased category
salience by mentioning the category in the conclusion (that is, participants made an inference
about “mammals” rather than dogs). This manipulation had no effect on Americans, but increased the degree to which Koreans performed
category-based induction. This suggests that
categories have a lower chronic accessibility
for Koreans and are thus more susceptible to
priming.
Category Learning There is evidence that culture may influence the processes through
which people acquire new categories. Norenzayan, Nisbett, Smith, and Kim (2000), adopting a procedure used by Allen and Brooks
(1991), presented East Asians and Americans
with cartoon extraterrestrial creatures, indicating some were from Venus and some were from
Saturn. One group of participants was asked to
examine a series of creatures and make guesses,
with feedback, about the category to which each
Matsumoto, D. (Ed.). (2001). The handbook of culture and psychology. Retrieved from http://ebookcentral.proquest.com
Created from waldenu on 2018-03-27 06:39:15.
CULTURE AND HUMAN INFERENCE
Copyright © 2001. Oxford University Press. All rights reserved.
belonged. Other participants went through a
more formal, rule-based category-learning procedure. In this condition, participants were told
to pay attention to five different properties of
the animals and were told that if the animal
had any three of these properties, it was from
Venus; otherwise, it was from Saturn. Although
Asian and American participants performed
equally well at the exemplar-based categorization task, the response times of Asian participants were slower in the rule-based condition.
Most telling, when presented with an animal
that met the formal criteria for a certain category, but more closely resembled animals in the
other category—thus placing rule-based and exemplar-based criteria in conflict—Asians made
more classification errors than did Americans.
The Category of Self The idea of “self” being
a category like “mammal” or “hammer” may
seem peculiar at first glance, but after considering cultural differences, seeing self as a culturally varying category becomes something of an
obligation. Considerable research attention has
been directed at how perceivers in the West
and East describe themselves. The results reveal several themes, most notably that perceivers in the West see the self as more bounded
and concrete, while perceivers in the East see
the self as more socially diffused, changeable,
and context bound. In 1998, A. P. Fiske, Kitayama, Markus, and Nisbett reviewed much of
the relevant research and showed that Americans are more likely to describe themselves in
abstract, fixed ways (e.g., using trait terms such
as “friendly”), while Koreans, Japanese, and
Chinese are more likely to refer to social roles
and other people (e.g., “I am Jane’s friend”).
Elsewhere, Shweder (1995) explored the Hindu
Indian self-concept. Whereas Americans appear to possess an independent view of self,
and Southeast Asians seem to see the self as
more socially distributed, Shweder argued that
the Indian notion of self invokes notions of
divinity. With beliefs in reincarnation, karma,
and the interconnectedness of all living things,
the category of self comes to include multiple
lifetimes and life forms.
Deduction and
Formal Reasoning
In this section, we review findings about
the role of culture in deduction and formal reasoning. Deduction has a rather well-accepted
meaning: moving from information that is given
to information that follows with certainty or
255
necessity (e.g., given that all donuts have holes,
if X is a donut, then it must have a hole). By
formal reasoning, we mean to broaden our
scope somewhat to include a variety of judgments based on propositions or highly distilled
arguments. Here, we note selected cultural research on syllogistic reasoning and dialectical
reasoning, particularly in the domain of thinking about contradiction. Historically, it was often assumed that such abilities were universal—or at least took on a single form such that
cultural differences could be ascribed to performance or intellectual differences (see Cole,
1996). However, a host of scholars have revealed
culture-specific concepts and approaches, differences that seem much more reflective of fundamental epistemologies and cultural assumptions than individual competence.
Syllogistic Reasoning
Russian psychologist Luria (1931) was an early
explorer of syllogistic reasoning and culture.
In his studies in remote areas of Russia, participants were given what most Western scholars
would view as a straightforward task of deduction. Participants were told that all bears in the
North are white, and that a particular village
was in the North. Participants were then asked
the color of the bears in the village. Most failed
to answer the question—and many questioned
the basic premises of the task, suggesting, for
instance, that the researcher go to the village
and find out firsthand.
Cole (1996) replicated part of Luria’s (1931)
work in Africa and similarly found that many
participants did not engage the question at the
theoretical level. Participants were given premises such as, “If Juan and Jose drink a lot of
beer, the mayor of the town gets angry,” and,
“Juan and Jose are drinking a lot of beer now.”
In this case, participants were asked to judge
if the mayor was angry with Juan and Jose.
Some participants treated the question theoretically, but many others saw it as an empirical
issue and gave answers such as, “No, so many
men drink beer, why should the major get
angry?”
A century ago, such supposed “deficits” of
reasoning might have been seen as evidence of
lack of intelligence and cultural development.
Now, most scholars would agree that such performance is not a deficit, but rather highlights
distinct cultural models of reasoning (D’Andrade, 1995). Indeed, Luria (1931) and Cole
(1996) came to stress practical, everyday activity and cultural artifacts as central to culture-
Matsumoto, D. (Ed.). (2001). The handbook of culture and psychology. Retrieved from http://ebookcentral.proquest.com
Created from waldenu on 2018-03-27 06:39:15.
256
CULTURE AND PERSONALITY
specific reasoning: It may be useless, and perhaps harmful, to presume that abstract Western
tasks such as syllogistic reasoning are the gold
standard of reasoning and deductive ability.
D’Andrade (1995) suggested that reasoning
relies on learned cultural models (such as inference rules) and may also incorporate physical
cultural artifacts (like an abacus). Using the Wason task, a widely employed puzzle that putatively tests logical reasoning, D’Andrade showed
that successful performance depends overwhelmingly on how the puzzle is framed in
terms of everyday knowledge and ordinary domains. Framed as an abstract issue in a “label
factory,” participants do poorly; framed as a
question about the drinking age, participants
excel. Such real-world grounding has similar
effects across a variety of syllogistic and other
kinds of reasoning tasks (D’Andrade, 1995).
Copyright © 2001. Oxford University Press. All rights reserved.
Dialectical Reasoning
While few people share the logician’s ability
and enthusiasm for formal reasoning, it is
tempting to characterize most everyday thinkers as broadly adhering to some core tenets of
argument that have been mobilized since Aristotle’s time—for instance, the “law of noncontradiction,” which implies that no statement
can be both true and false. However, Peng and
Nisbett (Peng, 1997; Peng & Nisbett, 1999) have
shown that such a characterization might best
be limited to Western thinkers; East Asians,
they argue, subscribe to a different epistemology, with different rules for constructing arguments and making judgments. This work highlights the fact that deductive and other kinds of
reasoning hinge on underlying epistemological
assumptions about what knowledge and truth
are and how one can know them—assumptions
that can vary by culture.
Peng and Nisbett (1999) describe Western
reasoning as embracing three core laws. The
law of identity (A = A) denotes that everything
must be identical with itself. The law of the
excluded middle (A is either B or not-B) implies
that any statement is either true or false; there
are no half-truths. The law of noncontradiction
(A is not equal to not-A) proposes that no statement can be both true and false. On their face,
such notions seem to fit with a variety of Western psychological phenomena, such as naive
realism (e.g., Ross & Ward, 1996) and essentialism (e.g., Gelman & Medin, 1993), as well as a
seeming abhorrence of vacillation and falsehood.
Following various philosophers and historians of the East and West (Liu, 1974; Lloyd, 1990;
Needham, 1954, 1962; Zhang & Chen, 1991),
Peng and Nisbett (1999) argued that a different
approach obtains in Eastern folk thinking: a
dialectical epistemology. This folk dialecticism
differs from the rarified (“dialectical”) philosophies of Hegel and Marx in that these approaches often assume or insist on some original contradiction or opposition that is then
resolved; the Eastern folk dialectical epistemology Peng and Nisbett describe accepts and even
embraces contradiction rather than attempting
to “fix” or resolve it.
Peng and Nisbett (1999) described three assumptions that underpin the Eastern dialectical
epistemology. First, the principle of change
suggests that reality is a dynamic process; something need not be identical with itself because
reality is fluid and changing. Second, the principle of contradiction notes that, since change
is constant, contradiction is constant; the very
nature of the world is such that old and new,
good and bad, exist at the same time in the
same object or event. Third, the principle of
holism holds that, since change and contradiction are constant, nothing in human life or nature is isolated and independent; rather, all
things are related, and attempts to isolate elements of a larger whole can only be misleading.
Peng and Nisbett (1999) claimed that these
sets of assumptions form two kinds of folk epistemologies: a dialectical epistemology that is
more widespread in the East and a more linear/
logical epistemology that is more widespread
in the West. Of course, elements from each epistemology are shared by many or all cultures, but
the comparative prevalence of these implicit
theories suggests cross-cultural studies might
reveal how culture-specific epistemologies affect inference. We turn now to evidence on
culture and dialectical thinking.
Folk Wisdom on Dialectical Thinking Peng
and Nisbett (1999) examined folk knowledge
as embodied in books of proverbs. They found
that dialectical proverbs that pose a contradiction or assertion of instability (e.g., “Too humble is half-proud”) were more common among
Chinese proverbs than among English ones.
When nondialectical (e.g., “Half a loaf is better than none”) and dialectical proverbs were
selected from among Chinese and English
proverbs equally and given to Chinese and
American undergraduates to evaluate, Chinese
participants had a greater preference for the
Matsumoto, D. (Ed.). (2001). The handbook of culture and psychology. Retrieved from http://ebookcentral.proquest.com
Created from waldenu on 2018-03-27 06:39:15.
CULTURE AND HUMAN INFERENCE
dialectical proverbs than did American participants. The same pattern of preference emerged
with Yiddish proverbs, stimuli equally unfamiliar to both Chinese and Americans.
Copyright © 2001. Oxford University Press. All rights reserved.
Dialectical Resolution of Social Contradictions
Peng and Nisbett (1999) presented Chinese and
American students with a variety of contradictions drawn from everyday life. For example,
participants were asked to analyze conflicts between mothers and their daughters and between having fun and going to school. American responses tended to come down clearly in
favor of one side or the other (e.g., mothers
should respect their daughters’ independence).
Chinese responses were more likely to find a
middle way that attributed fault to both sides
and attempted to reconcile the contradiction
(e.g., both the mothers and the daughters failed
to understand each other).
Dialecticism and Preferred Argument Form In
a study examining argument preferences, Peng
(1997) gave Chinese and American participants
two different types of arguments—a logic-based
one refuting contradiction and a dialectical
one—for several issues. In one case, participants read arguments against Aristotle’s proposition that a heavier object falls to the ground
first. The logical argument summarized Galileo’s famous thought experiment: If a heavy
object is joined to a lighter one, they now have
a weight greater than the lighter object alone
and hence should fall faster; on the other hand,
extending Aristotle’s view, the lighter object
should act as a brake and therefore the combined object should fall more slowly. Since
these entailments form a contradiction, it is
possible to reject the original proposal that
objects of a different weight fall at different
speeds. The dialectical argument, meanwhile,
was based on a holistic approach to the problem: Since Aristotle isolated objects from possible surrounding factors (e.g., wind, weather,
and height), the proposition must be wrong.
For several such issues, Chinese expressed a
greater preference for the dialectical arguments,
while Americans were drawn to the linear, logical arguments.
Tolerance of Apparent Contradiction One of
the strongest implications of the notion that
Westerners adhere to a logical analysis of problems is that, when presented with contradictory
propositions, they should be inclined to reject
one in favor of the other. Easterners, on the
257
other hand, might be inclined to embrace both
propositions, finding them each to have merit.
In one study, Peng and Nisbett (1999) presented
participants either with one proposition or with
two propositions that were seemingly contradictory. For instance, one proposition used was,
“A developmental psychologist studied adolescent children and asserted that those children
who were less dependent on their parents and
had weaker family ties were generally more
mature.” In some cases, this was paired with a
second, apparently contradictory statement: “A
social psychologist studied young adults and
asserted that those who feel close to their families have more satisfying social relationships.”
Participants read one, the other, or both of these
and then rated the plausibility of the statements
they read.
Across five issues, Chinese and American
participants agreed on which of the two statements offered was more plausible (i.e., a main
effect of statement). However, when reading
the statements in pairs, Americans found the
predominantly plausible statement even more
plausible than when reading it alone: They bolstered their belief in the plausible statement
that it was presented along with a contradiction
(cf. Lord, Ross, & Lepper, 1979). In contrast,
Chinese participants expressed lower plausibility ratings for the predominant statement
when it was paired with a contradiction, seemingly compromising between the two perspectives.
Conclusions
Two decades ago, cognitive anthropologist Edwin Hutchins, like Nisbett and Ross, published
a book. His was titled Culture and Inference
(1980), and it contains a careful ethnography
of reasoning among the Trobriand Islanders.
Hutchins was working against arguments that
the Trobrianders and other such cultures lacked
concepts of causality and logic (D. D. Lee, 1940,
1949). Thus, Hutchins, ironically enough, was
making something of a universalist argument:
Sophisticated inferences are not the kind of
thing that only members of “civilized” cultures
can do. However, in the process of showing
that complex reasoning, such as modus tollens
and plausible inference, existed among the Trobrianders, Hutchins also delivered important
conclusions about the ways in which inferences differ across cultures: Reasoning, he concluded, is inseparably intertwined with cultural models. What is universal is our capacity
Matsumoto, D. (Ed.). (2001). The handbook of culture and psychology. Retrieved from http://ebookcentral.proquest.com
Created from waldenu on 2018-03-27 06:39:15.
258
CULTURE AND PERSONALITY
to infer and judge, but this is always and only
done in light of cultural models (see D’Andrade, 1995).
Over the last 20 years, cultural psychologists
have done much to qualify, interpret, and expand on the ideas of both Nisbett and Ross and
Hutchins. Those in cultural psychology know
much now about how inference unfolds in different ways in various cultures—and they are
poised to learn even more. The differences reviewed in this chapter defy simple summation,
but the highlights deserve to be recounted briefly. After doing so, we consider cultural differences in inference in light of the value, self,
and theory traditions.
Lessons on Cultural
Differences
Findings on cultural differences in inference
can be grouped into two broad categories: induction and deduction.
Copyright © 2001. Oxford University Press. All rights reserved.
Induction
Covariation detection is a basic form of induction: Given evidence of the cooccurrence of various events and features, how and when do
perceivers infer a connection? Research that
focuses on the holistic, dialectical epistemology associated with Chinese culture shows that
Chinese may be more attuned to relations
among stimuli in a field: They show fewer primacy effects than Americans, and compared to
Americans, their confidence in judging covariation tracks better with their actual accuracy.
Attribution has enjoyed considerable attention from cultural researchers, in part because
of the compelling differences that emerge. In
the domain of social attribution, scholars have
repeatedly shown that Americans tend to isolate single individuals as causes, while Asians
and other collectivists comparatively stress situations and groups as causes. Similarly, in the
physical domain, Chinese are more likely to
highlight the role of the field in explanations,
whereas Americans tend to focus on the internal properties of objects.
Cultural differences emerge in judgments
about persons as well, including inferences
about their personalities and their mental
states. Americans appear to share a dispositionalist folk theory, such that they see individuals
as having stable, internal, enduring dispositions, whereas Asians are more likely to see
persons as changeable and context bound. Likewise, in forming impressions of a target person,
Americans tend to prefer information directly
from that person, while Chinese are comparatively more interested in others’ views of the
target and information about the target’s context. Further, Americans seem to expect that
mental states are more readily inferred by a
person’s own statements; Chinese seem to base
inferences of mental states more heavily on
other, unspoken cues.
In the realm of categorization, culture shapes
category coherence by directing attention to
culturally important phenomena; as the priorities of cultures differ, so do their categories.
Further, Asians seem more likely than Americans to categorize things by their relations, such
as social obligations, rather than isolable features. Compared to Americans, Asians may also
be less attuned to categories in their inferences
and category learning. These findings are perhaps more intriguing in light of cultural research on the category of self. Considerable
scholarship shows that Asian concepts of self
are more socially diffused and context and relationship bound, while American concepts of
self are more concrete and abstract.
Deduction
Given premises in some logical relation, do
people in all cultures draw the same inference?
Studies of culture and syllogistic reasoning suggest that this question needs to be reconsidered.
Namely, what count as premises and logical
relations depends on the culture-specific models. Within a culture, framing logical questions
with ordinary knowledge rather than abstractions has a massive difference on performance.
It seems safe to conclude that people in all
cultures are capable of making complex inferences, but each culture does so within its own
models.
Cultural studies have also highlighted diversity in basic epistemologies of what counts as
evidence and the nature of truth—and differences in epistemology give rise to different styles
of reasoning and deduction. Chinese appear to
share a dialectical epistemology that stresses
the changing nature of reality and the enduring
presence of contradiction. This stands in contrast to a Western linear epistemology built on
notions of truth, identity, and noncontradiction. As a result, some scholars argue, Chinese
prefer to seek a compromise in the face of contradiction, whereas Americans pursue more exclusionary forms of truth and resolution.
Matsumoto, D. (Ed.). (2001). The handbook of culture and psychology. Retrieved from http://ebookcentral.proquest.com
Created from waldenu on 2018-03-27 06:39:15.
CULTURE AND HUMAN INFERENCE
Copyright © 2001. Oxford University Press. All rights reserved.
The Three Traditions:
Relation to Culture
and Inference
In the beginning of this chapter, we reviewed
three perspectives for approaching culture: the
value tradition, the self tradition, and the theory tradition. The value tradition, for instance,
has shown that individualists tend to isolate
single persons as causes. The self tradition, for
example, has revealed that the category of self
differs substantially across cultures. And, the
theory tradition can be seen in work on the role
of culturally bound epistemologies in reasoning. Each tradition, then, has shed light on the
question of culture and inference, but is there
some way of integrating these perspectives? Do
scholars and concerned readers have to place
their loyalty in one tradition to the exclusion
of the others? We suggest that a synthesis is
both possible and preferable, at least at the level
of describing how the phenomena targeted by
each tradition might relate. The result is a rich
way for thinking about how culture and inference relate.
A starting point for building the synthesis
is to consider what folk theories do and their
origin. Virtually by definition, theories (whether implicit folk ones or scientific ones) support
inferences: They guide how evidence is collected and interpreted and support judgments
that go beyond immediate data. Indeed, it
would be nearly impossible to describe everyday inference in a psychologically rich way
without resorting to some folk knowledge structures like implicit theories. Thus, to understand
cultural effects on inference in a proximal sense
implies understanding how cultural theories
are at work in ordinary judgment.
But what is the origin of theories? It seems
quite clear that cultural values must be an important source for theories: Values guide our
attention to what is good and important. Our
views of what the world is like are shaped by
what we think the world should be like. Asian
norms about the importance of groups and social relations, for instance, no doubt yield rich
folk theories about those entities. The dynamic
seems to be at work within the tradition of the
self as well, for norms about how to be a “good”
self are seen as yielding beliefs about what
the self “is.” And, as James (1890) notes, self
concepts have a wide-ranging role in psychological processes, so concepts of self are likely
intertwined with a host of other beliefs, such
as beliefs about others.
259
In short, implicit theories may play something of a mediating role between values and
the concept of self on the one hand and inferences on the other. Values and concepts of self
may have a more removed, distal effect on inferences, but a more proximal impact on beliefs.
This mediating model may seem complete, but
it fails to address a final important question:
What are inferences for? As S. T. Fiske (1992)
and others have observed, thinking is always
for something; we would add that what thinking is for differs across cultures. Why is it, for
instance, that people judge causes? On occasion, it might be a private act, meant to be
shared with no one. More often, though, such
inferences are shared and put to use in some
kind of action. Take the example of a transgression: We seek an explanation in order to act—to
prevent, to punish, to forgive, and so on. Our
implicit theories may guide an attribution inference, but the inference is not alone in shaping
action. Action is also shaped by cultural values
and concepts of self. In the case of transgressions, Western theories may isolate a single person as a cause, and Western values may imply
some form of person-directed retributive justice. East Asian theories, meanwhile, might
identify a group or situation as a cause, and
East Asian harmony values might prescribe collective responsibility as an outcome.
Values and concepts of self thus play a dual
role: First, they shape the theories that, in turn,
drive inferences; second, they shape the contexts in which the resulting inferences are
turned into action (see Figure 13.1). In this
scheme, it makes no sense to ask which of the
three traditions is the “best” approach to studying culture and inference. Rather, these three
traditions target different parts of a system of
the influence of culture on inference. Isolating
one set of relationships at a time (for instance,
between theories and inferences or between
values and theories) is a practical, and perhaps
necessary, research strategy, but scholars are
well served to acknowledge the broader system
of the relationship of culture with inference. A
full story of how culture affects inference must
address each of these components.1
Looking Ahead
What is next for the cultural psychology of human inference? Several challenges emerge from
our review of findings. The traditions of value
and self each contain differing perspectives, but
each has also been dominated by a central construct, individualism-collectivism in the case
Matsumoto, D. (Ed.). (2001). The handbook of culture and psychology. Retrieved from http://ebookcentral.proquest.com
Created from waldenu on 2018-03-27 06:39:15.
260
CULTURE AND PERSONALITY
Copyright © 2001. Oxford University Press. All rights reserved.
Figure 13.1 A Model of Cultural Influence on Human Inference
of values and independent-interdependent
selves in the tradition of self. One challenge for
these traditions will be to expand these dimensions. The 1996 work of Nisbett and Cohen on
the “culture of honor,” for instance, suggests
an important alternative to the constructs of
value and self.
For its part, the theory tradition continues
to grow. As it does, it may face the danger of
becoming fragmented. While it holds the promise of being psychologically precise and rich in
terms of describing cultural effects on inference, it runs the risk of targeting an ad hoc
collection of representations detached from
broader cultural patterns. Scholars in this tradition are challenged to describe connections,
both among the implicit theories they are studying and between those theories and other cultural constructs (such as values).
All these traditions are challenged to study
inference with new populations. Most work to
date has been done in the United States and
Asia. More work needs to explore inference in
other parts of the world, such as Africa. As
Nisbett and Cohen (1996) have shown, research
on values and judgments can also fruitfully examine cultural differences within countries. As
globalization and immigration continue, cultural clashes in inferences deserve increased
attention, as does the issue of acculturation.
Perhaps the most important challenge for
the study of culture and human judgment is
one shared with other areas of cultural psychological work: the need for methodologies
that are meaningful at both the psychological
and cultural levels (Peng, Nisbett, & Wong, 1997).
This general challenge requires approaches that
are tractable and precise and, at the same time,
nuanced and sensitive. Current approaches
vary in their strengths and weaknesses, and it
seems clear there is no single superior perspective. Postmodern approaches stress the uniqueness of cultures, but sometimes eschew opportunities for fruitful cross-cultural comparisons.
Cultural system approaches focus on the important everyday ecology of practices and institutions, but may omit descriptions of the
mediating psychology of cultural members. Dimensional or typological approaches stress important factors for arraying cultures, but run
the risk of glossing over rich systems of sensemaking that have psychological reality. Many
theory and value approaches helpfully focus
on psychologically important aspects of culture, but may leave the broader picture undescribed.
As psychological research moves to embrace
the role of culture, it will do well to retain
its guiding methodological principles. Among
others, these include objectivity (attempting to
Matsumoto, D. (Ed.). (2001). The handbook of culture and psychology. Retrieved from http://ebookcentral.proquest.com
Created from waldenu on 2018-03-27 06:39:15.
CULTURE AND HUMAN INFERENCE
observe and describe with a minimal influence
of personal bias), validity (a consistent concern
with measures and operationalizations), generalization (attempting to go beyond single cases
to reveal lawlike mechanisms and processes of
psychology), and causal explanation (a focus
on the causal relationships between factors).
Yet, perhaps new principles will need to be
integrated as well, including holism (seeing the
important connections between cultural components of sense-making), suprapersonal levels
of analysis (moving beyond the individual),
and qualitative approaches (reflecting the richness of culture). It may be that a combination
of approaches is required to satisfy all these
principles—and so flexibility may itself become the most important principle of all.
Looking back, an impressive amount of compelling scholarship has emerged on the topic
of culture and inference in the past few decades. The topic and some basic tenets have
come into focus, yet there is much more to do.
We hope and expect that 20 years hence our
current understanding will look well intentioned but naive in the face of accumulating
insights on how culture shapes human inference.
Notes
Copyright © 2001. Oxford University Press. All rights reserved.
This project was supported by the Regents’ Junior
Faculty Research Fellow Award by the University of
California to the first author. We thank Sanjay Srivastava, Michael Shin, Coline McConnel, and other
members of the U. C. Berkeley Culture and Cognition
Lab for their comments and suggestions
1. Note that this system resembles what philosophers and developmental psychologists have called
belief-desire psychology (see, e.g., Dennett, 1987;
Searle, 1983; Wellman, 1990). This view holds that
the keys to understanding everyday human action
(e.g., “Carl eats celery”) are desires (e.g., “Carl wants
to lose weight”) and beliefs (“Carl believes that celery
aids weight loss”). Similarly, this system suggests that
cultural values, like desires, shape action goals, while
cultural theory-driven inferences yield beliefs relevant to actions.
References
Allen, S. W., & Brooks, L. R. (1991). Specializing
the operation of an explicit rule. Journal of Experimental Psychology (General), 120, 3–19.
Alloy, L. B., & Tabachnik, N. (1984). Assessment of
covariation by human and animals: The joint
influence of prior expectations and current sit-
261
uational information. Psychological Review,
91, 112–149.
Ames, D. (1999). Folk psychology and social inference: Everyday solutions to the problem of
other minds. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, Berkeley.
Ames, D., & Peng, K. (1999a). Culture and person
perception: Impression cues that count in the
US and China. Manuscript in preparation,
University of California, Berkeley.
Ames, D., & Peng, K. (1999b). Psychology of meaning: Making sense of sense making. Manuscript in preparation, University of California,
Berkeley.
Atran, S. (1990). Cognitive foundations of natural
history. New York:
Atran, S., & Medin, D. (1997). Knowledge and action: Cultural models of nature and resources
management in Mesoamerica. In M. Z. Bazerman & D. Messick (Eds.), Environment, ethics,
and behavior: The psychology of environmental valuation and degradation (pp. 171–208).
San Francisco: New Lexington Press.
Baron, R. M., & Misovich, S. J. (1993). Dispositional knowing from an ecological perspective.
Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 19,
541–552.
Barsalou, L. W. (1983). Ad-hoc categories. Memory
and Categories, 11, 211–227.
Barsalou, L. W. (1985). Ideals, central tendency,
and frequency of instantiation as determinants
of graded structure in categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,
and Cognition, 11, 629–654.
Betancourt, H., & Lopez, S. R. (1993). The study of
culture, ethnicity, and race in American psychology. American Psychologist, 48, 629–637.
Boyer, P. (1993). The naturalness of religious ideas.
Berkeley: University of California Press.
Capra, F. (1975). The tao of physics. Berkeley, CA:
Shambala.
Chiu, C. Y., Hong, Y. I., & Dweck, C. S. (1997). Lay
dispositionism and implicit theories of personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 19–30.
Chiu, L.-H. (1972). A cross-cultural comparison of
cognitive styles in Chinese and American children. International Journal of Psychology, 7,
235–242.
Choi, I., & Nisbett, R. E. (1998). Situational salience and cultural differences in the correspondence bias and in the actor-observer bias.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
24, 949–960.
Choi, I., Nisbett, R., & Norenzayan, A. (1999).
Causal attribution across cultures: Variation
and universality. Psychological Bulletin, 125,
47–63.
Matsumoto, D. (Ed.). (2001). The handbook of culture and psychology. Retrieved from http://ebookcentral.proquest.com
Created from waldenu on 2018-03-27 06:39:15.
Copyright © 2001. Oxford University Press. All rights reserved.
262
CULTURE AND PERSONALITY
Choi, I., Nisbett, R. E., & Smith, E. E. (1997). Culture, category salience, and inductive reasoning. Cognition, 65, 15–32.
Cole, M. (1996). Cultural psychology: A once and
future discipline. Cambridge: Belknap Press of
Harvard University Press.
D’Andrade, R. G. (1995). The development of cognitive anthropology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Dennett, D. C. (1987). The intentional stance. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Douglas, M. (1982). In the active voice. London:
Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Dweck, C. S. (1996). Capturing the dynamic nature
of personality. Journal of Research in Personality, 30, 348–362.
Dweck, C. S., Chiu, C. Y., & Hong, Y. I. (1995). Implicit theories and their role in judgments and
reactions: A world from two perspectives. Psychological Inquiry, 6, 267–285.
Fiske, A. P. (1992). The four elementary forms of
sociality: Framework for a unified theory of sociality. Psychological Review, 99, 689–723.
Fiske, A. P., Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., & Nisbett,
R. E. (1998). The cultural matrix of social psychology. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Linzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology 4th
ed.) (pp. 915–981). Boston: McGraw-Hill.
Fiske, S. T. (1992). Thinking is for doing. Portraits
of social cognition from daguerreotype to laserphoto. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 877–889.
Gelman, S. A., & Medin, D. L. (1993). What’s so essential about essentialism? A different perspective on the interaction of perception, language, and conceptual knowledge. Cognitive
Development, 8, 157–167.
Gilbert, D. T., & Jones, E. E. (1986). Perceiver-induced constraint: Interpretations of self-generated reality. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 50, 269–280.
Gopnk, A., & Meltzoff, A. (1997). Words, thoughts,
and theories. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal
relations. New York: Wiley.
Heine, S., & Lehman, D. R. (1995). Cultural variation in unrealistic optimism: Does the West
feel more invulnerable than the East? Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 595–
607.
Heine, S., Lehman, D. R. (1997). Culture, dissonance, and self-affirmation. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 389–400.
Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture’s consequences: International differences in work-related values.
Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Holland, D., & Quinn, N. (1987). Cultural models
in language and thought. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Hutchins, E. (1980). Culture and inference. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology.
New York: Holt.
Ji, L., Peng, K., & Nisbett, R. E. (2000). Culture, control, and perception of relationships in the environment. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 78(5), 943–955.
Jones, E. E., & Harris, V. A. (1967). The attribution
of attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 3, 1–24.
Kitayama, S., & Masuda, T. (1997). Shaiaiteki ninshiki no bunkateki baikai model: Taiousei bias
no bunkashinrigakuteki kentou [Cultural psychology of social inference: The correspondence bias in Japan]. In K. Kashiwagi, S. Kitayama, & H. Azuma (Eds.), Bunkashinrigaju:
riron to jisho [Cultural psychology: Theory
and evidence]. Tokyo: University of Tokyo
Press.
Knowles, E. D., and Ames, D. R. (1999). The mentalistic nature of folk person concepts: Individual and trait term differences. Manuscript in
preparation, University of California, Berkeley.
Lee, D. D. (1940). A primitive system of values.
Journal of Philosophy, 7, 355–379.
Lee, D. D. (1949). Being and value in a primitive
culture. Journal of Philosophy, 48, 401–415.
Lee, F., Hallahan, M., & Herzog, T. (1996). Explaining real life events: How culture and domain
shape attributions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 732–741.
Leung, K. (1997). Negotiation and reward allocations across cultures. In P. C. Earley & M. Erez
(Eds.), New perspectives on international industrial/organizational psychology (pp. 640–
675). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Lewin, K. (1935). A dynamic theory of personality.
New York: McGraw-Hill.
Lillard, A. (1998). Ethnopsychologies: Cultural variations in theories of mind. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 3–32.
Liu, S. H. (1974). The use of analogy and symbolism in traditional Chinese philosophy. Journal
of Chinese Philosophy, 1, 313–338.
Lloyd, G. E. R. (1990). Demystifying mentalities.
New York: Cambridge University Press.
Lopez, A., Atran, S., Coley, J. D., Medin, D. L., &
Smith, E. E. (1997). The tree of life: Universal
and cultural features of folkbiological taxonomies and inductions. Cognitive Psychology,
32, 251–295.
Lord, C., Ross, L., & Lepper, M. (1979). Biased assimilation and attitude polarization: The effects of prior theories on subsequently considered evidence. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 37, 2098–2109.
Luria, A. R. (1931). Psychological expedition to
Central Asia. Science, 74, 383–384.
Matsumoto, D. (Ed.). (2001). The handbook of culture and psychology. Retrieved from http://ebookcentral.proquest.com
Created from waldenu on 2018-03-27 06:39:15.
Copyright © 2001. Oxford University Press. All rights reserved.
CULTURE AND HUMAN INFERENCE
Malt, B. (1995). Category coherence in cross-cultural perspective. Cognitive Psychology, 29,
85–148.
Markus, H. R., & Cross, S. (1990). The interpersonal self. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of
personality: Theory and research (pp. 576–
608). New York: Guilford.
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Cultural variation in the self-concept. Culture and self: Implications for cognition, emotion and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–253.
McCloskey, M. (1983). Intuitive physics. Scientific
American, 248, 122–130.
Menon, T., Morris, M., Chiu, C. Y., & Hong, Y. I.
(1999). Culture and the construal of agency:
Attribution to individual versus group dispositions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 701–727.
Miller, J. G. (1984). Culture and the development
of everyday social explanation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 961–978.
Morris, M. W., Nisbett, R. E., & Peng, K. (1995).
Causal understanding across domains and cultures. In D. Sperber, D. Premack, & A. J. Premack (Eds.), Causal cognition: A multidisciplinary debate (pp. 577–612). Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Morris, M. W., & Peng, K. (1994). Culture and
cause: American and Chinese attributions for
social and physical events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 949–971.
Moscovici, S. (1984). The phenomenon of social
representations. In R. Farr & S. Moscovici
(Eds.), Social representations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Murphy, G. L., & Medin, D. L. (1985). The role of
theories in conceptual coherence. Psychological Review, 92, 289–316.
Needham, J. (1954). Science and civilization in
China (Vol. 1). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Needham, J. (1962). Science and civilization in
China: Vol. 4. Physics and physical technology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Nisbett, R. E., & Cohen, D. (1996). Culture of
honor: The psychology of violence in the
South. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
Nisbett, R. E., & Ross, L. (1980). Human Inference:
Strategies and Shortcomings of Social Judgment. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: PrenticeHall.
Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I., & Norenzayan, A.
(2001). Culture and system of thought: Holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychological Review, 108, 1–20.
Norenzayan, A., Choi, I., & Nisbett, R. (1999). Eastern and western perceptions of causality for
social behavior: Lay theories about personalities and social situations. In D. Prentice & D.
263
Miller (Eds.), Cultural divides: Understanding
and overcoming group conflict (pp. 239–272).
New York: Sage.
Norenzayan, A., Nisbett, R. E., Smith, E. E., & Kim,
B. J. (2000). Rules vs. similarity as a basis for
reasoning and judgment in East and West. Unpublished manuscript, University of Illinois.
Osherson, D. N., Smith, E. E., Wilkie, O., López,
A., & Shafir, E. (1990). Category-based induction. Psychological Review, 97, 185–200.
Peng, K. (1997). Naive dialecticism and its effects
on reasoning and judgment about contradiction. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor.
Peng, K., & Ames, D. (in press). Psychology of dialectical thinking. In N. J. Smelser & P. B.
Baltes (Eds.), International encyclopedia of
the social and behavioral sciences. Oxford, England: Elsevier Science.
Peng, K., & Knowles, E. (2000). Culture, ethnicity
and attribution of physical causality. Personality and social psychology bulletin (under review).
Peng, K., & Nisbett, R. E. (1996). Cross-cultural similarities and differences in the understanding
of physical causality. In G. Shields & M. Shale
(Eds.), Science and culture: Proceedings of the
seventh interdisciplinary conference on science and culture. Frankfort, KY: University
Graphics.
Peng, K., & Nisbett, R. E. (1999). Culture, dialectics, and reasoning about contradiction. American Psychologist, 54, 741–754.
Peng, K., Nisbett, R., & Wong, N. Y. C. (1997). Validity problems comparing values across cultures and possible solution. Psychological
Methods, 2, 329–344.
Rohner, R. (1984). Toward a conception of culture
for cross-cultural psychology. Journal of CrossCultural Psychology, 15, 111–138.
Rosch, E., & Mervis, C. B. (1975). Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of
categories. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 573–605.
Rosch, E., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W. D., Johnson, D., &
Boyes-Braem, P. (1976). Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 382–
439.
Ross, L. (1977). The intuitive psychologist and his
shortcomings. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances
in experimental social psychology (Vol. 10)
(pp. 172–200). New York: Academic Press.
Schwartz, S. H. (1991). The universal content and
structure of values: Theoretical advances and
empirical tests in 20 countries. Advanced in
Experimental Social Psychology, 25, 1–65.
Schwartz, S. H., & Sagiv, L. (1995). Identifying culture-specifics in the content and structure of
value. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology,
26, 92–116.
Matsumoto, D. (Ed.). (2001). The handbook of culture and psychology. Retrieved from http://ebookcentral.proquest.com
Created from waldenu on 2018-03-27 06:39:15.
264
CULTURE AND PERSONALITY
Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1987). What do
people think they’re doing? Action identification and human behavior. Psychological Review, 94, 3–15.
Vosniadou, S. (1994). Universal and culture-specific properties of children’s mental models of
the earth. In L. A. Hirschfeld & S. A. Gelman
(Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity
in cognition and culture. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Wagner, W. (1997). Local knowledge, social representations and psychological theory. In K.
Leung; U. Kim, S. Yamaguchi, & Y. Kashima
(Eds.), Progress in Asian social psychology.
Singapore: Wiley.
Wegner, D. M., & Vallacher, R. R. (1977). Implicit
psychology: An introduction to social cognition. New York: Oxford University Press.
Wellman, H. M. (1990). The child’s theory of mind.
Cambridge: MIT Press.
Whiting, B. B. (1976). The problem of the packaged
variable. In K. F. Reigel & J. A. Meacham
(Eds.), The developing individual in a changing world. The Hague: Mouton.
Zhang, D. L., & Chen, Z. Y. (1991). Zhongguo Siwei
Pianxiang [The orientation of Chinese thinking]. Beijing: Social Science Press.
Zukav, G. (1980). The dancing Wu Li masters: An
overview of the new physics. New York: Quill
Morrow.
Copyright © 2001. Oxford University Press. All rights reserved.
Shweder, R. (1995). Cultural psychology: What is
it? In N. R. Goldberger & J. B. Veroff (Eds.),
The culture and psychology reader (pp. 41–
86). New York: New York University Press.
Searle, J. R. (1983). Intentionality, an essay in the
philosophy of mind. New York: Cambridge
University Press.
Singelis, T. M. (1994). The measurement of independent and interdependent self-construals.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
20, 580–591.
Smith, P. B., & Bond, M. H. (1999). Social psychology across cultures. Needham Heights, MA:
Allyn & Bacon.
Sperber, D. (1990). The epidemiology of beliefs. In
C. Fraser & G. Gaskell (Eds.), The social psychological study of widespread beliefs (pp.
24–44). Oxford, England: Clarendon Press.
Stich, S. (1990). The fragmentation of reason. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Strauss, C., & Quinn, N. (1997). A cognitive theory
of cultural meaning. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Tanaka, J. W., & Taylor, M. E. (1991). Categorization
and expertise: Is the basic level in the eye of
the beholder? Cognitive psychology, 23, 457–
482.
Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism and collectivism. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
Matsumoto, D. (Ed.). (2001). The handbook of culture and psychology. Retrieved from http://ebookcentral.proquest.com
Created from waldenu on 2018-03-27 06:39:15.
17
Culture and Social Cognition
Toward a Social Psychology of Cultural Dynamics
Copyright © 2001. Oxford University Press. All rights reserved.
YOSHIHISA KASHIMA
Social cognition, broadly defined as human thought about social behavior, has received considerable attention in the literature since the cognitive revolution of the
1960s and, indeed, has become one of the most important areas of study in mainstream psychology. Within this large area, cross-cultural research on social cognition
has come to play an extremely important role in defining issues and in influencing research and theory.
In this chapter, Kashima presents a comprehensive overview of the area of culture
and social cognition. He first begins with an excellent discussion of the concept of
culture in psychology, distinguishing the concept of culture as meaning from cultural
dynamics. As Kashima suggests, cultural dynamics has to do with the paradoxical
phenomenon of cultural stability and change, which arises from two contemporary
views of culture: system oriented and practice oriented. These definitions and discussions about the concept of culture are essential to Kashima’s later points about the necessity for the development and creation of theories and research on cultural dynamics, which represent a further evolution of research and thinking about social
cognition, and an integration of approaches and knowledge from various disciplines.
The bulk of Kashima’s chapter is devoted to a state-of-the-art review of research
on culture and social cognition. This review promises to be one of the most comprehensive reviews on this topic. He begins with a treatment of the historical context of
early social cognition research and with a presentation of background studies in the
area. His detailed review spans such topics as availability of concepts, causal attributions, self-concepts, social and personal explanation, self-evaluation, and others. He
delineates many of the issues that are highlighted through his thorough evaluation of
the research literature, pointing out both what we know and what we do not in each
area. The reader is sure to view this area of his chapter as an important resource for
this line of inquiry.
Using his review of the literature as a platform, Kashima delineates his ideas concerning future research and theoretical work in the area. With regard to future empirical work, he suggests that two topics in the area of culture and social cognition—the
explanation of social action and the maintenance of self-regard—deserve closer scrutiny and further research in the future. In particular, while much is known about
what North Americans tend to do with regard to these topics, relatively much less
325
Matsumoto, D. (Ed.). (2001). The handbook of culture and psychology. Retrieved from http://ebookcentral.proquest.com
Created from waldenu on 2018-03-27 06:40:21.
326
CULTURE AND SOCIAL BEHAVIOR
Copyright © 2001. Oxford University Press. All rights reserved.
is known about other people around the world, leaving this area ripe for investigation. In particular, the holistic approach and worldview perspective of East Asians
may bring insights into this area of psychological functioning that heretofore were
unconsidered.
Clearly, however, the major thrust of Kashima’s argument for future work concerns
the creation of what he terms the social cognition of cultural dynamics. As he explains at the beginning of his chapter and throughout his literature review, much of
the early social cognition research and theories were characterized by an individualistic conception of meaning, according to which meaning is constructed solely within
an individual person’s mind. There are many reasons for these biases in the literature, including the fact that most research was done in the United States by American researchers. Even research that was conducted outside the United States was often conducted by researchers who were trained in the United States (and thus
influenced by Western educational dogma) or influenced by these factors. In the future, however, greater emphasis will need to be placed on the development of a theoretical framework that incorporates both cognitive and communicative processes in
understanding cultural dynamics—that is, the processes by which cultural meanings
are constructed in ongoing social activities among multiple individuals, as well as
within an individual’s mind. This view of social cognition is inherently more complex, involving relational, collective, and individual issues, including the incorporation of context and history, as well as future and present time orientations. For these
reasons, the development of such a theoretical viewpoint will necessitate fundamental changes in the ways in which we do research, which will ultimately lead to ways
in which we understand human behavior in potentially profoundly different ways
than now. This development of new theories and methodologies to ensure the continued evolution of knowledge in this area of psychology is commensurate with a message given by all authors throughout this volume.
Until recently, cult...
Purchase answer to see full
attachment