Broken Windows - The Atlantic
1/8/18, 3'12 PM
Broken Windows
The police and neighborhood safety
GEORGE L. KELLING AND JAMES Q. WILSON
Like The Atlantic? Subscribe to The Atlantic
Daily, our free weekday email newsletter.
|
MARCH 1982 ISSUE
Email
|
U.S.
SIGN UP
In the mid-1970s The State of New Jersey announced a "Safe and Clean
Neighborhoods Program," designed to improve the quality of community life in
twenty-eight cities. As part of that program, the state provided money to help
cities take police officers out of their patrol cars and assign them to walking
beats. The governor and other state officials were enthusiastic about using foot
patrol as a way of cutting crime, but many police chiefs were skeptical. Foot
patrol, in their eyes, had been pretty much discredited. It reduced the mobility of
the police, who thus had difficulty responding to citizen calls for service, and it
weakened headquarters control over patrol officers.
Many police officers also disliked foot patrol, but for different reasons: it was
hard work, it kept them outside on cold, rainy nights, and it reduced their
chances for making a "good pinch." In some departments, assigning officers to
foot patrol had been used as a form of punishment. And academic experts on
policing doubted that foot patrol would have any impact on crime rates; it was, in
the opinion of most, little more than a sop to public opinion. But since the state
https://www.theatlantic.com/magazine/archive/1982/03/broken-windows/304465/
Page 1 of 22
Broken Windows - The Atlantic
1/8/18, 3'12 PM
was paying for it, the local authorities were willing to go along.
Five years after the program started, the Police Foundation, in Washington,
D.C., published an evaluation of the foot-patrol project. Based on its analysis of a
carefully controlled experiment carried out chiefly in Newark, the foundation
concluded, to the surprise of hardly anyone, that foot patrol had not reduced
crime rates. But residents of the foot patrolled neighborhoods seemed to feel
more secure than persons in other areas, tended to believe that crime had been
reduced, and seemed to take fewer steps to protect themselves from crime
(staying at home with the doors locked, for example). Moreover, citizens in the
foot-patrol areas had a more favorable opinion of the police than did those living
elsewhere. And officers walking beats had higher morale, greater job
satisfaction, and a more favorable attitude toward citizens in their
neighborhoods than did officers assigned to patrol cars.
These findings may be taken as evidence that the skeptics were right- foot patrol
has no effect on crime; it merely fools the citizens into thinking that they are
safer. But in our view, and in the view of the authors of the Police Foundation
study (of whom Kelling was one), the citizens of Newark were not fooled at all.
They knew what the foot-patrol officers were doing, they knew it was different
from what motorized officers do, and they knew that having officers walk beats
did in fact make their neighborhoods safer.
But how can a neighborhood be "safer" when the crime rate has not gone down—
in fact, may have gone up? Finding the answer requires first that we understand
what most often frightens people in public places. Many citizens, of course, are
primarily frightened by crime, especially crime involving a sudden, violent
attack by a stranger. This risk is very real, in Newark as in many large cities. But
we tend to overlook another source of fear—the fear of being bothered by
https://www.theatlantic.com/magazine/archive/1982/03/broken-windows/304465/
Page 2 of 22
Broken Windows - The Atlantic
1/8/18, 3'12 PM
disorderly people. Not violent people, nor, necessarily, criminals, but
disreputable or obstreperous or unpredictable people: panhandlers, drunks,
addicts, rowdy teenagers, prostitutes, loiterers, the mentally disturbed.
What foot-patrol officers did was to elevate, to the extent they could, the level of
public order in these neighborhoods. Though the neighborhoods were
predominantly black and the foot patrolmen were mostly white, this "ordermaintenance" function of the police was performed to the general satisfaction of
both parties.
One of us (Kelling) spent many hours walking with Newark foot-patrol officers to
see how they defined "order" and what they did to maintain it. One beat was
typical: a busy but dilapidated area in the heart of Newark, with many
abandoned buildings, marginal shops (several of which prominently displayed
knives and straight-edged razors in their windows), one large department store,
and, most important, a train station and several major bus stops. Though the
area was run-down, its streets were filled with people, because it was a major
transportation center. The good order of this area was important not only to
those who lived and worked there but also to many others, who had to move
through it on their way home, to supermarkets, or to factories.
The people on the street were primarily black; the officer who walked the street
was white. The people were made up of "regulars" and "strangers." Regulars
included both "decent folk" and some drunks and derelicts who were always
there but who "knew their place." Strangers were, well, strangers, and viewed
suspiciously, sometimes apprehensively. The officer—call him Kelly—knew who
the regulars were, and they knew him. As he saw his job, he was to keep an eye
on strangers, and make certain that the disreputable regulars observed some
informal but widely understood rules. Drunks and addicts could sit on the
https://www.theatlantic.com/magazine/archive/1982/03/broken-windows/304465/
Page 3 of 22
Broken Windows - The Atlantic
1/8/18, 3'12 PM
stoops, but could not lie down. People could drink on side streets, but not at the
main intersection. Bottles had to be in paper bags. Talking to, bothering, or
begging from people waiting at the bus stop was strictly forbidden. If a dispute
erupted between a businessman and a customer, the businessman was assumed
to be right, especially if the customer was a stranger. If a stranger loitered, Kelly
would ask him if he had any means of support and what his business was; if he
gave unsatisfactory answers, he was sent on his way. Persons who broke the
informal rules, especially those who bothered people waiting at bus stops, were
arrested for vagrancy. Noisy teenagers were told to keep quiet.
These rules were defined and enforced in collaboration with the "regulars" on
the street. Another neighborhood might have different rules, but these,
everybody understood, were the rules for this neighborhood. If someone violated
them, the regulars not only turned to Kelly for help but also ridiculed the
violator. Sometimes what Kelly did could be described as "enforcing the law,"
but just as often it involved taking informal or extralegal steps to help protect
what the neighborhood had decided was the appropriate level of public order.
Some of the things he did probably would not withstand a legal challenge.
A determined skeptic might acknowledge that a skilled foot-patrol officer can
maintain order but still insist that this sort of "order" has little to do with the real
sources of community fear—that is, with violent crime. To a degree, that is true.
But two things must be borne in mind. First, outside observers should not
assume that they know how much of the anxiety now endemic in many big-city
neighborhoods stems from a fear of "real" crime and how much from a sense
that the street is disorderly, a source of distasteful, worrisome encounters. The
people of Newark, to judge from their behavior and their remarks to
interviewers, apparently assign a high value to public order, and feel relieved and
reassured when the police help them maintain that order.
https://www.theatlantic.com/magazine/archive/1982/03/broken-windows/304465/
Page 4 of 22
Broken Windows - The Atlantic
1/8/18, 3'12 PM
Second, at the community level, disorder and crime are usually inextricably
linked, in a kind of developmental sequence. Social psychologists and police
officers tend to agree that if a window in a building is broken and is left
unrepaired, all the rest of the windows will soon be broken. This is as true in nice
neighborhoods as in rundown ones. Window-breaking does not necessarily
occur on a large scale because some areas are inhabited by determined windowbreakers whereas others are populated by window-lovers; rather, one unrepaired
broken window is a signal that no one cares, and so breaking more windows costs
nothing. (It has always been fun.)
Philip Zimbardo, a Stanford psychologist, reported in 1969 on some
experiments testing the broken-window theory. He arranged to have an
automobile without license plates parked with its hood up on a street in the
Bronx and a comparable automobile on a street in Palo Alto, California. The car
in the Bronx was attacked by "vandals" within ten minutes of its
"abandonment." The first to arrive were a family—father, mother, and young son
—who removed the radiator and battery. Within twenty-four hours, virtually
everything of value had been removed. Then random destruction began—
windows were smashed, parts torn off, upholstery ripped. Children began to use
the car as a playground. Most of the adult "vandals" were well-dressed,
apparently clean-cut whites. The car in Palo Alto sat untouched for more than a
week. Then Zimbardo smashed part of it with a sledgehammer. Soon, passersby
were joining in. Within a few hours, the car had been turned upside down and
utterly destroyed. Again, the "vandals" appeared to be primarily respectable
whites.
Untended property becomes fair game for people out for fun or plunder and even
for people who ordinarily would not dream of doing such things and who
probably consider themselves law-abiding. Because of the nature of community
https://www.theatlantic.com/magazine/archive/1982/03/broken-windows/304465/
Page 5 of 22
Broken Windows - The Atlantic
1/8/18, 3'12 PM
life in the Bronx—its anonymity, the frequency with which cars are abandoned
and things are stolen or broken, the past experience of "no one caring"—
vandalism begins much more quickly than it does in staid Palo Alto, where
people have come to believe that private possessions are cared for, and that
mischievous behavior is costly. But vandalism can occur anywhere once
communal barriers—the sense of mutual regard and the obligations of civility—
are lowered by actions that seem to signal that "no one cares."
We suggest that "untended" behavior also leads to the breakdown of community
controls. A stable neighborhood of families who care for their homes, mind each
other's children, and confidently frown on unwanted intruders can change, in a
few years or even a few months, to an inhospitable and frightening jungle. A
piece of property is abandoned, weeds grow up, a window is smashed. Adults
stop scolding rowdy children; the children, emboldened, become more rowdy.
Families move out, unattached adults move in. Teenagers gather in front of the
corner store. The merchant asks them to move; they refuse. Fights occur. Litter
accumulates. People start drinking in front of the grocery; in time, an inebriate
slumps to the sidewalk and is allowed to sleep it off. Pedestrians are approached
by panhandlers.
At this point it is not inevitable that serious crime will flourish or violent attacks
on strangers will occur. But many residents will think that crime, especially
violent crime, is on the rise, and they will modify their behavior accordingly.
They will use the streets less often, and when on the streets will stay apart from
their fellows, moving with averted eyes, silent lips, and hurried steps. "Don't get
involved." For some residents, this growing atomization will matter little,
because the neighborhood is not their "home" but "the place where they live."
Their interests are elsewhere; they are cosmopolitans. But it will matter greatly
to other people, whose lives derive meaning and satisfaction from local
https://www.theatlantic.com/magazine/archive/1982/03/broken-windows/304465/
Page 6 of 22
Broken Windows - The Atlantic
1/8/18, 3'12 PM
attachments rather than worldly involvement; for them, the neighborhood will
cease to exist except for a few reliable friends whom they arrange to meet.
Such an area is vulnerable to criminal invasion. Though it is not inevitable, it is
more likely that here, rather than in places where people are confident they can
regulate public behavior by informal controls, drugs will change hands,
prostitutes will solicit, and cars will be stripped. That the drunks will be robbed
by boys who do it as a lark, and the prostitutes' customers will be robbed by men
who do it purposefully and perhaps violently. That muggings will occur.
Among those who often find it difficult to move away from this are the elderly.
Surveys of citizens suggest that the elderly are much less likely to be the victims
of crime than younger persons, and some have inferred from this that the wellknown fear of crime voiced by the elderly is an exaggeration: perhaps we ought
not to design special programs to protect older persons; perhaps we should even
try to talk them out of their mistaken fears. This argument misses the point. The
prospect of a confrontation with an obstreperous teenager or a drunken
panhandler can be as fear-inducing for defenseless persons as the prospect of
meeting an actual robber; indeed, to a defenseless person, the two kinds of
confrontation are often indistinguishable. Moreover, the lower rate at which the
elderly are victimized is a measure of the steps they have already taken—chiefly,
staying behind locked doors—to minimize the risks they face. Young men are
more frequently attacked than older women, not because they are easier or more
lucrative targets but because they are on the streets more.
Nor is the connection between disorderliness and fear made only by the elderly.
Susan Estrich, of the Harvard Law School, has recently gathered together a
number of surveys on the sources of public fear. One, done in Portland, Oregon,
indicated that three fourths of the adults interviewed cross to the other side of a
https://www.theatlantic.com/magazine/archive/1982/03/broken-windows/304465/
Page 7 of 22
Broken Windows - The Atlantic
1/8/18, 3'12 PM
street when they see a gang of teenagers; another survey, in Baltimore,
discovered that nearly half would cross the street to avoid even a single strange
youth. When an interviewer asked people in a housing project where the most
dangerous spot was, they mentioned a place where young persons gathered to
drink and play music, despite the fact that not a single crime had occurred there.
In Boston public housing projects, the greatest fear was expressed by persons
living in the buildings where disorderliness and incivility, not crime, were the
greatest. Knowing this helps one understand the significance of such otherwise
harmless displays as subway graffiti. As Nathan Glazer has written, the
proliferation of graffiti, even when not obscene, confronts the subway rider with
the inescapable knowledge that the environment he must endure for an hour or
more a day is uncontrolled and uncontrollable, and that anyone can invade it to
do whatever damage and mischief the mind suggests."
In response to fear people avoid one another, weakening controls. Sometimes
they call the police. Patrol cars arrive, an occasional arrest occurs but crime
continues and disorder is not abated. Citizens complain to the police chief, but
he explains that his department is low on personnel and that the courts do not
punish petty or first-time offenders. To the residents, the police who arrive in
squad cars are either ineffective or uncaring: to the police, the residents are
animals who deserve each other. The citizens may soon stop calling the police,
because "they can't do anything."
The process we call urban decay has occurred for centuries in every city. But
what is happening today is different in at least two important respects. First, in
the period before, say, World War II, city dwellers- because of money costs,
transportation difficulties, familial and church connections—could rarely move
away from neighborhood problems. When movement did occur, it tended to be
along public-transit routes. Now mobility has become exceptionally easy for all
https://www.theatlantic.com/magazine/archive/1982/03/broken-windows/304465/
Page 8 of 22
Broken Windows - The Atlantic
1/8/18, 3'12 PM
but the poorest or those who are blocked by racial prejudice. Earlier crime waves
had a kind of built-in self-correcting mechanism: the determination of a
neighborhood or community to reassert control over its turf. Areas in Chicago,
New York, and Boston would experience crime and gang wars, and then
normalcy would return, as the families for whom no alternative residences were
possible reclaimed their authority over the streets.
Second, the police in this earlier period assisted in that reassertion of authority
by acting, sometimes violently, on behalf of the community. Young toughs were
roughed up, people were arrested "on suspicion" or for vagrancy, and prostitutes
and petty thieves were routed. "Rights" were something enjoyed by decent folk,
and perhaps also by the serious professional criminal, who avoided violence and
could afford a lawyer.
This pattern of policing was not an aberration or the result of occasional excess.
From the earliest days of the nation, the police function was seen primarily as
that of a night watchman: to maintain order against the chief threats to order—
fire, wild animals, and disreputable behavior. Solving crimes was viewed not as a
police responsibility but as a private one. In the March, 1969, Atlantic, one of us
(Wilson) wrote a brief account of how the police role had slowly changed from
maintaining order to fighting crimes. The change began with the creation of
private detectives (often ex-criminals), who worked on a contingency-fee basis
for individuals who had suffered losses. In time, the detectives were absorbed in
municipal agencies and paid a regular salary simultaneously, the responsibility
for prosecuting thieves was shifted from the aggrieved private citizen to the
professional prosecutor. This process was not complete in most places until the
twentieth century.
In the l960s, when urban riots were a major problem, social scientists began to
https://www.theatlantic.com/magazine/archive/1982/03/broken-windows/304465/
Page 9 of 22
Broken Windows - The Atlantic
1/8/18, 3'12 PM
explore carefully the order maintenance function of the police, and to suggest
ways of improving it—not to make streets safer (its original function) but to
reduce the incidence of mass violence. Order maintenance became, to a degree,
coterminous with "community relations." But, as the crime wave that began in
the early l960s continued without abatement throughout the decade and into the
1970s, attention shifted to the role of the police as crime-fighters. Studies of
police behavior ceased, by and large, to be accounts of the order-maintenance
function and became, instead, efforts to propose and test ways whereby the
police could solve more crimes, make more arrests, and gather better evidence.
If these things could be done, social scientists assumed, citizens would be less
fearful.
A great deal was accomplished during this transition, as both police chiefs and
outside experts emphasized the crime-fighting function in their plans, in the
allocation of resources, and in deployment of personnel. The police may well
have become better crime-fighters as a result. And doubtless they remained
aware of their responsibility for order. But the link between order-maintenance
and crime-prevention, so obvious to earlier generations, was forgotten.
That link is similar to the process whereby one broken window becomes many.
The citizen who fears the ill-smelling drunk, the rowdy teenager, or the
importuning beggar is not merely expressing his distaste for unseemly behavior;
he is also giving voice to a bit of folk wisdom that happens to be a correct
generalization—namely, that serious street crime flourishes in areas in which
disorderly behavior goes unchecked. The unchecked panhandler is, in effect, the
first broken window. Muggers and robbers, whether opportunistic or
professional, believe they reduce their chances of being caught or even identified
if they operate on streets where potential victims are already intimidated by
prevailing conditions. If the neighborhood cannot keep a bothersome
https://www.theatlantic.com/magazine/archive/1982/03/broken-windows/304465/
Page 10 of 22
Broken Windows - The Atlantic
1/8/18, 3'12 PM
panhandler from annoying passersby, the thief may reason, it is even less likely
to call the police to identify a potential mugger or to interfere if the mugging
actually takes place.
Some police administrators concede that this process occurs, but argue that
motorized-patrol officers can deal with it as effectively as foot patrol officers. We
are not so sure. In theory, an officer in a squad car can observe as much as an
officer on foot; in theory, the former can talk to as many people as the latter. But
the reality of police-citizen encounters is powerfully altered by the automobile.
An officer on foot cannot separate himself from the street people; if he is
approached, only his uniform and his personality can help him manage whatever
is about to happen. And he can never be certain what that will be—a request for
directions, a plea for help, an angry denunciation, a teasing remark, a confused
babble, a threatening gesture.
In a car, an officer is more likely to deal with street people by rolling down the
window and looking at them. The door and the window exclude the approaching
citizen; they are a barrier. Some officers take advantage of this barrier, perhaps
unconsciously, by acting differently if in the car than they would on foot. We
have seen this countless times. The police car pulls up to a corner where
teenagers are gathered. The window is rolled down. The officer stares at the
youths. They stare back. The officer says to one, "C'mere." He saunters over,
conveying to his friends by his elaborately casual style the idea that he is not
intimidated by authority. What's your name?" "Chuck." "Chuck who?" "Chuck
Jones." "What'ya doing, Chuck?" "Nothin'." "Got a P.O. [parole officer]?" "Nah."
"Sure?" "Yeah." "Stay out of trouble, Chuckie." Meanwhile, the other boys laugh
and exchange comments among themselves, probably at the officer's expense.
The officer stares harder. He cannot be certain what is being said, nor can he join
in and, by displaying his own skill at street banter, prove that he cannot be "put
https://www.theatlantic.com/magazine/archive/1982/03/broken-windows/304465/
Page 11 of 22
Broken Windows - The Atlantic
1/8/18, 3'12 PM
down." In the process, the officer has learned almost nothing, and the boys have
decided the officer is an alien force who can safely be disregarded, even mocked.
Our experience is that most citizens like to talk to a police officer. Such
exchanges give them a sense of importance, provide them with the basis for
gossip, and allow them to explain to the authorities what is worrying them
(whereby they gain a modest but significant sense of having "done something"
about the problem). You approach a person on foot more easily, and talk to him
more readily, than you do a person in a car. Moreover, you can more easily retain
some anonymity if you draw an officer aside for a private chat. Suppose you want
to pass on a tip about who is stealing handbags, or who offered to sell you a stolen
TV. In the inner city, the culprit, in all likelihood, lives nearby. To walk up to a
marked patrol car and lean in the window is to convey a visible signal that you are
a "fink."
The essence of the police role in maintaining order is to reinforce the informal
control mechanisms of the community itself. The police cannot, without
committing extraordinary resources, provide a substitute for that informal
control. On the other hand, to reinforce those natural forces the police must
accommodate them. And therein lies the problem.
Should police activity on the street be shaped, in important ways, by the
standards of the neighborhood rather than by the rules of the state? Over the past
two decades, the shift of police from order-maintenance to law enforcement has
brought them increasingly under the influence of legal restrictions, provoked by
media complaints and enforced by court decisions and departmental orders. As
a consequence, the order maintenance functions of the police are now governed
by rules developed to control police relations with suspected criminals. This is,
we think, an entirely new development. For centuries, the role of the police as
https://www.theatlantic.com/magazine/archive/1982/03/broken-windows/304465/
Page 12 of 22
Broken Windows - The Atlantic
1/8/18, 3'12 PM
watchmen was judged primarily not in terms of its compliance with appropriate
procedures but rather in terms of its attaining a desired objective. The objective
was order, an inherently ambiguous term but a condition that people in a given
community recognized when they saw it. The means were the same as those the
community itself would employ, if its members were sufficiently determined,
courageous, and authoritative. Detecting and apprehending criminals, by
contrast, was a means to an end, not an end in itself; a judicial determination of
guilt or innocence was the hoped-for result of the law-enforcement mode. From
the first, the police were expected to follow rules defining that process, though
states differed in how stringent the rules should be. The criminal-apprehension
process was always understood to involve individual rights, the violation of
which was unacceptable because it meant that the violating officer would be
acting as a judge and jury—and that was not his job. Guilt or innocence was to be
determined by universal standards under special procedures.
Ordinarily, no judge or jury ever sees the persons caught up in a dispute over the
appropriate level of neighborhood order. That is true not only because most
cases are handled informally on the street but also because no universal
standards are available to settle arguments over disorder, and thus a judge may
not be any wiser or more effective than a police officer. Until quite recently in
many states, and even today in some places, the police made arrests on such
charges as "suspicious person" or "vagrancy" or "public drunkenness"—charges
with scarcely any legal meaning. These charges exist not because society wants
judges to punish vagrants or drunks but because it wants an officer to have the
legal tools to remove undesirable persons from a neighborhood when informal
efforts to preserve order in the streets have failed.
Once we begin to think of all aspects of police work as involving the application
of universal rules under special procedures, we inevitably ask what constitutes
https://www.theatlantic.com/magazine/archive/1982/03/broken-windows/304465/
Page 13 of 22
Broken Windows - The Atlantic
1/8/18, 3'12 PM
an "undesirable person" and why we should "criminalize" vagrancy or
drunkenness. A strong and commendable desire to see that people are treated
fairly makes us worry about allowing the police to rout persons who are
undesirable by some vague or parochial standard. A growing and not-socommendable utilitarianism leads us to doubt that any behavior that does not
"hurt" another person should be made illegal. And thus many of us who watch
over the police are reluctant to allow them to perform, in the only way they can, a
function that every neighborhood desperately wants them to perform.
This wish to "decriminalize" disreputable behavior that "harms no one"- and
thus remove the ultimate sanction the police can employ to maintain
neighborhood order—is, we think, a mistake. Arresting a single drunk or a single
vagrant who has harmed no identifiable person seems unjust, and in a sense it is.
But failing to do anything about a score of drunks or a hundred vagrants may
destroy an entire community. A particular rule that seems to make sense in the
individual case makes no sense when it is made a universal rule and applied to all
cases. It makes no sense because it fails to take into account the connection
between one broken window left untended and a thousand broken windows. Of
course, agencies other than the police could attend to the problems posed by
drunks or the mentally ill, but in most communities especially where the
"deinstitutionalization" movement has been strong—they do not.
The concern about equity is more serious. We might agree that certain behavior
makes one person more undesirable than another but how do we ensure that age
or skin color or national origin or harmless mannerisms will not also become the
basis for distinguishing the undesirable from the desirable? How do we ensure, in
short, that the police do not become the agents of neighborhood bigotry?
We can offer no wholly satisfactory answer to this important question. We are
https://www.theatlantic.com/magazine/archive/1982/03/broken-windows/304465/
Page 14 of 22
Broken Windows - The Atlantic
1/8/18, 3'12 PM
not confident that there is a satisfactory answer except to hope that by their
selection, training, and supervision, the police will be inculcated with a clear
sense of the outer limit of their discretionary authority. That limit, roughly, is
this—the police exist to help regulate behavior, not to maintain the racial or
ethnic purity of a neighborhood.
Consider the case of the Robert Taylor Homes in Chicago, one of the largest
public-housing projects in the country. It is home for nearly 20,000 people, all
black, and extends over ninety-two acres along South State Street. It was named
after a distinguished black who had been, during the 1940s, chairman of the
Chicago Housing Authority. Not long after it opened, in 1962, relations between
project residents and the police deteriorated badly. The citizens felt that the
police were insensitive or brutal; the police, in turn, complained of unprovoked
attacks on them. Some Chicago officers tell of times when they were afraid to
enter the Homes. Crime rates soared.
Today, the atmosphere has changed. Police-citizen relations have improved—
apparently, both sides learned something from the earlier experience. Recently,
a boy stole a purse and ran off. Several young persons who saw the theft
voluntarily passed along to the police information on the identity and residence
of the thief, and they did this publicly, with friends and neighbors looking on.
But problems persist, chief among them the presence of youth gangs that
terrorize residents and recruit members in the project. The people expect the
police to "do something" about this, and the police are determined to do just
that.
But do what? Though the police can obviously make arrests whenever a gang
member breaks the law, a gang can form, recruit, and congregate without
breaking the law. And only a tiny fraction of gang-related crimes can be solved
https://www.theatlantic.com/magazine/archive/1982/03/broken-windows/304465/
Page 15 of 22
Broken Windows - The Atlantic
1/8/18, 3'12 PM
by an arrest; thus, if an arrest is the only recourse for the police, the residents'
fears will go unassuaged. The police will soon feel helpless, and the residents will
again believe that the police "do nothing." What the police in fact do is to chase
known gang members out of the project. In the words of one officer, "We kick
ass." Project residents both know and approve of this. The tacit police-citizen
alliance in the project is reinforced by the police view that the cops and the gangs
are the two rival sources of power in the area, and that the gangs are not going to
win.
None of this is easily reconciled with any conception of due process or fair
treatment. Since both residents and gang members are black, race is not a factor.
But it could be. Suppose a white project confronted a black gang, or vice versa.
We would be apprehensive about the police taking sides. But the substantive
problem remains the same: how can the police strengthen the informal socialcontrol mechanisms of natural communities in order to minimize fear in public
places? Law enforcement, per se, is no answer: a gang can weaken or destroy a
community by standing about in a menacing fashion and speaking rudely to
passersby without breaking the law.
We have difficulty thinking about such matters, not simply because the ethical
and legal issues are so complex but because we have become accustomed to
thinking of the law in essentially individualistic terms. The law defines my rights,
punishes his behavior and is applied by that officer because of this harm. We
assume, in thinking this way, that what is good for the individual will be good for
the community and what doesn't matter when it happens to one person won't
matter if it happens to many. Ordinarily, those are plausible assumptions. But in
cases where behavior that is tolerable to one person is intolerable to many others,
the reactions of the others—fear, withdrawal, flight—may ultimately make
matters worse for everyone, including the individual who first professed his
https://www.theatlantic.com/magazine/archive/1982/03/broken-windows/304465/
Page 16 of 22
Broken Windows - The Atlantic
1/8/18, 3'12 PM
indifference.
It may be their greater sensitivity to communal as opposed to individual needs
that helps explain why the residents of small communities are more satisfied
with their police than are the residents of similar neighborhoods in big cities.
Elinor Ostrom and her co-workers at Indiana University compared the
perception of police services in two poor, all-black Illinois towns—Phoenix and
East Chicago Heights with those of three comparable all-black neighborhoods in
Chicago. The level of criminal victimization and the quality of police-community
relations appeared to be about the same in the towns and the Chicago
neighborhoods. But the citizens living in their own villages were much more
likely than those living in the Chicago neighborhoods to say that they do not stay
at home for fear of crime, to agree that the local police have "the right to take any
action necessary" to deal with problems, and to agree that the police "look out
for the needs of the average citizen." It is possible that the residents and the
police of the small towns saw themselves as engaged in a collaborative effort to
maintain a certain standard of communal life, whereas those of the big city felt
themselves to be simply requesting and supplying particular services on an
individual basis.
If this is true, how should a wise police chief deploy his meager forces? The first
answer is that nobody knows for certain, and the most prudent course of action
would be to try further variations on the Newark experiment, to see more
precisely what works in what kinds of neighborhoods. The second answer is also
a hedge—many aspects of order maintenance in neighborhoods can probably
best be handled in ways that involve the police minimally if at all. A busy bustling
shopping center and a quiet, well-tended suburb may need almost no visible
police presence. In both cases, the ratio of respectable to disreputable people is
ordinarily so high as to make informal social control effective.
https://www.theatlantic.com/magazine/archive/1982/03/broken-windows/304465/
Page 17 of 22
Broken Windows - The Atlantic
1/8/18, 3'12 PM
Even in areas that are in jeopardy from disorderly elements, citizen action
without substantial police involvement may be sufficient. Meetings between
teenagers who like to hang out on a particular corner and adults who want to use
that corner might well lead to an amicable agreement on a set of rules about how
many people can be allowed to congregate, where, and when.
Where no understanding is possible—or if possible, not observed—citizen patrols
may be a sufficient response. There are two traditions of communal involvement
in maintaining order: One, that of the "community watchmen," is as old as the
first settlement of the New World. Until well into the nineteenth century,
volunteer watchmen, not policemen, patrolled their communities to keep order.
They did so, by and large, without taking the law into their own hands—without,
that is, punishing persons or using force. Their presence deterred disorder or
alerted the community to disorder that could not be deterred. There are
hundreds of such efforts today in communities all across the nation. Perhaps the
best known is that of the Guardian Angels, a group of unarmed young persons in
distinctive berets and T-shirts, who first came to public attention when they
began patrolling the New York City subways but who claim now to have chapters
in more than thirty American cities. Unfortunately, we have little information
about the effect of these groups on crime. It is possible, however, that whatever
their effect on crime, citizens find their presence reassuring, and that they thus
contribute to maintaining a sense of order and civility.
The second tradition is that of the "vigilante." Rarely a feature of the settled
communities of the East, it was primarily to be found in those frontier towns that
grew up in advance of the reach of government. More than 350 vigilante groups
are known to have existed; their distinctive feature was that their members did
take the law into their own hands, by acting as judge, jury, and often executioner
as well as policeman. Today, the vigilante movement is conspicuous by its rarity,
https://www.theatlantic.com/magazine/archive/1982/03/broken-windows/304465/
Page 18 of 22
Broken Windows - The Atlantic
1/8/18, 3'12 PM
despite the great fear expressed by citizens that the older cities are becoming
"urban frontiers." But some community-watchmen groups have skirted the line,
and others may cross it in the future. An ambiguous case, reported in The Wall
Street Journal involved a citizens' patrol in the Silver Lake area of Belleville, New
Jersey. A leader told the reporter, "We look for outsiders." If a few teenagers
from outside the neighborhood enter it, "we ask them their business," he said.
"If they say they're going down the street to see Mrs. Jones, fine, we let them
pass. But then we follow them down the block to make sure they're really going
to see Mrs. Jones."
Though citizens can do a great deal, the police are plainly the key to order
maintenance. For one thing, many communities, such as the Robert Taylor
Homes, cannot do the job by themselves. For another, no citizen in a
neighborhood, even an organized one, is likely to feel the sense of responsibility
that wearing a badge confers. Psychologists have done many studies on why
people fail to go to the aid of persons being attacked or seeking help, and they
have learned that the cause is not "apathy" or "selfishness" but the absence of
some plausible grounds for feeling that one must personally accept
responsibility. Ironically, avoiding responsibility is easier when a lot of people
are standing about. On streets and in public places, where order is so important,
many people are likely to be "around," a fact that reduces the chance of any one
person acting as the agent of the community. The police officer's uniform singles
him out as a person who must accept responsibility if asked. In addition, officers,
more easily than their fellow citizens, can be expected to distinguish between
what is necessary to protect the safety of the street and what merely protects its
ethnic purity.
But the police forces of America are losing, not gaining, members. Some cities
have suffered substantial cuts in the number of officers available for duty. These
https://www.theatlantic.com/magazine/archive/1982/03/broken-windows/304465/
Page 19 of 22
Broken Windows - The Atlantic
1/8/18, 3'12 PM
cuts are not likely to be reversed in the near future. Therefore, each department
must assign its existing officers with great care. Some neighborhoods are so
demoralized and crime-ridden as to make foot patrol useless; the best the police
can do with limited resources is respond to the enormous number of calls for
service. Other neighborhoods are so stable and serene as to make foot patrol
unnecessary. The key is to identify neighborhoods at the tipping point—where
the public order is deteriorating but not unreclaimable, where the streets are
used frequently but by apprehensive people, where a window is likely to be
broken at any time, and must quickly be fixed if all are not to be shattered.
Most police departments do not have ways of systematically identifying such
areas and assigning officers to them. Officers are assigned on the basis of crime
rates (meaning that marginally threatened areas are often stripped so that police
can investigate crimes in areas where the situation is hopeless) or on the basis of
calls for service (despite the fact that most citizens do not call the police when
they are merely frightened or annoyed). To allocate patrol wisely, the
department must look at the neighborhoods and decide, from first-hand
evidence, where an additional officer will make the greatest difference in
promoting a sense of safety.
One way to stretch limited police resources is being tried in some public housing
projects. Tenant organizations hire off-duty police officers for patrol work in their
buildings. The costs are not high (at least not per resident), the officer likes the
additional income, and the residents feel safer. Such arrangements are probably
more successful than hiring private watchmen, and the Newark experiment
helps us understand why. A private security guard may deter crime or
misconduct by his presence, and he may go to the aid of persons needing help,
but he may well not intervene—that is, control or drive away—someone
challenging community standards. Being a sworn officer—a "real cop"—seems to
https://www.theatlantic.com/magazine/archive/1982/03/broken-windows/304465/
Page 20 of 22
Broken Windows - The Atlantic
1/8/18, 3'12 PM
give one the confidence, the sense of duty, and the aura of authority necessary to
perform this difficult task.
Patrol officers might be encouraged to go to and from duty stations on public
transportation and, while on the bus or subway car, enforce rules about smoking,
drinking, disorderly conduct, and the like. The enforcement need involve
nothing more than ejecting the offender (the offense, after all, is not one with
which a booking officer or a judge wishes to be bothered). Perhaps the random
but relentless maintenance of standards on buses would lead to conditions on
buses that approximate the level of civility we now take for granted on airplanes.
But the most important requirement is to think that to maintain order in
precarious situations is a vital job. The police know this is one of their functions,
and they also believe, correctly, that it cannot be done to the exclusion of
criminal investigation and responding to calls. We may have encouraged them to
suppose, however, on the basis of our oft-repeated concerns about serious,
violent crime, that they will be judged exclusively on their capacity as crimefighters. To the extent that this is the case, police administrators will continue to
concentrate police personnel in the highest-crime areas (though not necessarily
in the areas most vulnerable to criminal invasion), emphasize their training in
the law and criminal apprehension (and not their training in managing street
life), and join too quickly in campaigns to decriminalize "harmless" behavior
(though public drunkenness, street prostitution, and pornographic displays can
destroy a community more quickly than any team of professional burglars).
Above all, we must return to our long-abandoned view that the police ought to
protect communities as well as individuals. Our crime statistics and
victimization surveys measure individual losses, but they do not measure
communal losses. Just as physicians now recognize the importance of fostering
https://www.theatlantic.com/magazine/archive/1982/03/broken-windows/304465/
Page 21 of 22
Broken Windows - The Atlantic
1/8/18, 3'12 PM
health rather than simply treating illness, so the police—and the rest of us—ought
to recognize the importance of maintaining, intact, communities without broken
windows.
LATEST VIDEO
DOW
L O A D P Dog
D F Grooming
The Weird, Wondrous World
ofN
Competitive
View
PDF,are
Download
Here
At the world's largest grooming To
show,
dogs
transformed
into fantastical works of art.
https://www.theatlantic.com/magazine/archive/1982/03/broken-windows/304465/
Page 22 of 22
Race
and Justice
http://raj.sagepub.com/
Racial Disparity in Police Contacts
Robert D. Crutchfield, Martie L. Skinner, Kevin P. Haggerty, Anne McGlynn and Richard
F. Catalano
Race and Justice 2012 2: 179
DOI: 10.1177/2153368712448063
The online version of this article can be found at:
http://raj.sagepub.com/content/2/3/179
Published by:
http://www.sagepublications.com
On behalf of:
Official Journal of the American Society of Criminology, Division on People on Color and Crime
Additional services and information for Race and Justice can be found at:
Email Alerts: http://raj.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts
Subscriptions: http://raj.sagepub.com/subscriptions
Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav
Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
Citations: http://raj.sagepub.com/content/2/3/179.refs.html
>> Version of Record - Jun 29, 2012
What is This?
Downloaded from raj.sagepub.com at University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill on August 5, 2014
Racial Disparity in Police
Contacts
Race and Justice
2(3) 179-202
ª The Author(s) 2012
Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/2153368712448063
http://raj.sagepub.com
Robert D. Crutchfield1, Martie L. Skinner1,
Kevin P. Haggerty1, Anne McGlynn1, and
Richard F. Catalano1
Abstract
Criminologists agree the race disparity in arrests cannot be fully explained by differences in criminal behavior. The authors examine social environment factors that may
lead to racial differences in police contact in early adolescence, including family, peers,
school, and community. Data are from 331 eighth-grade students. Blacks were almost
twice as likely as Whites to report a police contact. Blacks reported more property
crime but not more violent crime than Whites. Police contacts were increased by
having a parent who had been arrested, a sibling involved in criminal activity, higher
observed reward for negative behavior, having school disciplinary actions, and knowing adults who engaged in substance abuse or criminal behavior. Race differences in
police contacts were partially attributable to more school discipline.
Keywords
race disparity, police contacts, environment
Introduction
For more than 30 years, criminologists have studied racial differences in criminal
justice experiences. Studies have considered both juvenile and adult patterns of arrest
(Piliavin & Briar, 1964), conviction (Adler, 2006; Harmon, 2004), and sentencing
(Kleck, 1981). These studies have traced individuals in the criminal (Engen, Gainey,
Crutchfield, & Weis, 2003) and juvenile justice systems (Bridges & Steen, 1998), and
a number of studies have considered correlates of aggregate race differences in
criminal justice processing (Blumstein, 1982; Bridges & Crutchfield, 1988). Results
1
University of Washington, Seattle, WA, USA
Corresponding Author:
Martie L. Skinner, University of Washington, 9725 3rd Ave. NE, Suite 401, Seattle, WA 98115, USA
Email: skinnm@u.washington.edu
Downloaded from raj.sagepub.com at University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill on August 5, 2014
180
Race and Justice 2(3)
vary about how much racial disparity exists and about the proportion of observed
differences that can be attributed to legally relevant variables, (e.g., criminal involvement, offense seriousness, and offenders’ criminal histories). Though criminologists do not agree on the proportion of racial differences in criminal justice processing
that can be explained by legal differences, they do concur that not all of the observable
disparity can be explained by legally relevant race differences (Blumstein, 1982;
Crutchfield, Bridges, & Pitchford, 1994; Crutchfield, Fernandes, & Martinez, 2010;
Langan, 1985). What then explains why people of color have more contacts with police,
have higher arrest and conviction rates, and are more likely to spend time in prison?
The popular answer is discrimination on the part of police, prosecutors, and judges.
Unfortunately, we cannot directly assess with standard quantitative analytic
methods either specific acts of discrimination or racist sentiments on the part of
criminal justice officials while performing their duties. Not surprisingly, officials
generally do not reveal on questionnaires or in interviews that they are prejudiced.
Laboratory research on implicit bias (Greenwald, Oakes, & Hoffman, 2003;
Joy-Gaba & Nosek, 2010) has made important strides measuring unconscious
reactions to race by studying physiological changes produced by controlled stimuli, but this methodology does not apply well to field studies to definitively
draw conclusions about discriminatory attitudes or behaviors in criminal justice
settings. Qualitative studies have reported subtle forms of racial biases on the part
of court actors (Bridges & Steen, 1998; Harris, 2009). So discrimination remains
a viable interpretation of legally unexplained racial differences in criminal justice
decisions; but one difficult to measure in large field studies.
Another possible explanation is seemingly racially neutral criminal justice practices,
such as bail standards that emphasize the importance of defendant characteristics, disadvantage some segments of the population (e.g., residential stability disadvantaging
migrant workers). Here we focus on racial differences in police contacts for juveniles.
Obviously, one reason that young people come into contact with police is delinquency.
We address two questions: first, are there racial differences in police contacts which are
not explained by criminal involvement, and second, are there features of social life that
help explain these differences. Differential contacts with the police are important because
they potentially put young people who have these experiences at risk in other aspects of
their lives. We more fully discuss these risks below.
Garland (2001, p. 114) argues that in the wake of the upheaval, riots, and criticisms
in the 1960s and 1970s, police strove to become more professional and used more
technology, and moved away from close community contact. Because that approach
did not produce reductions in crime, police departments changed their strategies,
leading to widespread adoption of community policing practices in the last decades of
the 20th century. The earlier period, where professionalization was stressed, led to
poor interaction with the populace, especially poor and minority communities. The
newer community policing approach brings officers into more daily contact with the
public. This means that young people are now more likely to encounter police in a
broad array of their social environments, including in their neighborhoods and
schools.
Downloaded from raj.sagepub.com at University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill on August 5, 2014
Crutchfield et al.
181
If the social environments of young people of color and White juveniles differ, the
former may be at greater risk of having contacts with police independent of their
involvement in delinquency. If the former are more likely to live, go to school, and
work where there is greater police presence or where there is more surveillance, they
may be more likely to come to the attention of the criminal justice system. For
instance, Krivo and Peterson (1996) report that racial residential segregation consigns
African Americans to neighborhoods where they are more likely to experience victimization. To the extent that police departments focus patrols where there is more
crime (Bayley, 2008; Brunson & Weitzer, 2009), or where police perceive there to be
more crime (Beckett, Nyrop, & Pfingst, 2006), African Americans are at greater risk
of encounters with police. In this article, we will examine several important ‘‘environments’’ to assess the extent to which, in the face of community policing, they
increase the likelihood that young African Americans will be more likely to have
police contacts than Whites above and beyond what would be expected by observable
differences in delinquency.
Racial differences in criminal justice processing go to the heart of our collective
notions of fairness and justice. When the questions are about juveniles they take on
added importance. Labeling theorists, more than three decades ago (Becker, 1963;
Schur, 1969) convinced us, and continue to affirm (Bernburg, Krohn, & Rivera,
2006; Tapia, 2011) the formative influence of interactions with agents of social
control. Braithwaite (1989), in making the case for re-integrative shaming, criticized normal justice procedures for engaging in ‘‘shaming’’ that is disintegrative
(stigmatization). Here we take their invocations to heart in our effort to study social
environment factors that contribute to race differences in police contacts. When
teens have adverse interactions with police, the consequences can be longer lasting
than for adults (Brunson & Weitzer, 2009). If they are not treated fairly, or if they
perceive that they are not treated fairly, their future behavior and interactions with
law enforcement might be negatively affected. The question of the propriety of the
behavior of police and other justice actors is also important to communities of
color. When communities observe differences that cannot be explained, it
potentially gives credence to an injustice narrative that harms community–police
relations. So, as Garland (2001) described, officers engaging in community policing
seek to have more interactions with citizens, but this presents risk for perceptions of
police, and for the youths themselves who interact with officers. Here we do not
test Garland’s assertion that community policing leads to more public/police interaction. He more than adequately makes the case, as does the continuing existence
of the Federal COPS Office in the Justice Department, whose website states their
mission as to ‘‘advance the practice of community policing in America’s state, local
and tribal law enforcement agencies’’ (http://www.cops.usdoj.gov/). Our purpose
here is to examine the extent to which, in the context of contemporary policing,
there are racial disparities in juveniles’ contacts with police that cannot be
accounted for by differential delinquency involvement. And, if there are unaccounted for disparities, how do the social environments experienced by juveniles
explain them?
Downloaded from raj.sagepub.com at University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill on August 5, 2014
182
Race and Justice 2(3)
Here we are interested not in arrest or subsequent decision points in the juvenile
justice system, but in contact with police. Of course, contact is a necessary condition
for arrest, but we should no more presume that the former has only benign effects on
children, than we should assume that all contacts with police are negative. While some
effects of contacts appear immediately (e.g., arrest and its aftermath), others may
show up later, some much later. The nature and consequences of such effects is an
empirical question that gains importance if there are racial differences in likelihood
that juveniles have contact with officers. Skogan (2006) reported that while negative
interactions between citizens and police have the predictable effect of increasing
hostile feelings and cynicism, positive interactions do not help, they have no substantial effect at all. One wonders if such patterns are exacerbated in minority communities where predominantly negative narratives about police–minority community
relations are present.
Racial Disparities in Crime and Criminal Justice
We are not the first to analyze social environments and police contacts. Stewart,
Baumer, Brunson, and Simons (2009) studied neighborhood context and Black
youths’ perceptions of police treatment. The environment did matter. Black youths
were more likely to have adverse interactions with police when they were in White
neighborhoods, especially those with growing Black populations. Similar patterns
were uncovered by Brunson and Weitzer (2009) in a qualitative study of both Black
and White juveniles in low-income communities. Tapia (2010) found that both race
and social class were significant predictors of juvenile arrests, but interestingly, being
‘‘out of place,’’ a minority person in a nonminority neighborhood, resulted in stronger
effects for higher income Blacks than for those of lower socioeconomic status (SES).
Here we add to this line of research by examining the experiences of young Black and
White juveniles in two important social environments, as well as individual and family characteristics, while accounting for self-reported level of involvement in
delinquency.
Offenders, including juveniles, are arrested because they do something wrong.
Perhaps, people have contact with police because of their suspicious behavior or
because they are in places where they probably should not be. Though some people
concur with such statements (Wilbanks, 1987), research results have long shown that
it is not so simple. For nearly 40 years, criminologists have periodically explored the
factors which lead police to make an arrest once they have made contact with a
suspect. Piliavin and Briar (1964) stressed the importance of suspects’ demeanor.
Black and Reiss (1970) found that the seriousness of offense and the wishes of victims
(when present) influence police the most in their decisions to make an arrest.
Recently, Beckett and her colleagues (2006) reported that police drug enforcement
practices affect who is arrested more than behavior patterns of users and sellers. Other
researchers have also reported that suspects’ demeanor and nonlegal factors such as
race, social class, and gender affect police decision making when they encounter
citizens (Dai & Nation, 2009). And, Brunson and Weitzer (2011) have documented
Downloaded from raj.sagepub.com at University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill on August 5, 2014
Crutchfield et al.
183
that citizens, particularly in inner-city neighborhoods, try to prepare their children for
‘‘unwelcome’’ contact with police because of the potential for escalation during
encounters.
Generally, researchers have found that not all of the observed racial differences in
police contact, arrest, or incarceration can be explained by patterns of criminal
behavior. Three coordinated studies, in Denver, Rochester, and Seattle, used existing
longitudinal data sets to examine the extent to which arrest and charging decisions
could be accounted for by self-reported involvement in delinquency (Huizinga et al.,
2007). While there were modest differences across the sites, the investigators all found
that the racial differences in arrest were attenuated by differences in self-reported
offending, but the race differences in police contacts remained statistically significant
and substantial. These researchers, like others who have studied racial disparities,
acknowledge that discrimination may be a part of the explanation of higher arrest and
charging of Black youth, but they cannot dismiss other unmeasured reasons for the
unexplained race differences. By examining critical environments that may lead to
more police contact for Black juveniles beyond what would be expected by differences in offending behavior, we can specify important, previously unmeasured forces
affecting racial differences. The two environments that we focus on are school and
community, while also considering individual, familial, and peer influences.
Race and Environment
When discussing discrimination, the general public and many criminal and juvenile
justice practitioners mean that criminal justice agents make racially prejudiced
decisions to arrest, prosecute, or to sentence. It is possible, even likely, that this
happens sometimes. But this alone is too limited of a conception of how race can
negatively affect outcomes. In the United States, African Americans are at greater risk
of victimization, school dropout, unemployment, and a host of other problems, in part,
because they live in racially segregated communities (Massey, 1990; Peterson &
Krivo, 1999, 2010; Williams & Collins, 2001). Racial residential segregation is a
direct result of discriminatory practices in real estate markets and in mortgage lending
(Charles, 2003; Munnell, Tootell, Browne, & McEneaney, 1996; Oliver & Shapiro,
1995; Ross & Yinger, 2002). The consequences of segregated communities are
examples of institutionalized racism, not just individual prejudice. The environments
in which African Americans live are the consequences of continuing racialized patterns of social life (Massey & Denton, 1993; Peterson & Krivo, 2010).
Peterson and Krivo (2010) advance what they call ‘‘a structural race perspective’’
to explain how stratification affects those living in Black and Latino neighborhoods.
They write: ‘‘ . . . the internal structural conditions of neighborhoods and the circumstances of surrounding areas are fundamentally racialized in ways that specifically reinforce and maintain superiority in the local contexts of Whites, and
widespread deprivation in the contexts of African Americans, Latinos, and sometimes
others’’ (Peterson & Krivo, 2010, p. 115). In the current analyses, we integrate a basic
tenant of labeling theory, that particular social statuses (such as race), help to
Downloaded from raj.sagepub.com at University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill on August 5, 2014
184
Race and Justice 2(3)
determine who is subjected to social control, with the structural race perspective of
Peterson and Krivo, to add a micro-level component that allows us to specify elements
of environments that may cause the negative outcomes from contemporary policing
practices written about by Garland (2001). And these negative outcomes add to the
structured inequality faced by people of color.
We expect that characteristics of juveniles will in part explain variation in their
contacts with police. Those engaging in delinquency, reasonably, will have more
police contact, but several other individual-level patterns may also increase the probability of contact. While these are not legally relevant factors that can be said to
‘‘account’’ for observed racial differences in police interactions, familial and peer
characteristics may lead to differential contact. We discuss our reasons for these
predictions below. We also expect that two particular environments, school and
community, are especially important for explaining racial differences in police
contacts. Important racial differences in these environments make it more likely
that African Americans will experience more police contacts than would be
expected by any observably higher involvement in self-reported delinquent behavior. Police presence in schools, particularly in schools in ‘‘trouble areas,’’ and
officer engagement in ‘‘problem communities’’ increase the likelihood of having
contact with officers.
Clearly, these two environments, schools and communities, are neither mutually
exclusive nor independent of one another or from familial or peer group characteristics. Several careful qualitative studies demonstrate this in examinations of school
violence. Brunson and Miller (2009) found that violence in school is linked to the
disadvantage and violence that occurs in communities where students live.
Mateu-Gelabert (2000) used ethnography to study the bidirectionality of school and
neighborhood violence. And in a subsequent study, he reports that schools that foster
commitment to education can counter the negative effects of street codes that students
are exposed to in their home communities (Mateu-Gelabert & Lune, 2007). Sullivan
(2002) argues that to understand school violence, the individual, school, and community factors must be addressed within a nested conceptualization. Hagan, Hirschfield,
and Shedd (2002) demonstrated that there are links between neighborhoods and
schools where serious violence has occurred. Also, families select neighborhoods
based on multiple factors, including cost, and in doing so make choices about schools
and the peers to which their children will be exposed. And Hellman and Beaton (1986)
report that school violence in middle school is not linked to communities, but violence
in high schools is.
Why do we predict that family factors will influence police contacts independent of
self-reported delinquency? Family factors, including parental monitoring, parent–
child attachment, and the number of siblings influence children’s behavior and experiences. We have included both problematic (risk) and nonproblematic (protective)
family functioning as potential contributors to differential contacts with police. If a
household has an abusive or alcoholic parent, the familial interaction and the child are
affected, and such factors may especially increase the likelihood that members of the
family, including children, will come to the attention of authorities.
Downloaded from raj.sagepub.com at University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill on August 5, 2014
Crutchfield et al.
185
How else might ‘‘bad families’’ lead to more police contacts? We can begin to
answer this question by turning to old-school (and now very much discredited as
social science) criminology, where the Jukes and the Kallikaks, two 19th-century families, were cited in very early works about the intergenerational transmission of criminal behavior (Sutherland, 1924). Presumably, local authorities of that day would have
looked at the younger members of the Juke and Kallikak clans when there was trouble
about because they were so frequently involved in trouble. A more compelling argument, though, for how families might increase police contacts, independent of the
behavior of children, is provided by Anderson (1999). Members of ‘‘street families,’’
adhering to the ‘‘code of the street,’’ will get in trouble with the law (Anderson, 1999).
In this era of community policing, it is likely that effective officers will have knowledge of who are in such families. In these situations, they may be more likely to seek
out members of those families for questioning, just as 19th-century constables looked
to the Jukes and the Kallikaks when something bad happened. Families will not only
affect the behavior of juveniles, but we predict that, independent of that behavior, children from ‘‘problem families’’ will get more police attention, warranted or not.
Family influences might be considered those most proximal to the teen, whereas
peer influences are only slightly less so. Associating with deviant peers is one of the
primary risk factors for engaging in problem behavior (Brody et al., 2001), but may
also lead more directly to contact with law enforcement. As might be the case with
family members, peers may also have had previous contact with law enforcement.
Teens who associate with other teens who engage in illegal activity are more likely to
experience a police contact regardless of their own illegal activity.
No institution other than the family so dominates children’s lives as their school. A
major portion of their waking hours are spent there and, at least for those who do
homework, school affects even the time not spent there. School also heavily influences young people’s behavior. Those who are educationally successful are more
likely to develop attachments to school and have positive feelings toward their
teachers, and as a result are less likely to be delinquent (Hamre & Pianta, 2001;
Newcomb et al., 2002). School environments affect children’s social lives in positive
directions through extracurricular activities, friendships, and budding love lives; and
in negative directions through rivalries, bullies, and gangs. Peer relations in school are
important determinants of young people’s social status (Corsaro & Eder, 1990). Some
students go to schools that must be equipped with metal detectors, others do not. Some
schools have guards or police on hand while others have the luxury of not worrying so
much about security. It is likely that these and other environmental differences will
affect both delinquency and police contact. And because of the racial residential
distribution of the American population, school environments vary by race.
School environments are likely to affect the probability that a student will have
police contact, above what might be expected based on behavior, in two ways: directly
in school and indirectly in the community. First, if there is a police presence in the
schools, officers may become involved in school discipline issues that would not have
provoked a police response in the community. During the period when the data that we
are using was collected, all Seattle middle schools had officers assigned to them.1
Downloaded from raj.sagepub.com at University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill on August 5, 2014
186
Race and Justice 2(3)
Typically, one officer was assigned to two or three schools. The actual police presence
in schools depended on the perceived need. Likely those schools with more discipline
problems or those with more ‘‘at-risk’’ students had more actual ‘‘officer present’’
times. The officers had some discretion in how they allocated their time. It is easy to
imagine that schools may have used the officers in ‘‘get tough’’ discipline strategies,
but Seattle Police Department policy was that they not be involved in standard,
noncriminal problems. But readers should recognize that officers may have intervened
in the belief that early intervention would deter wayward children from a life of crime.
This is, of course, the philosophy of the early juvenile courts (Platt, 1969).
A second means by which school environments might produce racial disparities in
police contacts is school discipline. Kirk (2008) reports there are common (school and
community) social control linkages between school suspension and juvenile arrest.
And other researchers report that African American students are more likely to be
suspended or expelled from school (Eitle & Eitle, 2004; Gordon, Piana, & Keleher,
2000; Skiba, Michael, Nardo, & Peterson, 2002). Seattle Public Schools, like other
districts, has a history of racial disparity in school discipline, including suspensions
(Wright, 2005), and in a recent national study, the U.S. Department of Education
found that African American and Latino students are more likely to receive harsher
punishments (http://www.npr.org/blogs/thetwo-way/2012/03/06/148032586/reportminority-students-receive-harsher-punishments). So if students are suspended, they
may end up being on the street during school hours where they could potentially be
viewed with suspicion.
Racial residential segregation means that the neighborhoods from which young
White and Black people come will be different in many important respects. It is well
established that race and social disadvantage of neighborhoods—characterized by
poverty, unemployment, welfare dependency, and so on—are related (de Bodman &
Bennett, 2011; Jargowsky, 1996; Massey, 1990). Black children live in neighborhoods
with more negative social and economic influences (Leventhal & Brooks-Gunn, 2000;
McLoyd, 1990). They are likely to be exposed to criminogenic forces, are more likely
to be victims of crime, and more frequently live where there is greater fear of crime
(Elliott, 1994; Finkelhor & Ormrod, 2001; Peterson & Krivo, 2010; Shihadeh &
Flynn, 1996; Snyder & Sickmund, 2006). Consequently, there will be a greater police
presence in the neighborhood environments of Black juveniles as well as in their
school environments.
The neighborhood or community environment can affect police contacts in two
ways as well. As the criminological literature has long documented, there are
important social forces that lead to higher levels of delinquency among some segments of the population (Bursik & Grasmick, 1993; Sampson, Raudenbush, & Earls,
1997; Shaw & McKay, 1942), but here we are interested in forces that will increase the
probability of police contacts beyond those produced by criminal involvement.
Because police know which neighborhoods have higher crime rates, enforcement is
concentrated there, increasing the odds that juveniles will experience police contact.
A second means by which juveniles may be put at risk of involvement with the
police in their community (Elliott, 1994) is by exposure to ‘‘risk-producing elements,’’
Downloaded from raj.sagepub.com at University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill on August 5, 2014
Crutchfield et al.
187
specific people who officers may watch out for. Families who embrace the ‘‘code of
the street’’ (Anderson, 1999; Jones, 2010; Stewart & Simons, 2010) are not just known
by members of the community but also by law enforcement. Juveniles who spend time
with carriers of the code of the street or who spend time where such people frequent
will have more opportunities to encounter police (‘‘street’’ families are defined earlier
in this article).
Other researchers (mentioned above) have studied social environments and contact
with justice system actors. What this study uniquely contributes is the opportunity to
study the impact of multiple social environments, here measured with considerable
detail, on racial differences in police contact, net of Black and White differences in
self-reported delinquency. The current study complements qualitative research on
school violence and extends quantitative examinations of peers, neighborhoods, and
police contacts by adding simultaneous consideration of families and schools.
Method
Sample
Parents of eighth-grade students during the 2001–2002 school year in the Seattle
school district received a letter describing the study, and the parents were contacted
by phone. Families were included if the teen and one or both parents consented to participate. Eligibility included self-identifying as African American or European American, speaking English as their primary language, and planning to live in the area for at
least 6 months. Recruitment stopped when an adequate number of African American
and European American males and females had agreed to participate. Forty-six
percent of families who received letters consented (55% of African Americans and
40% of European Americans). The parents who refused were more likely to be
European American, married, and had a higher education on average than those who
consented. Other ethnic groups were not recruited.
The sample was stratified by teen race and gender. There were significant differences by race in several demographic variables. European Americans reported higher
per capita income and parental education, and African Americans reported higher
prevalence of single parenthood (Table 1). Some teens in each race group selfidentified as mixed race (19.6% African American, 12.5% European American), but
were included in these analyses. Most primary caregivers were female (> 80%), with
71.6% being the adolescent’s biological mother. Caregiver gender and relationship
were similar across race with one exception: more African American youth had
another female caregiver (e.g., grandmother, aunt) as a primary caregiver than did
European American youth, w2(1) ¼ 13.95, p < .001.
Data collectors went to the families’ homes. Teens and their parents completed
self-administered questionnaires in their homes using laptop computers while the data
collector was present. This ensured that parents did not monitor their teens’ responses.
Prior to parent/teen interaction tasks, a trained research assistant set up video equipment, provided oral and written standardized instructions to each family, then left the
Downloaded from raj.sagepub.com at University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill on August 5, 2014
188
Downloaded from raj.sagepub.com at University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill on August 5, 2014
0/1
0 to 4
0 to 4
0/1
0/1
12 to 15
785 to 83,333
0/1
0 to 5
0/1
1.03 to 3.18
1 to 4.8
1 to 4.33
0/1
1 to 5
0.63 to 3.38
2.50 to 5
1 to 4
1 to 5
Range
0.50
0.47
9,361
0.49
1.36
0.34
0.63
0.72
0.56
0.31
1.01
0.95
0.54
0.59
1.14
0.49
13.68
7,816
0.57
1.33
0.13
0.13
3.02
1.38
0.11
3.33
0.34
3.88
2.97
2.13
SD
0.49
0.68
0.49
M
0.40
0.25
0.12
Significance tests indicate race differences of *p < .05, **p < .001, ***p < .0001.
Police contact
Property crime
Violent crime
Race
Gender
Age
Per capita income
Single parent
Parent juv. del.
Sibling crime
Family conflict
Obs. reward positive behavior
Obs. reward negative/problem behavior
Delinquent peers
Grades
School discipline
Parent neighborhood
Teen neighborhood
Deviant adult network
Measure
African American
0.48
13.67
21,970
0.24
1.12
0.08
0.12
3.37
1.34
0.19
3.99
0.25
3.94
3.13
1.53
0.21
0.11
0.05
M
0.50
0.40
15,958
0.43
0.97
0.27
0.52
0.68
0.46
0.39
1.11
0.69
0.39
0.54
0.83
0.41
0.37
0.25
SD
European American
0.30**
0.18*
0.09
0.49
0.48
13.67
15,025***
0.40***
1.22
0.11
0.00***
3.20***
1.36
0.15*
3.66***
0.04***
3.91
3.05*
1.82***
M
Total
0.46
0.55
0.39
0.50
0.50
0.44
14,913
0.49
1.18
0.31
0.59
0.72
0.51
0.36
1.11
0.88
0.47
0.57
1.04
SD
Table 1. Descriptive Statistics for Police Contacts and Individual, Family, Peer, School, and Neighborhood Predictors Collected in the Eighth Grade
Crutchfield et al.
189
room while the family completed the task. Upon completion of a warm-up task, the
primary caregiver and the teen completed two structured interaction tasks: (a) a 10min problem-solving interaction and (b) a 5-min recognition task during which the
parent and child complimented one another. Family members received $15 each time
they completed questionnaires. The family received $50, and each participant (one
teen and one or two parents) received $15 each for completing observational
measures.
Measures
Police contact was collected on the teen surveys. Contact was coded as having
occurred if the teen responded affirmatively to any of the following questions. Have
you ever (a) been picked up or stopped by the police, but not arrested; (b) been in
trouble with the police for something you did; (c) been arrested by the police; (d) spent
time in a juvenile detention center for something you did wrong? Of the 331 eighth
graders in the sample, 70% had no contact with police. Ninety-nine (30%) had some
contact with police, 76 (23%) had ‘‘only contact’’ but were not arrested, 23 (7%) had
been arrested, and 6 (less than 2%) had been placed in detention. No questions were
asked about where contacts took place or any specific information about the context of
the actual police contacts.
Teen self-report of criminal activity was measured separately for property and
violent crimes. Each was computed as the mean of 2 items measured on a 5-point scale
of frequency in the past 30 days (0 ¼ never, 1 ¼ 1 to 2 times, 2 ¼ 3 to 5 times, 3 ¼ 6 to
10 times, and 4 ¼ more than 10 times). Property crime includes arson and theft. Violent crime includes carrying a gun to school and hitting someone with the intent to hurt
them.
Race was based on parents’ reports of their child’s race on school enrollment forms
(0 ¼ White, 1 ¼ Black). Gender was reported by teens on the survey (0 ¼ male, 1 ¼
female). Age was measured in years calculated from birthdates reported on the survey
and the date the survey was completed. Household per capita income was calculated
from parent’s endorsement of 1 of 11 categories for annual household income (before
taxes). We assigned the midpoint of the range and then divided by the number of
people in the household as reported by parents on the survey. Single-parent status was
reported on the parent survey (0 ¼ partnered, 1 ¼ no spouse or partner).
Parent juvenile delinquency was an index based on parent retrospect accounts of
their own teen years, ‘‘Before you turned 18 did you . . . .’’ Response options were
0 ¼ No and 1 ¼ Yes. The 5 items were (a) skip school, (b) get drunk, (c) run away from
home overnight, (d) use a weapon in a fight, and (e) often start physical fights. Sibling
criminal activity was measured with a single item, ‘‘In the past year, have any of your
brothers or sisters done anything that could have gotten them in trouble with the police
(like stealing, selling drugs, vandalism, etc.)’’ 0 ¼ No, 1 ¼ Yes.
Family conflict was computed as the average of 13 items (a ¼ .84) taken from the
Strauss Conflict Tactics scale (Straus, 1990). Response options for 8 items were a
Likert-type scale from 1 to 7. One item ranged from 1 to 4, and 4 items were
Downloaded from raj.sagepub.com at University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill on August 5, 2014
190
Race and Justice 2(3)
dichotomous. The item scores were standardized (M ¼ 0, SD ¼ 1) and then averaged.
Items included ‘‘Family members often criticize each other’’ and ‘‘In the past month,
how often did you yell, insult, or swear at your teen when the two of you have disagreed about something?’’
Observed rewards for positive or negative behavior were computed as composites
of item responses from trained raters reviewing videotapes of parent–teen interactions.
Variables were measured using the Social Development Model-Observational Coding
System (Spagnolo et al., 2002). Eighteen raters (5 men, 13 women; 28% African
American, 66% European American, 6% Hispanic) completed an average of 93 hours
of training. Ratings were made using 5-point Likert-type scales (Not at all, A little,
Sometimes, Often, Very often). Twenty percent of the videotapes were double rated
to check inter-rater reliability. Inter-rater agreement was high (M ¼ 89%; Lindahl,
2001).
Rewards for positive behavior was computed as the mean of seven items, including
‘‘Caregiver was warm and encouraging of teen’s opinions’’ and ‘‘Caregiver reinforced
or rewarded teen’s prosocial behavior or attitudes.’’
Rewards for negative behavior was computed as the mean of 4 items, including
‘‘Caregiver failed to respond to teen’s negative or antisocial behavior or attitude’’ and
‘‘Caregiver reinforced or rewarded negative behavior or attitudes.’’
Delinquent peers were measured with teen report. The teens were asked to name
their three best (or closest) friends (first names or initials only) and were then asked a
series of questions about each of those friends, including having done anything in the
last year that could have gotten them in trouble with the police. A dichotomous score
was created, with 1 indicating at least one of the friends had engaged in the behavior.
Academic success/grades were based on reports from teens: ‘‘Putting them all
together, what were your grades like last year?’’ Responses ranged from 1 ¼ very poor
to 6 ¼ very good. School discipline was computed as the average of four standardized
items from the teen survey, including ‘‘In the past year, how often have you been sent
out of the classroom for doing something wrong?’’ (a ¼ .88). These items did not ask
about police involvement in school discipline, although it is possible that some
incidences of school discipline may have led to police contact.
Community/neighborhood resources and cohesion was measured using 10 items
from the parent survey. Item responses were from 1 to 5, indicting how accurate the
description or how frequent the activity.2 High scores indicate positive neighborhoods. Items include ‘‘How often do your neighbors visit each other’s homes’’ and
descriptors such as ‘‘nice parks and playgrounds’’ and ‘‘crime’’ (a ¼ .74). Neighborhood environment was assessed using the average of 9 items from the teen survey.
All items were measured on a 4-point scale (YES, yes, no, NO), coded so high scores
indicate safer, less deviant neighborhoods. Items included ‘‘If a kid carried a handgun
in your neighborhood, would he or she get caught by the police?’’ and ‘‘ Do you feel
safe in your neighborhood?’’ (a ¼ .79). Teen report of deviant adult network was computed as the mean of four items (a ¼ .86) measured on a 5-point scale. The items were,
‘‘About how many adults (over 21) do you know personally who have . . . in the past
year?’’ The deviant behaviors were: used marijuana, crack, cocaine, or other drugs;
Downloaded from raj.sagepub.com at University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill on August 5, 2014
Crutchfield et al.
191
Table 2. Odds Ratios for Logistic Regressions Predicting Self-Report Police Contact by Eighth
Grade
Predictor
Step 1 Step 2 Step 3 Step 4 Step 5 Step 6 Step 7
Property crime
Violent crime
Race
Gender
Age
Per capita income
Single parent
Parent juv. del.
Sibling crime
Family conflict
Obs. reward positive behavior
Obs. reward negative/problem
behavior
Delinquent peers
Grades
School discipline
Parent neighborhood
Teen neighborhood
Deviant adult network
Adj.a pseudo R2
2.70* 2.44** 2.03*
2.14* 2.20
2.29
2.26 1.97* 1.93*
0.51* 0.46*
1.49
1.52
1.15
1.04
1.04
1.12
1.34*
4.22**
a
1.66
2.15
1.95*
0.43**
1.38
1.05
1.05
1.32*
4.26**
1.48
0.81
1.45
1.92
2.32*
0.43**
1.36
1.03
0.98
1.32*
4.47**
1.40
0.76
1.37
1.56
1.51
0.60
1.40
1.01
0.80
1.24
3.65**
1.20
0.85
1.10
1.14
1.40
0.50*
1.19
1.13
0.83
1.17
3.75**
1.22
0.82
2.72** 2.62** 2.31** 2.65**
2.58*
.07
.10
.15
.20
.31
2.05
1.50
0.80
0.78
2.18** 1.94**
0.77
0.79
1.65**
.35
.41
2
Adjusted pseudo R are based on models without imputed data.
*p < .05. **p < .01.
sold or dealt drugs; gotten drunk or high; and done other things that could get them in
trouble with the police, like stealing, selling stolen goods, mugging, assaulting others,
and so on.
Analysis
Preliminary analyses included chi square and t tests to examine simple race differences in police contacts, and the 18 predictors from six domains (self-reported crime,
demographics, family, peer, school, and community). Logistic regression analyses
were conducted to determine the unique contribution of predictors to the probability
of reporting a police contact at any time before the eighth-grade survey was conducted. Predictors were entered in blocks to reduce the number of separate models
to be tested and to examine the effects of predictors within a domain simultaneously.
Seven models were tested (see Table 2) in a hierarchical fashion. The order in which
we added predictors was based on our specific questions. First we wanted to know if
there were race differences in the likelihood of police contacts after controlling for
criminal involvement based on self-report. We added demographic variables in the
next step because they were least likely to be influenced by having had a police contact and were therefore most exogenous to the outcome. The rest of the predictors
Downloaded from raj.sagepub.com at University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill on August 5, 2014
192
Race and Justice 2(3)
were added starting with the environments most proximal to the individual and working out to the least proximal. Teens originate and live their lives in families first, then
eventually have some choice about their peers. Schools and communities/neighborhoods are typically not directly chosen by teens, but are to some extent chosen by their
parents. Step 1 included the teen’s self-report of criminal activity (property and violent crimes separately) as well as race predicting police contacts. Step 2 included selfreported criminal activity, race, and demographic variables (gender, age, per capita
income, and single-parent household). In order to determine if variability in other
domains accounts for race differences, predictors were added in successive steps in
this order: parent and sibling criminal activity, family interactions, delinquent peers,
school/grades, and community context.
Although it is not possible to calculate a true R2 for logistic regression models, a
measure of the variance explained by the model can be calculated using the likelihood
ratio index (pseudo R2), comparing the log likelihood of the intercept-only model to
that of the model including predictors and then adjusting for the number of predictors
in the model. To examine whether predictors of police contacts were different for
African American and European American teens, we tested the interactions of each
predictor by race in separate models. The clustering of families within schools was
addressed using SAS GENMOD (Liang & Zeger, 1986; SAS Institute, 2002). Missing
data were imputed (Schafer & Graham, 2002). Missing data ranged from 0% to 4%
across variables, with slightly less than 19% missing across variables. Forty imputations were calculated and appropriate standard errors were computed using MIANALYZE (SAS Institute, 2002). Models reported in Table 2 used multiple imputations.
However, because adjusted pseudo R2 is only an approximation of variance explained
by the model and has not been validated for estimation across multiple imputed data
sets, pseudo R2 was calculated using only those participants with complete data.
Results
Means and standard deviations are presented by race and for the total sample in Table
1. African American teens are almost twice as likely as Whites to report having had a
police contact (40% vs. 21%, w2 ¼ 13.41, p ¼ .0003). Significant race differences
were evident for 10 of the 18 predictors. African Americans reported more property
crime (t ¼ 5.26, p ¼ .02), but not more violent crime than White teens. As reported
above, African American families had lower income and were more likely to have
single parents than White families. No race differences were apparent in parentreported juvenile delinquency. African American teens were no more or less likely
to report sibling criminal behavior than Whites. Family conflict was higher
(t ¼ 16.37, p < .0001), and observed rewards for positive behavior were lower
(t ¼ 19.54, p < .0001) among Blacks than Whites. No race differences were evident
for rewards for negative/problem behavior. Blacks in the sample had fewer delinquent
peers than Whites (11% vs. 19%, w2 ¼ 13.41, p ¼ 3.90, p ¼ .05). African Americans
reported lower grades and more school disciplinary events (t ¼ 32.85, p < .0001; t ¼
41.37, p < .0001) than White teens. Although parent reports of neighborhood
Downloaded from raj.sagepub.com at University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill on August 5, 2014
Crutchfield et al.
193
resources and cohesion were not different, African American teens did report less positive neighborhoods than Whites (t ¼ 6.67, p ¼ .01), and reported more adults in their
network with recent deviant behavior (t ¼ 28.88, p < .001).
Odds ratios (OR) are presented in Table 2. Self-reported property crime and violent
criminal activity did significantly increase the probability of ever having a police contact in Step 1 (OR ¼ 2.70 and 2.14, respectively). After controlling for self-reported
criminal activity and race in Step 2, Black teens were more than twice as likely to have
a police contact as White teens. Girls were half as likely to report a police contact as
boys (OR ¼ .54).
In Step 3, parent juvenile delinquency increased police contacts by about a third
(OR ¼ 1.34), but sibling criminal activity increased the likelihood of police contact by
over 4 times (OR ¼ 4.22). Race differences were significant after controlling for family
criminal activity. In Step 4, higher levels of observed parent rewards for negative
behavior were significantly associated with police contacts (OR ¼ 2.72), while family
conflict and observed parent rewards for positive behavior were not. In this step, selfreported criminal activity was not significantly predictive; however, race differences
were. In Step 5, associating with at least one close friend who broke the law more than
doubled the risk of police contact (OR ¼ 2.58), and this effect was statistically significant before school and broader environmental factors were included.
School factors were added in Step 6. Teen reports of school disciplinary contacts
significantly increased the likelihood of police contacts (OR ¼ 2.18), but grades did
not. In this step, race differences were not significant. In the last model (Step 7),
community contextual measures were added. Teen and parent reports of neighborhood
quality and cohesion were not related to police contacts. However, teen reports of
knowing adults who drank, got high, or committed crimes in the past 12 months were
significant. Knowing adults who exhibit these deviant behaviors significantly
increased the likelihood of police contacts (OR ¼ 1.65).
Pseudo R2 statistics for these models should be interpreted with some caution.
There are no statistical tests for the incremental change or differences between nested
models. Furthermore, they have been calculated without addressing missing data and are
therefore based on subsamples of the original 331. Adjusted pseudo R2 increases from 3%
to 11% with the addition of predictors from each domain. The last model, including 18
predictors, explains just 41% of the variability in the risk of a police contact.
Tests of interactions between race and each predictor separately produced two
statistically significant interactions: neighborhood quality (p ¼ .04) and academic
performance (p ¼ .04). More cohesive, safe, resourced neighborhoods significantly
reduced the probability of police contact for Blacks (p ¼ .04), but not for Whites
(p ¼ .39). Higher grades reduced the probability of police contact for Whites (p ¼ .03),
but not for Blacks (p ¼ .43).
Discussion
We examined several social environments that influence the likelihood of police
contact, although not all of them accounted for racial disparities. Interestingly, low
Downloaded from raj.sagepub.com at University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill on August 5, 2014
194
Race and Justice 2(3)
income and single-parent household were not significant predictors. But we did find
that family patterns contributed strongly to police contacts. Having parents who have a
history of involvement in juvenile crime increased the odds of police contact by about
one third. Parents who were delinquent in their youth are perhaps more tolerant of similar behavior in their children and view this behavior as just normal teenage activity
that their children will grow out of. Youth with a sibling involved in criminal activity
were also more likely to have police contact. We suspect this may be in part due to
police knowing the older siblings and being aware of the household. We did not examine if these families are, in the words of Anderson (1999), ‘‘street families,’’ if the
police had such a conception of them or other views of them, but these results are consistent with what would be expected if police were aware that parents, brothers, and
sisters have been in trouble.
In addition, families were important because of observable behaviors between
parents and teens in which parents encouraged or failed to discourage negative
behavior. This finding points to the importance of parenting practices—or the practice
of everyday parenting—in which parents have the opportunity to influence their
children in both positive and negative ways. In video observations, we observed things
like parents laughing or teasing about the child’s misbehavior or ignoring deviant
behaviors such as fighting. For example, if a parent talked about doing something fun
together when their child was suspended from school it was coded as a reward for
negative behavior. Although this is not the main thrust of this research, we note that
the effect of parent’s own juvenile delinquency is no longer significant when observed
rewards for negative behavior are included. This is consistent with the notion that
parents with a history of delinquency are more tolerant of their children’s problem
behaviors, which in turn increases the likelihood of a police contact. Other family
predictors, conflict, and observed rewards for positive behavior were not predictive of
police contacts. This finding is consistent with families socializing children in a
counternormative direction, exposing their offspring to increased scrutiny from law
enforcement.
Turning to the two environments that are central to our analyses, school and
community, we found that the relationship between grades and police contacts was
significant for White teens, but not for Blacks, suggesting that high academic performance, although important in other ways, does not protect Black teens from early
contacts with law enforcement. Self-reported school discipline (including suspensions) was higher for Blacks than for Whites and accounts for racial differences in
eighth-grade police contacts. During the time of the study, schools in Seattle had programs that linked police officers to middle schools. This practice is consistent with
community policing as described by Garland (2001), which we expected might lead
to greater risks for African American youths. Even though Seattle Public Schools’ policy stated that officers were not to participate in school discipline, one cannot but help
but speculate that these data may be reflecting police involvement in student disciplinary actions. Some officers may have believed that by intervening early they would
decrease the likelihood of future police contacts and arrests. But, analyses by Hirschfield (2009) and Sweeten (2006), which find that arrest and involvement with the
Downloaded from raj.sagepub.com at University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill on August 5, 2014
Crutchfield et al.
195
juvenile justice system increases school dropout should give these officers pause. In
general, there is reason to believe that further criminalizing school infractions will not
only have deleterious effects on future behavior, but also on school performance
(Thompson, 2011). Of course, it is also possible that youth with a police contact are
the ones who are more likely to act out in school, which is not illegal, but can lead to
more frequent and serious responses from school authorities. These cross-sectional
data do not allow for a clear causal interpretation.
Finally, teen reports of neighborhood quality were not predictive of police contacts;
however, parent report of resources and cohesiveness were, but only for Black teens.
Parent’s views of the neighborhood may be more objective and reliable. Cohesion and
resources are related to crime rates, and poorer neighborhoods have a higher police
presence, increasing the chances of contacts for Black teens. The same may not hold
for Whites. Black teens may also live in a wider range of neighborhoods, with the
poorest Blacks in poorer neighborhoods than poor Whites. If this is true, the relationship between neighborhood quality and police contacts may be stronger when
comparing poor to moderate neighborhoods than when comparing moderate to highquality ones.
Juveniles’ rep...
Purchase answer to see full
attachment