School Psychology Quarterly
2008, Vol. 23, No. 3, 389 – 406
Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association
1045-3830/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/1045-3830.23.3.389
Using Behavioral Interventions to Assist Children With Type 1
Diabetes Manage Blood Glucose Levels
Kim Lasecki
Daniel Olympia, Elaine Clark,
William Jenson, and
Lora Tuesday Heathfield
Bellin Psychiatric Center
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
University of Utah
Treatment and management of chronic disease processes on children occurs across
multiple settings, placing demands for consultation and expertise on school personnel,
including school psychologists. One such chronic condition in children is type I
diabetes. Children with type I insulin dependent diabetes mellitus exhibit high rates of
noncompliance to treatment, which can lead to a variety of medical problems. This
study examined the effectiveness of a specific behavioral intervention using behavioral
consultation (BC) and conjoint behavioral consultation (CBC) to reduce uncontrolled
blood glucose levels in medically at-risk children. An intermittent reward procedure
was utilized to reinforce individualized target behaviors associated with treatment
noncompliance. Specific target behaviors were individually established for six patients
ages 8-12 through behavioral consultation interviews. Each child was randomly assigned to a reward ⫹ BC or reward ⫹ CBC condition. Results of the study showed that
all participants improved; with slightly greater gains shown in the CBC condition.
Follow-up data for 3 of the 4 participants completing the study showed improved
compliance and mental health status. Treatment acceptability date indicate the intervention was viewed positively by parents and school based nurses.
Keywords: interventions, behavioral, Type 1 diabetes, children
The contributions of school psychologists to
the treatment and management of students
whose medical conditions are frequently associated with educational and social difficulties
has grown significantly in the last several years
(Brown & DuPaul,1997). Indeed, schools are
more often asked to address a wide range of
behavioral issues or problem behaviors that
About the authors: Kim Lasecki received his PhD in
educational psychology at the University of Utah in 2000.
Currently, he works as a licensed psychologist for Bellin
Psychiatric Center where he previously served as Director
of Outpatient Services. Dr. Lasecki has interests in outpatient psychological assessment, therapy, and behavioral interventions related to child and adolescent issues.
Daniel Olympia is an Associate Professor in the Department of Educational Psychology at the University of Utah.
He received his doctorate in 1992 from the University of
Utah. Dr. Olympia’s research interests include academic
and behavioral interventions, consultation strategies, practice, and ethical issues in applied settings and autism.
Elaine Clark is Professor of School Psychology, and
Chair of the Department of Educational Psychology at the
University of Utah. Dr. Clark’s research interests include
assessment and interventions with conditions that impact a
child’s cognitive functioning, including neurodevelopmental and acquired conditions, academic performance, and
psychosocial status.
William Jenson is a Professor in the Department of Ed-
ucational Psychology at the University of Utah. He received
his doctorate from Utah State University in 1976, with a
specialization in child clinical psychology. Dr. Jenson’s
research interests include behavioral interventions for tough
kids, parent training, generalization of treatment effects, and
autism. Dr. Jenson is the author of The Tough Kid Book.
Lora Tuesday Heathfield, PhD, is an Associate Professor
in the Department of Educational Psychology at the University of Utah in Salt Lake City, UT. Her research interests
focus on improving service delivery to children with or at
risk for learning and/or behavioral difficulties and their
families in the areas of prevention and intervention strategies, as well as assessment issues. Dr. Tuesday Heathfield
received her PhD in School Psychology from the University
of Oregon and completed post-doctoral training in Pediatric
Psychology.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Daniel Olympia, University of Utah, Department
of Educational Psychology, 327 MBH, SLC, UT 84112.
E-mail: dan.olympia@utah.edu
389
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
390
LASECKI, OLYMPIA, CLARK, JENSON, AND TUESDAY HEATHFIELD
may be associated with specific health problems. Treatment or management of chronic disease processes in children also occurs across
multiple settings, placing additional demands
for consultation and expertise on school personnel (Nastasi, 2000). This is especially true of
Type I diabetes, an insulin-dependent autoimmune disease that has serious implications
for a child’s mental and physical health and for
their ability to be successful in school settings
(Daley, Woderich, & Hasan, 2004). Type I diabetes, hereafter referred to as TI diabetes, destroys insulin producing beta cells in the pancreas and makes it difficult for the body to use
glucose and break down foods into simple sugars. Without insulin, cells are starved for fuel
and dangerous levels of unabsorbed glucose
build up in the body. As a result, individuals
with TI diabetes have to inject insulin and effectively manage dietary intake (Foster, 1991).
TI diabetes affects 1 in 500 children under the
age of 18 (Sperling, 1990), making it the second
most common chronic disease in school-age
children, the first being asthma (Strawhacker,
2001). Characteristics of TI diabetes at the time
of initial diagnosis include: high blood-sugar
levels, excessive thirst and urination, weight
loss, and vision problems. When blood-sugar
(glucose) levels cannot be stabilized, other
problems often occur. Although some problems
are short-term (e.g., slowed information processing, poor concentration and memory, dizziness, moodiness, and fatigue), others are more
long term (Chase, 1985) and can include a variety of mental health and psychosocial related
disorders (Kovacs, Goldston, Obrosky, & Bonar, 1997; Kovacs, Obrosky, Goldston, &
Drash, 1997; Smith & Baum, 1980).
Adherence to Typical Treatment Regimens
Treatment for TI diabetes consists of a combination of regular blood-glucose testing, insulin injections (when needed), daily exercise, and
dietary management (Karam, 1996). Children
have particular difficulty with this regimen for
several reasons. Blood sugar levels need to be
checked 3 to 5 times a day (before meals and
bedtime snacks). In addition, insulin injections
are often needed before breakfast and dinner.
The targeted goal for most individuals is a blood
sugar level between 70 and 150 mg/dl at least
50% of the time (Betteridge, 2000). This means
some children will need to check their bloodsugar levels more often during the day, and
inject insulin more frequently to achieve recommended levels. More often than not, blood testing and insulin injections are done at home;
however, in some cases insulin has to be administered at school (e.g., before lunch). In addition to insulin injection, treatment also includes exercise and a proper diet. Children need
to manage their food intake by eating regular
meals and having appropriate snacks (e.g., midmorning and midafternoon). In many cases this
requires monitoring by educational personnel.
Lastly, treatment must include exercise, another
activity that school staff may regularly control.
It is difficult to estimate the extent of children’s adherence to the treatment regimen;
however, Kovacs and colleagues report that
children are at greater risk for noncompliance
than other age groups (Kovacs, Goldston, Obrosky, & Iyengar, 1992; Kovacs, Mukerji, Iyengar, & Drash, 1996). There are a number of
possible reasons for noncompliance, including
poor communication between health providers
and families, a lack of understanding of the
complications associated with TI diabetes, and a
lack of parental involvement and skill in managing the treatment (Delamater, 2000). Given
these issues, it is critical that interventions involve multiple individuals (e.g., families and
peers) and address multiple contexts (e.g.,
home, school, and community).
Improving Compliance Using Behavioral
Interventions and Consultation
Most school psychologists are familiar with
the use of behaviorally based interventions to
address academic and behavioral issues. The
increased prevalence of children with medically
complex conditions or children who experience
some aspect of a medical treatment in school
has created both opportunities and expectations
for school psychologists. For example, under
both the Individuals with Disabilities Improvement Act (IDEIA) and Section 504 of the Rehabilitation Act and the Americans with Disabilities Act, chronic health issues must be addressed if they affect a child’s ability to
participate in school activities or learning.
Empirical support for interventions that rely
on behaviorally based principles to effectively
manage pediatric diabetes were recently sum-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
BEHAVIORAL MANAGEMENT OF TYPE 1 DIABETES
marized by Wysocki (2006). These include
treatments targeting treatment compliance, social coping skills, and family based management techniques. However, there is no research
that addresses behavioral management of the
disorder across multiple settings (i.e., home,
clinic, and school) or uses a consultation centered framework to develop individualized
treatment protocols. Additionally, traditional
school consultation research has often focused
on interactions with only teachers and parents,
not other related personnel (i.e., school nurses).
Innovative behavior management strategies
supported by behavioral consultation can be a
means to ensure that interventions aimed at
increasing treatment compliance involve critical
parties in all settings (Sheridan, Welch, &
Orme, 1996).
Mystery motivator. The mystery motivator
(Jenson, Rhode, & Reavis, 1995) uses the principles of positive reinforcement, performance
feedback, and randomized contingencies to initiate and, more importantly, to maintain compliance and anticipatory motivation. A central
element of the intervention consists of an envelope, labeled with a question mark and containing a card identifying a high value, high interest
reward. The child gains access to several reward
choices (including the mystery motivator)
through the achievement of a daily individualized goal. Several studies have determined that
the mystery motivator can effectively improve
academic and behavioral goals (DeMartiniScully, Bray, & Kehle, 2000; Leblanc, 1999;
Moore et al., 1994; Robinson & Sheridan, 2000;
Valum, 1996). However, no published studies
have applied this intervention to facilitate the
management of a medical treatment across settings.
Behavioral consultation. Behavioral consultation (BC) involves an indirect service delivery approach, typically with a parent or
teacher as the consultee. Conceived by Bergan
and Kratochwill (1990), BC involves indirect
service delivery to a student through a consultee, such as a parent or teacher, by a consultant (e.g., psychologist). Therefore, BC involves
three participants: consultant, consultee, and
client. The consultant is generally a psychologist, counselor, or mental health worker. The
consultee is typically a teacher or parent, but is
not limited to these individuals. Consultees
could also be nursing staff in a hospital or
391
school setting. Clients are often school-age children identified to be struggling with significant
behavioral, emotional, social, or academic difficulties. BC’s approach to helping the consultee and client is based upon the identification
of specific antecedents or consequent events
that affect a target behavior. The BC model has
its foundation in applied behavioral analysis; as
such, data are collected to determine if the intervention is effective. Typically, the model involves four distinct stages (problem identification, problem analysis, plan implementation,
and problem evaluation). Large scale metaanalytic studies of behavioral consultation have
reported effect sizes as high as .95 (Bergan &
Kratochwill, 1990; Kratochwill, Elliott, &
Busse, 1995; Medway & Updyke, 1985; Sibley,
1986).
Conjoint behavioral consultation (CBC).
The CBC model is a variant of BC. Although it
uses an indirect form of service-delivery (with
distinct stages for problem identification, analysis, etc.), it makes use of a broader range of
consultees. For example, parents, teachers, and
other support staff from schools work with a
consultant (often a school psychologist) to address problem behaviors. These individuals
form an alliance to provide assistance in the
environments where children are having difficulty. Working with a consultant, multiple consultees share responsibility for developing and
implementing the intervention, and evaluating
its effectiveness (Sheridan & Kratochwill,
1992; Sheridan, Kratochwill, & Bergan, 1996).
These partnerships are considered to be critical
in that each consultee is viewed as bearing
partial responsibility for meeting the needs of a
student because of different perspectives and
connections with different environments (e.g.,
home and school).
Given an emphasis on a broad-based multisource, multisetting, multimethod assessment
approach (Sheridan, Kratochwill, & Bergan,
1996), the CBC model seems to be particularly
well suited to address the problem of treatment
compliance in TI diabetes. Parents and medical
professionals, together with school personnel
(e.g., teachers and school psychologists), can
help identify specific issues associated with
treatment noncompliance problems in various
settings, including the school, home, and community. In other models, including the BC
model and mental health consultation, the atten-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
392
LASECKI, OLYMPIA, CLARK, JENSON, AND TUESDAY HEATHFIELD
tion of the consultant is often focused on an
identified client with a problem in a particular
milieu (and with a single consultee associated
with that milieu). Critical information, and individuals, may be omitted in treatment planning
and implementation, thereby limiting the effectiveness of the treatment plan. The CBC approach addresses this problem by increasing
sources for information and the number of potential interventionists (e.g., parents, teachers,
and other health professionals) needed to ensure
that an effective treatment is designed, implemented, and maintained (Sheridan et al., 1996).
The CBC model has also proven to be effective
in improving a wide array of academic, behavioral, and social problems. Sheridan and her
colleagues have found effect sizes to range from
⫺0.62 to 6.31 (e.g., Galloway & Sheridan,
1994; Sheridan, Colton, Fenstermacher,
Lasecki, & Wilson, 1996; Sheridan, Eagle,
Cowan, & Mickelson, 2001; Weiner, Sheridan,
& Jenson, 1998).
This study examined the usefulness of a specific behavioral intervention (mystery motivator) under two variations of behavioral consultation to improve treatment compliance for TI
diabetes. To date there has been no systematic
research that examines the effectiveness of
these methods, singly or in combination, to help
children with TI diabetes comply with the treatment regimen needed to stabilize their bloodglucose level. A single reinforcement strategy
(mystery motivator) based on the principles of
reinforcement unpredictability (when positive
reinforcement becomes available) and reinforcement value (what is earned) was employed
across two consultation conditions. Two variations of behavioral consultation, traditional Behavioral Consultation (BC) and Conjoint Behavioral Consultation (CBC) were implemented
across four participants with well established
difficulty in maintaining appropriate blood glucose levels.
Method
Participants
Children who participated in the study were
selected from a patient roster in a hospital affiliated with an outpatient clinic for children
with diabetes in a large, urban Midwestern city.
Each participant met the following criteria:
chronological age between 8 years-0 months
and 12 years-0 months at the time of the study,
a diagnosis of TI diabetes made 4 to 6 years
earlier, no other chronic illnesses, and most
importantly, evidence of an uncontrolled bloodglucose level (i.e., recurrent hyperglycemia using hemoglobin A1c or glycolated hemoglobin
ratings above 250 mg/dl). In cases where there
was no recent hemoglobin A1c, participants
were included if their blood-glucose levels averaged above 250 mg/dl (documented by daily
glucose testing). Six children were randomly
selected from a list of children who met these
criteria.
Half of the children were randomly assigned
to the mystery motivator ⫹ BC condition and
the other half assigned to the mystery motivator ⫹ CBC condition. Two children dropped out
of the study during the Baseline phase, due in
the first case to a psychiatric hospitalization
(and later placement in a residential treatment
program) and, in the second case, to parental
inability to participate in the study due to a job
schedule change. Two participants remained in
the BC condition and 2 participants in the CBC
condition. An abbreviated summary of participant demographic information is provided in
Table 1.
Participant 1 (CBC). Participant 1 was a 9
year-old, third grade male who had been TI
diagnosed with diabetes for 4 years, 1 month at
the time of the study. Since diagnosed, the child
had not been consistently compliant with treatment, and therefore, was having frequent episodes of hyperglycemia. No specific intervention had been used to improve the child’s compliance with the treatment regimen for his
diabetes. The child was living with his biological mother and younger sibling at the time of
the study (his mother had divorced his stepfather 2 years earlier). Participant 1 had also
been diagnosed with an Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), combined type,
and enrolled in a special education program for
students with Emotional Disturbance (SED).
The child’s mother described him as having
significant behavior problems, including oppositional behaviors that were difficult to control.
The school nurse further reported that Participant 1 was having academic problems as well as
difficulty with lying, being manipulative, failing
to follow rules, refusing (at times) to eat, and
not bringing his diabetic monitor to school on a
BEHAVIORAL MANAGEMENT OF TYPE 1 DIABETES
393
Table 1
Age, Gender, Grade Level, Family Status, Length of Diagnosis and Treatment Target Behavior for Each
Participant
Age/Gender
Grade
Family Status
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Length of Diagnosis
Target Behavior
Participant 1
Participant 2
Participant 3
Participant 4
9/Male
3rd
Low income/single
parent
4 years-1 month
Percent of insulin
injections
monitored by an
adult
11/Male
6th
Low income/single
parent
5 years-5 months
Incidents per day
of snacking
without parent
permission
11/Female
5th
Middle income/
two parent
4 years-4 months
Percent compliance
with monitoring
blood glucose
levels and taking
insulin
10/Male
4th
Middle income/
two parent
4 years-4 months
Percent compliance
with monitoring
blood glucose
levels and taking
insulin
consistent basis. No formal, or structured, interventions were being used at school to assist with
these behaviors (except the general special education program).
Participant 2 (BC). Participant 2 was an 11
year-old, sixth grade male who had been diagnosed with TI diabetes for 5 years 5 months.
Prior to the study, the child had been eating
inappropriate snack food and had an elevated
blood-glucose level. Variable supervision at
home had reportedly contributed to Participant 2 having access to inappropriate foods
(e.g., unsupervised snacking after school). Although the child (an only child) and his biological mother had received counseling for behavior problems before the study, his mother reported using mostly negative feedback to
manage negative behaviors. Participant 2 had
been diagnosed with ADHD and was prescribed
Adderall. In addition, he was classified as “other
health impaired” (OHI) and was receiving special education services in the schools. Primary
concerns identified for Participant 2 included:
poor academic performance, a poor relationship
with his mother, and noncompliance with house
rules and management of his diabetes.
Participant 3 (BC). Participant 3 was an 11
year-old, fifth grade girl. She was diagnosed 4
years 4 months earlier with diabetes. Prior to the
study, the child (the oldest of 3 children) had
been noncompliant with treatment. Her noncompliance was associated with her parents’
work schedules, a lack of supervision in terms
of her snacking (reported to be excessive), and
poor monitoring of her blood sugar level. Participant 3’s mother reported that she and her
husband differed in their parenting styles, that
is, she was lenient and her husband was strict
and at times negative. Although the child’s parents reported using time-out to help manage
inappropriate behaviors at home, the use was
inconsistent. Academically, Participant 3 was
described by her teachers as being “average”
and reported to be outgoing and having an engaging personality. She was also reported to be
motivated by rewards.
Participant 4 (CBC). Participant 4 was a 10
year-old, fourth grade male. He had been diagnosed with TI diabetes 4 years 4 months before
the study. Since the initial diagnosis, the child
was frequently noncompliant with monitoring
his diabetes, leading to numerous episodes of
hyperglycemia. Prior to the study, Participant
4’s mother had met with a nurse and dietitian at
a local diabetes clinic. The child, the middle of
three, was living with his siblings and biological
parents at the time of the study. Participant 4’s
relationship with his parents was observed to be
positive. He did not have any psychiatric diagnosis and had not received any special education service. The school nurse who served as a
consultee, however, reported some concern
about children teasing him and about his “lying”
about his blood-sugar levels.
Consultant and Consultees
The consultant for this study was a male
doctoral graduate student in the School Psychology Program at the University of Utah. He
received extensive supervised training to mastery through specific coursework and supervised practice through a two year placement in
a federally funded training grant in evidence
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
394
LASECKI, OLYMPIA, CLARK, JENSON, AND TUESDAY HEATHFIELD
based intervention strategies and BC and CBC
models.
To conform to the definition of each consultation strategy, consultees in the study included
a “parent only” in the BC condition, and both a
parent and school representative in the CBC
condition. In both conditions, however, the
child’s biological mother served as the parent
consultee. The average age of the “parent consultees” in the study was 35 years of age
(range ⫽ 26 to 41). None of the consultees
reported familiarity with CBC or BC (and no
one had any prior experience with these approaches). School nurses served as consultees
during the CBC condition (i.e., for Participants 1 and 4). The nurse working with Participant 1 was 44 years-old and had 17 years of
experience in the school district. The nurse
working with Participant 4 was 32 years-old and
had 6 years of experience. Neither nurses participating in the study was familiar (or had
experience) with CBC or BC.
Setting
Consultation cases were primarily conducted
at a hospital-based diabetes clinic and/or in the
child participant’s school. For Participant 2, it
was necessary on one occasion to meet for
consultation at the mother’s home due to transportation difficulties. During the treatment
phase for both conditions, the specific behavioral intervention (mystery motivator) was provided only within the home environment for all
participants in the study.
Design
A single subject multiple-baseline design
across participants was used to evaluate the
effectiveness of the mystery motivator intervention under two consultation methods, (i.e., mystery motivator ⫹ Behavioral Consultation and
mystery motivator ⫹ Conjoint Behavioral Consultation). The behaviorally based reinforcement technique (mystery motivator) was used to
reinforce the reduction of hyperglycemia in the
participants. The baseline was initiated for all
participants simultaneously. Given that 2 participants failed to complete the study, baseline
data for Participants 1 and 4 (in the CBC condition) were not paired, therefore, treatment
ended up beginning at the same time. Partici-
pants 2 and 3 (in the BC condition) did not
begin treatment at the same time. A brief follow
up period for each participant occurred 1 month
after treatment to obtain data for target behaviors and blood glucose levels.
Intervention Procedures
In this study, the mystery motivator was used
in combination with a “spinner” (a small cardboard circle divided into unequal, numbered
segments with a small arrow or pointer, which
is placed at the center and allowed to rotate).
For each participant, the following steps were
used to implement the mystery motivator program. Rewards to be used with each participant
were selected during both the problem identification and problem analysis phases of BC and
CBC. Consultees and child participants were
given opportunities to list rewards they believed
would be most effective and reasonable. After
rewards were selected, a menu listing the rewards was completed. The menu identified
numbers on the spinner corresponding to specific rewards. The menu also identified a space
on the spinner that was designated as the “mystery” section. Parent consultees were asked to
determine a “mystery” reward for each participant in the study prior to the intervention. Once
the mystery reinforcer was determined, consultees were instructed to write the reward on a
piece of paper, fold the paper, and place it in an
envelope to maintain the secrecy of the reward
from the participants. Table 2 displays the rewards including the mystery motivator selected
and used during the intervention phase for each
participant.
Both the CBC and BC models involved the
four typical stages: problem identification,
problem analysis, treatment implementation,
and treatment evaluation. Within CBC model,
the consultant and the two consultees participated in three interviews: Conjoint Problem
Identification Interview (CPII), Conjoint Problem Analysis Interview (CPAI), and Conjoint
Treatment Evaluation Interview (CTEI). In the
BC condition, only the consultant and parent
consultee participated in three interviews: Problem Identification Interview (PII), Problem
Analysis Interview (PAI), and Treatment Evaluation Interview (TEI). Each interview was
completed in a single session and followed a
similar standard protocol (Sheridan et al.,
BEHAVIORAL MANAGEMENT OF TYPE 1 DIABETES
395
Table 2
Reinforcement Used by Participants during Mystery Motivator
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Reinforcer
Menu Space
Participant 1
Participant 2
1
Pick t.v. show for the
evening
2
Spend night at
grandparents house
Use cell phone to
make 10 minute
call.
Have friend stay over
night w/ permission
3
Stay up 1⁄2 hour later
4-Mystery
Trip to local
amusement park
Pack of Pokemon
cards
5
Staying up 1⁄2 hour
later
Baseball Batting Glove
Picking appropriate
dessert for dinner
1996). Each interview was scheduled by the
consultant. For CBC cases, coordination of the
nurse and parent schedules was necessary to
allow both to be involved in each phase of the
consultation process. All consultation sessions
were conducted using a standardized set of
questions adapted from Sheridan et al. (1996)
for CBC and from Kratochwill and Bergan
(1990) for BC.
Children in the study participated in the problem analysis and treatment evaluation interviews (for both CBC and BC conditions). However, children in this study were excluded from
the problem identification phase (in either CBC
or BC condition) to prevent potential reactivity
during collection of baseline data.
Target behaviors. Parent consultees for all
children in the study were given behavioral
charts to write down the target behavior identified during the problem identification interview
that was most critical to facilitate adherence to
the proscribed treatment. One target behavior
was identified for each child in the study. Parent
consultees were instructed to keep the chart and
menu in a visible location in the home such as
posting it on the refrigerator. For CBC, school
consultees did not take an active role in the
mystery motivator intervention other than to
supply data with the use of a homenote adapted
from the Tough Kid Tool Box (Jenson, Rhode,
& Reavis, 1995) to parents regarding the target
behavior observed within the school environment. For Participant 1, the school nurse reported daily to the parents whether or not insulin injections were necessary and if so whether
they were observed. For Participant 4, the
Participant 3
Participant 4
Special shopping trip
to mall with mom
No chores for
week
Staying up 1 hour later
at night
Staying up 1
hour later for
the night
Friend sleep over
Getting to control what
is on tv. for night
$25 Gift Certificate for
clothes
Permission to use
phone for 1⁄2 hour.
Soccer Ball
1 hour of
computer time
school nurse was active in giving daily feedback
to parents about whether or not blood glucose
levels were checked at school and if insulin was
taken if necessary.
On the first weekly chart, stars were randomly placed in boxes corresponding to the
days of the week. Stickers were also used to
cover each box to conceal where stars were
located. Hidden stars were placed in 4 of the 7
possible locations corresponding to the days of
the week. If the child participant attained the
daily goal, they were then allowed to peel off
the sticker to determine if they had earned a spin
of the spinner. If the child uncovered a star, they
were allowed to spin one time for a reward. If
the child participant landed on the mystery section of the spinner, they were immediately allowed to open the mystery motivator envelope
with the enclosed reward.
All parent consultees were instructed to begin
fading the program after the child had successfully completed their goal of performing the
target behavior across an entire week without
error. The fading method employed by parent
consultees involved reducing the number of
hidden stars by one per week after each successful week of target behavior performance.
All parent consultees were contacted by telephone to collect follow-up data one month after
consultation was completed. Data for the most
recent four days of blood-glucose levels and
target behavior were collected.
Data collection. Baseline data collection
procedures were discussed with consultees during the problem identification phase. After
agreement and definition of a target behavior for
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
396
LASECKI, OLYMPIA, CLARK, JENSON, AND TUESDAY HEATHFIELD
each case, consultees were asked to unobtrusively collect baseline data. For CBC cases,
baseline data were collected in both the home
and school environment. For BC cases, baseline
data were collected in the home environment
only. In all cases, the parent consultees were
responsible for managing the collection of baseline data related to blood-glucose levels. Each
parent was then responsible for bringing their
children to the diabetes clinic to have bloodglucose data downloaded from the child’s
blood-glucose monitor by the first author.
During problem identification interviews
within CBC and BC formats, all consultees consistently expressed concern about noncompliance toward treatment of diabetes. Within each
case, however, the specific target behavior differed due to factors specific to each individual
child’s case. The problem identification phase
was designed to evaluate baseline data and environmental factors contributing to diabetes
treatment noncompliance. For each case, antecedent conditions and setting events, consequent
conditions, and environmental/sequential conditions associated with target behaviors were analyzed during the problem identification interview.
Table 1 summarizes the target behaviors that were
monitored for each child as well as demographic
variables such as marital status of parents, income
level, age of child, and length of diagnosis. The
target behaviors listed in Table 1 were reviewed
by the consultant and consultees and determined
to be the most significant factor affecting each
participant child at the time of the study’s problem
identification interviews.
To increase the likelihood of consultees understanding and properly implementing the procedures of this study, each consultee was contacted by telephone or in person weekly during
the intervention phase. Consultees were given
opportunities to discuss questions about data
collection or procedures during those contacts.
Additionally, parent consultees completed a
treatment plan worksheet that highlighted the
key components in carrying out the intervention
procedures.
Dependent Measures
Blood glucose levels taken daily by each
child participant under the supervision of consultees, were collected as baseline data and subsequently during intervention to establish glu-
cose levels prior to, during, and after treatment.
One-month from the point where consultation
was terminated, follow-up measurements of
blood glucose levels were also taken. Each participant’s target behaviors were also recorded
across baseline, treatment, and follow-up conditions and plotted in relationship to changes in
blood glucose levels.
Additional data was collected to assess the
social validity of treatment outcome, treatment
acceptability, and treatment integrity. Goal Attainment Scaling (GAS) was utilized to provide an
index of social validation (Kiresuk, Smith, & Cardillo, 1994). The GAS ratings range from ⫹ 2 to
⫺2 (⫹2 ⫽ behavior goal fully met, ⫹1 ⫽ goal
partially met, 0 ⫽ no progress, ⫺1 ⫽ behavior
somewhat worse, ⫺2 ⫽ behavior significantly
worse). All consultees including parents and
school nurses (during CBC) completed the GAS
to assess the level of attainment of stated treatment
goals for each consultation case.
The acceptability of the behavioral intervention and consultation services used across cases
was assessed via the Behavioral Intervention
Rating Scale – Revised (BIRS-R; Elliott & Von
Brock Treuting, 1991). The BIRS-R is comprised of 24 items scored on a 6 point Likert
scale (1 ⫽ not at all acceptable; 6 ⫽ highly
acceptable). Factor analysis of the BIRS-R has
yielded three factors: Acceptability (15 items);
Effectiveness (7 items); and Time to Effectiveness (2 items) (Elliott & Von Brock Treuting,
1991). Parent and school consultees completed
the BIRS-R-R after the successful completion
of the treatment evaluation phase of both types
of consultation. Likewise, child participants
completed the Children’s Intervention Rating
Profile (CIRP) (Witt & Elliott, 1985) to assess
acceptability of the intervention.
Treatment integrity for this study was assessed in two ways. For each session of consultation, the consultant quantitatively evaluated
treatment integrity using behavioral checklists
adopted from Sheridan and Colton (1994).
Checklists were used as a self-monitoring technique to determine the percentage of compliance in following the intended structure for each
type of consultation (Sheridan et al., 1996;
Kratochwill & Bergan, 1990) for CBC and BC.
The consultant completed the appropriate selfmonitoring checklist immediately after the consultation session had ended and percentage of
compliance was calculated. Treatment plan
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
BEHAVIORAL MANAGEMENT OF TYPE 1 DIABETES
worksheets completed by the parent consultees
were also used as a self-monitoring checklist,
and consisted of a review/verification of each
step of the mystery motivator intervention.
Instrumentation. The One Touch FastTake
compact blood glucose monitoring system was
used by all child participants. The One Touch
FastTake system was developed by Lifescan
(retrieved from http://www.lifescan.com/
company/, May 15, 2006) and used across all
participants to minimize variability in measurement of glucose levels due to instrumentation.
All participants were trained how to properly
use the One Touch FastTake compact blood
glucose monitoring system. FasTtake Test
Strips were used by participants to measure
blood-glucose levels. Test strips from each participant were placed in the meter after a small
amount of blood was applied to the white target
area of the FastTake Test Strip. Blood glucose
test results appear after the meter counts down
from 45 to 1 second. The FastTake Meter displays results between 20 and 600 mg/dL. The
FastTake Meter also stores up to 250 blood
glucose test results with date and time for later
recording and review by the first author.
Data Analysis Procedures
Behavioral data were evaluated using effect
sizes and visual analysis to detect level change.
Immediacy of change and level stability also
were calculated. Percentage of nonoverlapping
data points (PND) was not used, as baseline
data were too variable. Effect size (ES) was
calculated using the Busk and Serlin (1992) “no
assumptions” method. Baseline and treatment
levels were determined by summing average
daily blood glucose levels (mg/dL) and dividing
by the number of data points in each phase.
Comparisons of mean baseline and treatment
levels were made visually to determine treatment effects. Immediacy of change was used to
calculate the strength of intervention by determining the difference between the first treatment data point and last baseline data point for
each participant (Tawney & Gast, 1984). As the
difference between these two data points becomes
greater, the immediacy of change (and the effectiveness of the intervention) can be ascertained.
Level stability also was determined for baseline and treatment phases. Data were considered
stable in each phase if at least 80% of data
397
points fell within a 15% margin surrounding the
mean level for each baseline and treatment condition. Throughout the study, specific insulin
doses or medication as prescribed each participant’s physician and the treatment team at the
diabetes clinic were not changed. The most significant side effect of intensive treatment anticipated was an increase in the risk for low blood
sugar episodes severe enough to require assistance
from another person. To guard against any increased risk of hypoglycemia, strict control over
blood sugar levels was not exercised through increased use of insulin injections beyond the protocol normally in place at the diabetes clinic.
Results
Blood Glucose Levels
Figure 1 displays baseline, treatment, and follow-up data for each participant’s daily blood
glucose levels. Each participant in this study was
required through their normal regimen to check
blood glucose levels four times per day. Data
points represent daily averages for blood glucose
levels calculated for each participant during baseline, treatment, and follow-up phases.
Participant 1. For Participant 1, baseline
blood-glucose levels were calculated to be an average of 273.0 mg/dL (range ⫽ 95– 460), with a
treatment average of 141.3 mg/dL (range 35–
425). An effect size for change in average bloodglucose levels for Participant 1 was calculated to
be 1.23. At follow-up, Participant 1 was calculated
to have an average daily blood glucose level
across four days of 161.3 mg/dL.
Participant 2. For Participant 2, baseline
blood-glucose levels were calculated to be an average of 302.0 mg/dL (range ⫽ 70 –599), with a
treatment average of 185.8 mg/dL (range ⫽ 44 –
499). An effect size for Participant 2 was calculated to be 0.89. At follow-up, Participant 2 was
found to have an average daily blood-glucose
level across four days of 214.0 mg/dL.
Participant 3. Baseline blood-glucose levels were calculated to be an average of 255.1
mg/dL (range ⫽ 74 –598). A treatment average
was found to be 159.8 mg/dL (range ⫽ 69 –
245), generating an effect size of 1.48. However, at follow-up, Participant 3 was found to
have an average daily blood-glucose level
across four days of 267.69 mg/dL, representing
a return to baseline levels.
398
LASECKI, OLYMPIA, CLARK, JENSON, AND TUESDAY HEATHFIELD
Treatment
Follow-up
400
Subject 1
300
CBC
200
100
23
21
19
17
15
13
11
9
7
5
3
1
0
500
Subject 2
400
Behavioral
300
Consultation
200
100
23
21
19
17
15
13
11
9
7
5
3
1
0
500
Subject 3
400
Behavioral
300
Consultation
200
100
23
21
19
17
15
13
11
9
7
5
3
1
0
400
Subject 4
300
CBC
200
100
23
21
19
17
15
13
11
9
7
5
3
0
1
Blood Glucose Level (mg/dL)
Blood Glucose Level (mg/dL)
Blood Glucose Level (mg/dL)
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Blood Glucose Level (mg/dL)
Baseline
Day of Study
Figure 1.
Changes in blood glucose level by subject across experimental phases.
Participant 4. For Participant 4, baseline
blood-glucose levels were calculated to be an
average of 270.6 mg/dL (range ⫽ 79 – 644),
with a treatment average of 154.7 mg/dL (range
57–259). An effect size for change in average
blood-glucose levels for Participant 4 was calculated to be 2.27. At follow-up, Participant 4
was calculated to have an average daily bloodglucose level across four days of 218.5 mg/dL.
Target Behavior Response
Figure 2 Displays Behavioral Data for Each
Participant Across Baseline, Treatment, and follow-up Phases.
Participant 1. For Participant 1, the target
behavior identified during the PII of CBC was
to have the Participant take insulin while observed by his mother, the school nurse, or a
BEHAVIORAL MANAGEMENT OF TYPE 1 DIABETES
while allowing his mother, school nurse, or
grandparent to observe the event occurred an
average of 33.33% (0 –50%, std ⫽ 25.82).
Treatment data was calculated to be an average
of 94.44% (range ⫽ 50 –100%; std ⫽ 16.17)
with a corresponding effect size of 2.37. At
follow-up, Participant 1 was calculated to have
Treatment
Follow-up
100
80
Subject 1
60
CBC
40
20
23
21
19
17
15
13
11
9
7
5
3
1
0
12
10
Subject 2
8
6
4
2
Behavioral
Consultation
23
21
19
17
15
13
11
9
7
5
3
1
0
100
80
Subject 3
60
Behavioral
Consultation
40
20
23
21
19
17
15
13
11
9
7
5
3
1
0
100
80
Subject 4
60
CBC
40
20
23
21
19
17
15
13
11
9
7
5
3
0
1
Percent Compliance with Checking
BS Level and Tak ing Insulin
Incidents/Day of Snacking
without Permission
Percent of Insulin Injections
Monitored by Adult
Baseline
Percent Compliance with Checking BS
Level and Tak ing Insulin
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
grandparent. Prior to the study, this was not
occurring and was of concern because it was
feared that this boy was not taking his insulin
correctly. Participant 1’s mother felt that there
were times when he did not take insulin or took
only a portion of the prescribed dosage. For
Participant 1, baseline data for taking insulin
399
Day of Study
Figure 2.
Target behavior response by subject across experimental phases.
LASECKI, OLYMPIA, CLARK, JENSON, AND TUESDAY HEATHFIELD
a 75% average rate of compliance (std ⫽ 28.9).
At follow-up, the school year was completed
and data from the school nurse was not possible
to obtain because she did not have daily contact
with Participant 1 during the day to observe
insulin injections.
Participant 2. For Participant 2, the identified target behavior consisted of asking permission from an adult before snacking on foods, particularly in the afternoon after school. Prior to the
study, Participant 2 was eating food from the
refrigerator or cupboards without permission. This
behavior was contributing to his hyperglycemia
based upon information gathered during the Behavioral Consultation PII. For Participant 2, baseline data was calculated to be 7.37 (range ⫽ 4 –11;
std ⫽ 2.56) episodes per day of taking food from
the refrigerator or cupboards without asking permission from his mother. Treatment data was calculated to be an average of 1.33 episodes per day
(range ⫽ 0 –3; std ⫽ 1.0) with an effect size
of 2.36. At follow-up, Participant 2 was calculated
to be taking food without permission 3.25
(std ⫽ 1.71) times per day.
Participant 3. For Participant 3, the target
behavior consisted of checking blood glucose
levels four times per day and taking insulin
appropriately. Prior to the study, Participant 3’s
mother was concerned that her daughter was
inconsistently checking her blood glucose levels and inappropriately administering her insulin. For Participant 3, baseline data was calculated to be 64.29% compliance with checking
blood sugar levels and taking insulin (range ⫽
0 –100; std ⫽ 28.95). Treatment data was calculated to be an average of 93.75% compliance
with treatment (range ⫽ 75–100; std ⫽ 11.57)
with an effect size of 1.02. At follow-up, Participant 3 was calculated to be complying with
treatment at a reduced rate of 50% (std ⫽ 45.6).
Participant 4. For Participant 4, the identified target behavior consisted of checking blood
glucose levels four times per day and taking
insulin appropriately. Prior to the study, Participant 4 was inconsistently checking his blood
glucose levels. He was also suspected of lying
about his blood glucose levels so he would not
have to take insulin. For Participant 4, baseline
data was calculated to be 48.21% compliance
with checking blood sugar levels and taking
insulin (range ⫽ 0 –75; std ⫽ 18.25). Treatment
data was calculated to be an average of 86.11%
compliance with treatment (range ⫽ 50 –100%;
std ⫽ 18.16) with an effect size of 2.08. At
follow-up, Participant 4 was calculated to be
complying with treatment at a reduced rate
of 56.25% (std ⫽ 23.94).
Comparing BC to CBC
To determine the level of effectiveness for
the two consultation models used, effect size
(ES) was calculated for each using the Busk and
Serlin (1992) “no assumptions” approach. ES
was calculated for both CBC and BC cases to
determine the strength of change related to
blood glucose levels and noncompliance behaviors. Figure 3 shows the ES calculated for each
2.5
2
Effect Size
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
400
1.5
Target Behaviors
1
Blood Glucose
Levels
0.5
0
CBC
Behavioral
Consultation
Figure 3. Differences in effect size calculations for CBC and BC related to changes in blood
glucose level and target behavior response.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
BEHAVIORAL MANAGEMENT OF TYPE 1 DIABETES
type of consultation related to changes in blood
glucose levels and target behaviors. ES calculations were determined based upon changes
from baseline to the end of treatment and exclude follow-up data. Reviewing the data, all
participants made positive gains during intervention. The average ES for target behaviors
was higher than the average ES for blood glucose levels.
In Figure 3, CBC was calculated to have
average effect sizes of 1.75 and 2.22 for
changes in blood glucose level and target behaviors from baseline to treatment, respectively.
Behavioral Consultation was found to have average effect sizes of 0.88 and 1.69 for changes
in blood glucose level and target behaviors,
respectively. The average ES for CBC and BC
were found to be 1.99 and 1.29, respectively.
This data, while interesting, is limited to only
four subjects and does not by itself support the
utility of one approach over another.
Consumer Satisfaction
Consumer satisfaction measures were collected in a variety of ways as part of this research. Consultees were asked to complete the
Behavioral Intervention Rating Scale—Revised
(BIRS-R) to assess the acceptability of using
mystery motivator as the primary intervention
and to assess the acceptability of the consultation model used. Children in the study were
asked to complete the Children’s Intervention
Rating Profile (CIRP) to assess how agreeable
they were to the use of mystery motivator as the
intervention in the study.
Acceptability of mystery motivator. Consultees rated their acceptability of mystery motivator by completing the Behavioral Intervention Rating Scale (BIRS-R). Items on the
BIRS-R are rated on a 6-point Likert scale, with
total possible scores ranging from 24 to 144.
High scores on the BIRS-R reflect a high degree
of acceptability. Average acceptability responses for parent and school consultees were
calculated to be 124 and 133, respectively. Individual item means for the parent and school
consultee responses to the BIRS-R were calculated to be 5.17 and 5.54, respectively. These
results suggest that both parent and school nurse
consultees believed that mystery motivator was,
overall, highly acceptable as an intervention.
Children in the study rated their acceptability
401
of mystery motivator with the CIRP. Items on
the CIRP are rated on a 5-point Likert scale,
with low scores indicating high acceptability.
The individual item mean for all participants
was 2.72. This suggests a rather neutral opinion
as to their acceptability of mystery motivator in
treating identified behaviors related to hyperglycemia.
Consultee acceptability of BC and CBC.
Consultees also rated their acceptability of the
type of consultation used (either BC or CBC) by
completing a revised format of the Behavioral
Intervention Rating Scale (BIRS-R). The
BIRS-R is rated on a 6-point Likert scale, with
total possible scores ranging from 24 to 144.
High scores on the BIRS-R reflect a high degree
of acceptability. Parent acceptability of the consultation procedures resulted in an overall mean
of 119.25 with an individual item mean of 4.97.
School consultee acceptability of the consultation procedures resulted in an overall mean of
129.0 with an individual item mean of 5.38.
Acceptability ratings by all consultees for CBC
were found to be 126.5 (individual item mean
of 5.27). Acceptability ratings by parent consultees for Behavioral Consultation were found
to be 114.5 (individual item mean of 4.77).
Treatment Integrity
Intervention integrity. Treatment plan
worksheets were given to each of the parent
consultees and completed on a daily basis to
determine the reported percent compliance in
carrying out the mystery motivator intervention
in the proper manner. All parents completed the
forms for each day of intervention. Across all
four parent consultees, a 96% compliance with
the outlined steps of mystery motivator was
calculated. Thus, parents reported a high level
of compliance in following the steps of mystery
motivator.
Consultation integrity. All consultation sessions were conducted using a standardized set
of questions adapted from Sheridan et al. (1996)
for CBC and from Kratochwill and Bergan
(1990) for BC. The standard formats were used
to help ensure that both BC and CBC procedures used were consistent with each model’s
original intent and standards.
After each session of consultation, the consultant quantitatively evaluated treatment integrity using behavioral checklists. Checklists were
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
402
LASECKI, OLYMPIA, CLARK, JENSON, AND TUESDAY HEATHFIELD
used as a self-monitoring technique to determine the percentage of compliance in following
the intended structure for each type of consultation.
Results for the problem identification, problem analysis, and treatment evaluation phases of
BC and CBC treatment were determined to
be 96, 98.5, and 100%, respectively. Based
upon this self-monitoring technique, treatment
integrity for each of the three phases of either
BC or CBC were high and suggest consistent
adherence to each consultation approaches’ intended formats. No significant differences were
found between treatment integrity data for BC
and CBC (98.3% vs. 98%).
Social Validity
Goal attainment scaling procedures provided
a means of assessing parent and school consultee perceptions of case outcomes. The scale
requires parents and school nurses to report the
degree of goal completion, with a possible response range of ⫺2 (goal not at all met) to ⫹ 2
(goal fully met). In all cases for this research,
parents and school nurses reported that either
the goal was fully met (⫹2) or partially met
(⫹1) for all cases at the time that consultation
was discontinued. Parent consultees reported
for 75% of the cases (N ⫽ 3) that the consultation goal was fully met and for 25% (N ⫽ 1)
that the goal was partially met. Of the two
school nurse consultees, one rated the goal as
fully met and the other rated the goal as partially
met.
The BIRS-R effectiveness factor (7 items)
was also utilized as a way to summarize social
validity data. The BIRS-R effectiveness factor
as rated by both parent and school consultees
yielded a mean of 5.21 across all participants.
Thus, consultee ratings suggest that for the mystery motivator intervention and consultation
models used in the study, both were rated as
highly effective.
Discussion
This study assessed a specific behavioral intervention (mystery motivator) in conjunction
with the use of CBC and BC strategies to alleviate difficulties children face due to Type I
diabetes. There is no previous research that utilizes specific behavioral modification strategies
based on intermittent reinforcement to lead to
better control of blood glucose levels for children with T1 diabetes. Likewise, no study previously used either CBC or BC to assist in
implementing a behavioral strategy focusing on
children with diabetes.
Results show that the behavioral intervention
implemented within the context of either BC or
CBC significantly reduced hyperglycemia in
children with T I diabetes. All four participants
in this study improved from baseline levels reflecting uncontrolled blood sugar levels to more
normalized levels during treatment. The average overall reduction in blood glucose measured daily by participants was 115.3 mg/dL. In
other words, on an average day during treatment, participants were experiencing markedly
less hyperglycemia than they did at baseline. It
is important to note that all participants made
positive gains during intervention. While the
average ES for target behaviors was higher than
the average ES for blood glucose levels, this
may be attributed to the fact that even when
target behaviors improved, not all variables affecting blood glucose are impacted and therefore do not result in equally large improvements
or gains.
Follow-up results obtained 1 month after
treatment suggest mild to moderate maintenance of improvement across time. Participants 1, 2, and 4 all continued to achieve better
blood glucose measures compared to their baseline levels by an average of 83.8 mg/dL. Only
Participant 3 declined significantly from treatment gains made when follow-up data was gathered. Participant 3 went from baseline blood
glucose levels of 255.07 mg/dL to 267.69
mg/dL at follow-up. This suggests that Participant 3 reverted to a very similar pattern of
treatment noncompliance after consultation was
discontinued. Participants 1, 2, and 4 continued
to do much better in controlling blood glucose
levels compared to baseline rates.
The reduction of hyperglycemia demonstrated in this research has a potential for directly reducing the risk of both short and long
term medical complications for children with
diabetes. Secondary medical complications
such as tissue damage leading to retinopathy,
neuropathy, and nephropathy as well as diabetic
ketoacidosis may have a reduced occurrence
due to better blood glucose control. Past research indicates that keeping blood sugar levels
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
BEHAVIORAL MANAGEMENT OF TYPE 1 DIABETES
as close to normal also slows the onset and
progression of eye, kidney, and nerve diseases
caused by diabetes (Wysocki, 1999).
Results also show that mystery motivator,
combined with either variation of consultation,
significantly decreased noncompliance related
target behaviors associated with each participant’s hyperglycemia. For instance, at baseline
Participant 1 was rated as allowing his mother
to supervise injections of insulin only 33% of
the time whereas during the treatment phase,
this Participant became much more motivated
to do so. In the treatment phase, Participant 1
allowed an adult to supervise injections of insulin 94% of the time. This represents a particularly important gain in light of health risks and
the fact that this participant was not taking
insulin properly prior to treatment or indicating
that he was taking insulin when in fact he was
not. Participant 1’s ability to dramatically improve compliance with adult supervision of injections correlates significantly with gains in
improved blood glucose level across the same
time. Participant 1 may have demonstrated even
better gains if the school year continued and
allowed follow-up data to be collected there.
Since all monitoring responsibilities were
shifted to the home environment, slightly lower
levels of compliance may have been more
likely. In addition, gains were even more impressive in light of the student’s history of having behavioral problems warranting special education classification.
Participant 2 also made positive gains related to
the behavior targeted during consultation. Participant 2’s gains were acquired by complying with
the rule targeted during consultation that permission must be gained by an adult prior to snacking.
During baseline, Participant 2 had been snacking
without permission on foods that played a role in
elevating his blood sugar levels. Prior to consultation, it was felt that after school hours were most
problematic for this participant. With increased
supervision and use of mystery motivator, snacking levels from baseline to treatment were reduced
six fold, and corresponded directly to improved
blood glucose levels.
Interestingly, Participant 2’s mother also restricted food availability by not buying some
items that she had in the past. Thus, during treatment, Participant 2’s snacking not only was reduced by the mystery motivator intervention, but
also by food being removed from home. This may
403
have reduced opportunities for Participant 2 to
snack at home without parental permission. Participant 2’s mother also seemed to restrict typical
activities outside of the family home after baseline. This may have allowed for better control over
inappropriate snacking behaviors.
Participant 3, the only female child in the study
also improved in both target behaviors of checking blood glucose levels and taking insulin. This
again was associated with better blood glucose
levels during the treatment phase. At follow-up,
Participant 3’s compliance with monitoring blood
glucose and taking insulin injections had faltered
slightly when compared to gains made during
treatment. Participant 3’s mother indicated that
her use of the mystery motivator had declined,
likely leading to Participant 3’s decline in performance at follow-up. Unfortunately, this also impacted this Participant’s blood glucose levels,
which reverted to baseline levels when follow-up
data was gathered.
Participant 4 who participated in CBC also
improved in monitoring blood glucose levels
and taking insulin compared to pretreatment
levels for these targeted behaviors. Participant 4
responded very well to the procedures and may
have benefited most from the positive nature of
the intervention. Prior to the study, Participant 4
was described as more anxious and possibly
depressed about managing his diabetes. Mystery motivator, by reinforcing positive treatment
behaviors, appeared to boost the confidence of
this participant, in turn improving his confidence level around peers.
Improvements were also anecdotally noted
for participants related to psychological functioning. Participant 1’s self-reports of depression were noticeably reduced and confirmed by
parental report. Participant 2 self-reported significant improvements in anxiety reduction, attention, and social relationships. Participants 3
and 4 also self-reported improvement in selfesteem. Some of the patterns of psychological
functioning found in the literature for children
with TI diabetes were also observed among
participants and their families in this study. For
instance, Participant 1 experienced significant
levels of depression often found in children with
diabetes (Kovacs et al., 1997). All participants
acknowledged risk factors related to feelings of
poor social competence, interpersonal problems, or poor relations with parents. These findings are consistent with typically reported dif-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
404
LASECKI, OLYMPIA, CLARK, JENSON, AND TUESDAY HEATHFIELD
ficulties children with diabetes experience socially, emotionally, or in family relationships.
However, as Kovacs et al., (1992) points out,
variables such as social competence, selfesteem, and aspects of family functioning do
not predict noncompliance with treatment. For
example in this study, Participant 3, with the
fewest identified psychological risk factors, was
later found to be the least compliant with treatment at follow-up.
Adverse psychosocial factors such as lowincome level, single parent family situations,
and so forth may have also contributed to poor
psychological adjustment for Participants 1
and 2. Participants 1 and 2 both came from
single parent, low income families compared
with Participants 3 and 4, each coming from
two-parent families with middle income levels.
When comparing BC and CBC, it was found
that CBC was slightly more effective in reducing
levels of hyperglycemia and improving treatment
compliance. This limited data on only four subjects, while interesting, does not by itself support
the utility of one approach over another, although
results are consistent with previous research
(Sheridan, et al., 1990). Although CBC was found
to be slightly more effective, either method of
consultation appears effective because of the positive gains made within each model. BC may be
particularly useful when it is determined that the
identified problem related to uncontrolled blood
glucose levels is isolated to only one environment.
CBC on the other hand may be most useful when
it is felt that a child and his or her family require
assistance from a team of individuals across a
variety of settings because the identified problem
is not specific to one environment. CBC in particular demands an interplay between team members
in treating problems related to diabetes. As such
CBC appears effective in going beyond traditional
child-centered strategies when implementing effective interventions for helping children by incorporating strategies that consider the organizational
and systems variables contributing to the identified problem of diabetes. School psychologists
and other school based mental health professionals may find that a careful analysis of setting
factors in the Problem Identification Interview
will identify the most appropriate strategy.
Based upon the feedback from treatment acceptability measures, parent and school consultees reported the mystery motivator intervention
and either type of consultation as highly accept-
able. This was not the case when child participants were asked to give feedback on the use of
the mystery motivator. Their level of endorsement was more modest or reflected a neutral
appraisal. Goal Attainment Scaling measures
also provide a high degree of social validity for
changes related to decreases in blood glucose
levels and improvement in treatment compliance. This suggests that approaches utilized in
this study are likely to find a receptive audience
among parents and caregivers who work with
children who struggle with the medical management of diabetes. Additionally, the procedures
employed in this study are very compatible with
actual practice guidelines in both school and
medical clinic settings. Despite their improvements, more substantive feedback is needed
from child participants to understand how they
experience the motivational aspects of the intervention.
Limitations of the Study
Results of this research suggest that an intervention such as the mystery motivator, applied
in the context of either BC or CBC is an effective management strategy for children with diabetes who have difficulty maintaining adherence to a treatment protocol. However, only
four participants were studied, making it difficult to generalize results to a larger population.
It is not clear how the addition of an additional
consultee (school nurse) in the CBC condition
may be more effective than BC due to the fact
that only two participants were involved in each
consultation condition during the study. The
study involved school nurses rather than teachers as consultees. A single school psychologist
served as a consultant in both treatment conditions and for all consultees and clients, thus was
not blind to the model used. The study was also
managed from a medical/clinic setting by the
first author, although some meetings took place
in a school setting. While it is likely that the
consultation strategies and specific interventions could have been implemented completely
in a school setting, there may be certain expectations or other aspects of the clinical treatment
setting that could not be replicated in a school
setting. Further studies originating out of a
school setting and replicating the procedures
used in this study with similarly trained consul-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
BEHAVIORAL MANAGEMENT OF TYPE 1 DIABETES
tant school psychologists would address these
issues.
Another limitation of study was the relatively
short intervention period coupled with a brief
fading technique used with all participants.
Based upon follow-up results, it is possible to
assume that better generalization across time for
improved treatment compliance and blood glucose levels could have been achieved by giving
participants more opportunities for reinforcement. Continued consultee contact with the consultant across additional sessions may be important to assure that intervention procedures are
carried out consistently and without fading too
quickly or extinguishing the program completely. This may have been a factor in the case
of Participant 3, who at follow-up reverted back
to baseline behavior (failure to check blood
glucose and/or take insulin as prescribed by her
treatment team).
Interestingly, school consultees rated consultation as slightly more acceptable than parents.
This may be due to the fact that school consultees were required to implement less of the actual intervention than parents and this led to
higher reported levels of acceptability for both
consultation strategies.
Implications for Future Research
This study provides support for a specific
intervention (mystery motivator) used in concert with behaviorally based consultation strategies to treat behaviors associated with poor
adherence to a treatment protocol for children
with TI diabetes. Future research should determine the efficacy of these strategies across a
greater length of time to determine the impact
beyond a 2-month period. Chronic conditions
such as TI diabetes may require a more continuous stream of intervention support or periodic
“tune-ups” to insure that interventions are maintained or periodically adjusted. Future research
is needed to determine how variables such as
age, gender, and length of diagnosis impact the
effectiveness of these strategies. Additionally,
further research may provide some understanding as to how psychological and family variables influence treatment compliance in the
context of these strategies.
405
References
Bergan, J. R., & Kratochwill, T. R. (1990). Behavioral consultation and therapy. New York, Plenum
Press.
Betteridge, J. (2000). Diabetes: Current perspectives.
London: Martin Dunitz.
Brown, R. T., & DuPaul, G. W. (1999). Introduction
to the Mini-series: Promoting school success in
children with chronic medical conditions. School
Psychology Review, 28, 175–181.
Busk, P. L., & Serlin, R. C. (1992). Meta-analysis for
single-case research. In T. R. Kratochwill & J. R.
Levin (Eds.), Single case research design and
analysis: Applications in psychology and education (pp. 187–212). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Chase, H. P. (1985). Avoiding the short and long
term complications of juvenile diabetes. Pediatrics
in Review, 7, 140 –149.
Daley, K. B., Woderich, D. L., & Hasan, K. (2004).
Classroom based and computerized measures of
attention among children with Type 1 Diabetes
mellitus: The influence of glucose instability. Paper
presented at annual convention of the American
Psychological Association, Honolulu.
Delamater, A. M. (2000). Quality of life in youths
with diabetes. Diabetes Spectrum, 13, 42.
DeMartini-Scully, D. D., Bray, M. A., & Kehle, T. J.
(2000). A packaged intervention to reduce disruptive behaviors in general education students. Psychology in the Schools, 37, 149 –156.
Elliott, S. N., & Von Brock Treuting, M. (1991). The
Behavior Intervention Rating Scale: Development
and validation of a pretreatment acceptability and
effectiveness measure. Journal of School Psychology, 29, 43–51.
Foster, D. W. (1991). Diabetes Mellitus. In J. D.
Wilson, E. Braunwald, K. J. Isselbacher, R. G.
Petersdorf, J. B. Martin, A. S. Fauci, & R. K. Root
(Eds.), Harrison’s Principles of Internal Medicine
(12th ed.) (pp. 1739 –1759). New York: McGrawHill.
Galloway, J., & Sheridan, S. M. (1994). Implementing scientific practices through case studies: Examples of using home-school interventions and
consultation. Journal of School Psychology, 32,
385– 413.
Jenson, W. R., Rhode, G., & Reavis, H. K. (1995).
The Tough Kid Tool Box. Longmont, CO: Sopris
West.
Karam, J. H. (1996). Diabetes mellitus and hypoglycemia. In L. Tierney, Jr., S. McPhee, & M.
apadakis, (Eds.), Current Medical Diagnosis and
Treatment (35th ed.), (pp. 1030 –1068). Stamford:
Appleton & Lange.
Kiresuk, T., Smith, A., & Cardillo, J. (1994). Goal
attainment scaling: Applications, theory, and easurement. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
406
LASECKI, OLYMPIA, CLARK, JENSON, AND TUESDAY HEATHFIELD
Kovacs, M., Goldston, D., Obrosky, D. S., & Bonar,
L. K. (1997). Psychiatric disorders in youths with
IDDM: Rates and risk factors. Diabetes Care, 20,
36 – 44.
Kovacs, M., Goldston, D., Obrosky, D. S., & Iyengar,
S. (1992). Prevalence and predictors of ervasive
noncompliance with medical treatment among
youths with insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus.
Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Adolescent Psychiatry, 31, 1112–1119.
Kovacs, M., Mukerji, P., Iyengar, S., & Drash, A.
(1996). Psychiatric disorder and metabolic control
among youths with IDDM. A longitudinal study.
Diabetes Care, 19, 318 –323.
Kovacs, M., Obrosky, D. S., Goldston, D., & Drash, A.
(1997). Major depressive disorder in youths with
IDDM. A controlled prospective study of course and
outcome. Diabetes Care, 20, 45–51.
Kratochwill, T. R., & Bergan, J. R. (1990). Behavioral consultation in applied settings: An individual guide. New York: Plenum Press.
Kratochwill, T. R., Elliott, S. N., & Busse, R. T.
(1995). Behavior consultation: A five year evaluation of consultant and client outcomes. School
Psychology Quarterly, 10, 87–117.
LeBlanc, D. M. (1999). Mystery motivator versus
reward menu: An investigation of the effects of
home-based reinforcement delivery systems used
with home-school notes on disruptive/disengaged
classroom behavior, Unpublished doctoral dissertation, U Southern Mississippi, Hattiesburgh.
Medway, F. J., & Updyke, J. F. (1985). Metaanalysis of consultation outcome studies. merican
Journal of Community Psychology, 13, 489 –505.
Moore, L. A., Waguespack, A., Wickstrom, Witt, J.,
& Gaydos, G. A. (1994). Mystery motivator: An
effective and time efficient intervention, School
Psychology Review, 23, 106 –118.
Natasi, B. (2000). School psychologists as health-care
providers in the 21st century: Conceptual framework,
professional identity, and professional practice.,
School Psychology Review, 29, 540 –554.
One Touch FastTake. Lifescan Systems, Inc (2006,
May 15). Retrieved May 15, 2006, from http://
lifescan.com/productmeters/select/
Robinson, K. E., & Sheridan, S. M. (2000). Using the
mystery motivator to improve child bedtime compliance. Child & Family Behavior Therapy, 22, 29 – 49.
Sheridan, S. M., & Colton, D. L. (1994). Conjoint
behavioral consultation: A review and case study.
Journal of Educational and Psychological Consultation, 5, 211–228.
Sheridan, S. M., Colton, D. L., Fenstermacher, K.,
Lasecki, K., & Wilson, K. (1996, August). Efficacy
of conjoint behavioral consultation as a vehicle for
inclusion. Paper presented at the annual convention of the American Psychological Association,
Toronto.
Sheridan, S. M., & Kratochwill, T. R. (1992). Behavioral parent-teacher consultation: Conceptual and
research considerations. Journal of School Psychology, 30, 117–139.
Sheridan, S. M., Kratochwill, T. R., & Bergan, J. R.
(1996). Conjoint behavioral consultation: A procedural guide. New York: Plenum Press.
Sheridan, S. M., Kratochwill, T. R., & Elliott, S. N.
(1990). Behavioral consultation with parents and
teachers: Delivering treatment for socially withdrawn children at home and school. School Psychology Review, 19, 33–52.
Sheridan, S. M., Welch, M., & Orme, S. (1996). Is
consultation effective? Remedial and Special Education, 17, 341–354.
Sheridan, S. M.: Eagle, J. W., Cowan, R. J., & Mickelson, W. (2001). The Effects of Conjoint Behavioral
Consultation Results of a 4-Year Investigation. Journal of School Psychology, 39, 361–385.
Sibley, S. (1986). A meta-analysis of school consultation research. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
TX Woman’s University, Denton.
Smith, G. A., & Baum, J. D. (1980). Emotional,
behavioral, and educational disorders in diabetic
children. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 55,
371–375.
Sperling, M. A. (1990). Diabetes Mellitus. In S. A.
Kaplan (Ed.), Clinical pediatric endocrinology
(pp. 127–164). Philadelphia: W. B. Saunders.
Strawhacker, M. T. (2001). Multidisciplinary teaming to promote effective management of Type 1
diabetes for adolescents. Journal of School
Health, 71, 213–217.
Tawney, J. W., & Gast, D. L. (1984). Single Participant research in special education. Columbus,
OH: Merrill.
Valum, J. L. (1996). Student managed study skills
teams: Academic survival for adolescents at risk of
school, Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Utah, Salt Lake City.
Weiner, R. K., Sheridan, S. M., & Jenson, W. R.
(1998). The effects of conjoint behavioral consultation and a structured homework program on
math completion and accuracy in junior high students. School Psychology Quarterly, 13, 281–309.
Witt, J. C., & Elliott, S. N. (1985). Acceptability of
classroom intervention strategies. In T. R.
Kratochwill (Ed.), Advances in school psychology
(Vol. 4; pp. 251–288). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Wysocki, T. (1999, June). Early information on an
ongoing study of intensive therapy and Children:
Advances in behavioral medicine. Paper presented
at the meeting of the American Diabetes Association, San Diego, CA.
Wysocki, T. (2006). Behavioral assessment and intervention in pediatric diabetes. Behavior Modification, 30, 72–92.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Psychological Bulletin
2009, Vol. 135, No. 1, 121–141
© 2009 American Psychological Association
0033-2909/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0014210
What Works in Coping With HIV? A Meta-Analysis With Implications
for Coping With Serious Illness
Judith Tedlie Moskowitz and Jen R. Hult
Cori Bussolari
University of California, San Francisco
University of San Francisco
Michael Acree
University of California, San Francisco
Knowledge of effective ways of coping with HIV is critical to help individuals with HIV maintain the
best possible psychological and physical well-being. The purpose of the present article is to determine,
through meta-analysis, the strength of the evidence regarding 2 questions: (a) Which types of coping are
related to psychological and physical well-being among people with HIV? and (b) Do contextual
(pre–post introduction of highly active antiretroviral therapies [HAART]; time since diagnosis), measurement (HIV-related event vs. generic prompts for coping measurement), or individual (gender)
variables affect the extent to which coping is related to physical and psychological well-being? The
authors’ analysis demonstrates that Direct Action and Positive Reappraisal were consistently associated
with better outcomes in people coping with HIV across affective, health behavior, and physical health
categories. In contrast, disengagement forms of coping, such as Behavioral Disengagement and Use of
Alcohol or Drugs to Cope, were consistently associated with poorer outcomes. The findings also indicate
that in some cases, coping effectiveness was dependent on contextual factors, including time since
diagnosis and the advent of HAART.
Keywords: coping, positive affect, negative affect, health behaviors, physical health
the question of what works in coping with HIV. The larger
literature on coping with serious illness, which suffers from the
same heterogeneity, provides little guidance.
As in the general coping literature, the majority of research on
coping with HIV has been based on a transactional theory of stress
and coping (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984), in which coping is viewed as
part of a process that unfolds in response to the demands of the
situation and includes cognitive appraisals of the significance of the
event and associated emotional responses. Coping is defined as efforts
to deal with demands taxing or exceeding the resources of the person
(Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). In other words, coping is a cognitive or
behavioral response to something appraised as stressful. It is the
subjective appraisal, more than the objective characteristics of the
situation, that determines how stressful the event is for an individual
and subsequently drives the coping response.
Many aspects of HIV are potentially stressful (Moskowitz &
Wrubel, 2005). For example, fear of death, the need to adhere to
complex medication regimens, side-effects of the treatments, interactions with a complex medical system, symptoms associated
with disease progression, financial difficulties, stigma, and the
need to incorporate a new identity as someone with a serious
illness are all potential stressors associated with being HIVpositive. It is important to note that these stressors are not necessarily unique to HIV. People living with other illnesses—such as
cancer, diabetes, and arthritis—are subject to many of the same
concerns. Thus, although our focus is on HIV, the findings have
potential application to coping with other illnesses, and our methods could serve as a model for meta-analyses of coping with other
serious illnesses.
Twenty-five years after the identification of the first AIDS
cases, it is estimated that more than one million people in the
United States are living with HIV, the virus that causes AIDS, and
40,000 people become infected each year (Centers for Disease
Control and Prevention, 2007). Although great strides have been
made toward extending length of life and improving quality of life
for people living with the virus, for many people HIV remains a
stressful and demanding illness (Remien et al., 2006). Stress has
deleterious effects on mental health and can hasten the progression
of HIV disease (Bottonari, Roberts, Ciesla, & Hewitt, 2005; Remor, Penedo, Shen, & Schneiderman, 2007; Sledjeski, Delahanty,
& Bogart, 2005; Weaver et al., 2005). Therefore, knowledge about
effective ways of coping with the stress of HIV is key to helping
individuals with HIV maintain the best possible psychological and
physical well-being.
The literature on coping with HIV is large and heterogeneous
with respect to measures of coping, outcomes, and conclusions.
There is little consistency across studies regarding types of coping
and types of outcomes that are assessed and reported. Even a
careful reading of the literature does not provide a clear answer to
Judith Tedlie Moskowitz, Jen R. Hult, and Michael Acree, Osher Center
for Integrative Medicine, Department of Medicine, University of California, San Francisco; Cori Bussolari, Department of Counseling Psychology,
University of San Francisco.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Judith
Tedlie Moskowitz, Osher Center for Integrative Medicine, University of
California, San Francisco, 1701 Divisadero, Suite 150, San Francisco, CA
94115. E-mail: moskj@ocim.ucsf.edu
121
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
MOSKOWITZ, HULT, BUSSOLARI, AND ACREE
122
The purpose of the present article is to critically review the
literature on coping with HIV and to determine, through metaanalysis, the strength of the evidence regarding two questions: (a)
Which types of coping are related to psychological and physical
well-being among people with HIV? and (b) Do contextual (pre–
post introduction of highly active antiretroviral therapies
[HAART]; time since diagnosis), measurement (HIV-related event
vs. generic prompts for coping measurement), or individual (gender) variables affect the extent to which coping is related to
physical and psychological well-being?
Coping
One of the biggest challenges to a comprehensive summary of
the coping literature is the diversity of measures and labels for the
various ways of responding to stress (Schwarzer & Schwarzer,
1996). Conceivably, there are a countless number of ways of
coping, from making a plan of action or fantasizing about an ideal
outcome, to reminding oneself of the good that will come out of
the situation or pretending the stressful event did not happen. In
their comprehensive review of the literature on the structure of
coping, Skinner, Edge, Altman, and Sherwood (2003) listed 400
coping responses.
Most coping measures are based on Folkman and Lazarus’s
(1980, 1988) Ways of Coping Questionnaire. The Ways of Coping
Questionnaire consists of 50 items that generally factor into eight
subscales: Confrontive, Distancing, Self-Controlling, Seeking Social Support, Self-Blame, Escape/Avoidance, Planful ProblemSolving, and Positive Reappraisal. Another frequently used measures is the COPE (Carver, Scheier, & Weintraub, 1989), which is
a 52-item questionnaire designed to measure 14 types of coping.
The COPE subscales are Active Coping, Planning, Suppression of
Competing Activities, Restraint, Seeking Social Support for Instrumental Reasons, Seeking Social Support for Emotional Reasons, Positive Reinterpretation and Growth, Acceptance, Turning
to Religion, Focus on and Venting of Emotions, Denial, Behavioral Disengagement, and Mental Disengagement. The major differences among these and other commonly used coping scales lie
primarily in the number and specificity of types of coping assessed. For example, the Ways of Coping assesses more general
seeking social support, whereas the COPE assesses two more
specific types of seeking support: Seeking Social Support for
Emotional Reasons and Seeking Social Support for Instrumental
Reasons. The COPE also includes subscales that are not tapped by
the Ways of Coping, such as Suppression of Competing Activities,
and Focus on and Venting of Emotions.
In the HIV literature, although many studies use the Ways of
Coping or the COPE (e.g., Ashton et al., 2005; Blaney et al., 2004;
Carels, Baucom, Leone, & Rigney, 1998; Deschamps et al., 2004;
Folkman, Chesney, Pollack, & Coates, 1993), others have developed scales specific to coping with HIV (e.g., Moneyham et al.,
1998; Murphy, Rotheram-Borus, & Marelich, 2003). Even though
these scales were developed specifically to measure coping with
HIV, they have subscales that are similar to the Ways of Coping
and the COPE. For example, the scale developed by Murphy et al.
(2003) has seven factors: Positive Action, Passive Problem Solving, Self-Destructive Escape, Social Support, Spiritual Hope, Depression/Withdrawal, and Nondisclosure/Problem Avoidance.
Moneyham et al.’s (1998) scale includes Seeking Peer Support,
Living Positively, Managing HIV Disease, Seeking Support of
Family/Friends, Isolation/Withdrawal, Spiritual Activities, Denial/
Avoidance, and Seeking Information.
Higher Order Coping Classifications
As evidenced by the above review, working with even a half
dozen coping subscales can become unwieldy for reporting and
analysis, so researchers often group the subscales, either empirically or theoretically, into factors. These second or higher order
coping distinctions include Problem-Focused/Emotion-Focused
(Folkman & Lazarus, 1980), Approach/Avoidance (Roth & Cohen,
1986), Active/Passive (Billings & Moos, 1981; Mercado, Carroll,
Cassidy, & Cote, 2000; Moneyham et al., 1998), Engagement/
Disengagement (Tobin, Holroyd, Reynolds, & Wigal, 1989), and
Cognitive/Behavioral (Billings & Moos, 1981), among others (see
Skinner et al., 2003, for a review and critique of approaches for
classifying coping).
In the HIV literature, a few studies rely on the EmotionFocused/Problem-Focused dichotomy (Gore-Felton et al., 2002;
Pakenham & Rinaldis, 2001); a larger number use an Approach/
Avoidance distinction (Avants, Warburton, & Margolin, 2001;
Halkitis, Kutnick, & Slater, 2005; Heckman et al., 2004; Schmitz
& Crystal, 2000; Semple, Patterson, & Grant, 2000) or a variation
on this distinction: Avoidant/Proactive (Nicholson & Long, 1990),
Avoidant/Adaptive (Simoni & Ng, 2000), and Maladaptive/
Adaptive (Safren, Radomsky, Otto, & Salomon, 2002). For example, Heckman et al. (2004) conducted a principal components
analysis of the items from the Ways of Coping Checklist (Folkman
& Lazarus, 1980) in their sample of 329 men and women living
with HIV. They found two factors and labeled them Active (“I
made a plan of action and followed it”) and Avoidant (“I refused
to believe that it had happened”). Leslie, Stein, and RotheramBorus (2002) selected two factors from the seven measured by the
Coping with Illness Questionnaire (Murphy et al., 2003) to assess
Active and Passive coping in their sample of parents living with
HIV or AIDS.
In summary, a number of general and disease-specific coping
measures are available, most of which are based on the Ways of
Coping Questionnaire. The extensive variability in the specific
types of coping that are measured and reported across studies
presents a challenge to the synthesis of the literature on coping
effectiveness.
Meta-Analyses of Coping
Previous meta-analytic studies of coping have addressed the
challenge of wide variation in types of coping either by focusing
on a few select measures (e.g., Jordan & Revenson, 1999; Penley,
Tomaka, & Wiebe, 2002) or grouping the subscales into established categories based on the descriptions of types of coping
provided in the articles (Aldridge & Roesch, 2007; Ano & Vasconcelles, 2005; Connor-Smith & Flachsbart, 2007; Duangdao &
Roesch, 2008; Littleton, Horsley, John, & Nelson, 2007; Mullen &
Suls, 1982; Nes & Segerstrom, 2006; Roesch & Weiner, 2001;
Suls & Fletcher, 1985; Tamres, Janicki, & Helgeson, 2002). For
example, in a meta-analysis of coping and physical and psychological health, Penley et al. (2002) restricted their analysis to 11
ways of coping measured either with a version of the Ways of
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
COPING WITH HIV
Coping (Folkman & Lazarus, 1985; Folkman, Lazarus, DunkelSchetter, DeLongis, & Gruen, 1986) or the Ways of Coping
Checklist scales developed by Vitaliano, Russo, Carr, Maiuro, and
Becker (1985) based on the Ways of Coping (Folkman & Lazarus,
1980). This approach of restricting the sample to a few select
coping measures simplifies the task of combining findings across
disparate studies. However, this efficiency may come at the expense of comprehensiveness; some potentially important types of
coping are not included on the selected coping measures.
In a meta-analysis of coping and personality, Connor-Smith and
Flachsbart (2007) classified coping hierarchically with three levels
(from broadest to most specific): (a) Engagement versus Broad
Disengagement; (b) Primary Control Engagement, Secondary
Control Engagement, and Narrow Disengagement; and (c) Specific
Coping Strategies (problem solving, social support, emotion regulation, distraction, cognitive restructuring, acceptance, religious
coping, avoidance, withdrawal, and wishful thinking). In addition,
at the broadest level, they also included Negative Emotion Focused
(emotion regulation and uncontrolled expression) and Mixed Emotion Focused coping (a mix of controlled and uncontrolled emotion
regulation and expression strategies). The results supported the
value of distinguishing among specific lower order coping strategies because they were differentially related to facets of personality; these findings would have been obscured had the authors
restricted their analysis to the higher levels of the coping hierarchy.
Meta-Analyses of Coping With Serious Illness
Meta-analyses of studies of coping with serious illness and other
medical procedures can inform the present work on coping with
HIV and provide a metric against which the effects sizes can be
compared. Roesch and colleagues conducted meta-analyses of
coping in people with diabetes (Duangdao & Roesch, 2008), men
with prostate cancer (Roesch et al., 2005), and people who had a
chronic illness or were undergoing a medical procedure (Roesch &
Weiner, 2001). Roesch and Weiner (2001) classified coping three
ways: (a) Approach/Avoidance; (b) Cognitive Approach, Cognitive Avoidance, Behavioral Approach, and Behavioral Avoidance;
and (c) Problem-Focused/Emotion-Focused. The authors noted
that although they chose this classification to deal with the diversity of coping measured in the primary studies, the drawback is
that the unique effectiveness of lower order forms of coping cannot
be determined. Results indicated that Approach, Cognitive Approach, Behavioral Approach, Problem-Focused and EmotionFocused forms of coping were associated with better psychological
adjustment, and Avoidance and Cognitive Avoidance were associated with poorer psychological adjustment. According to Cohen’s (1988) criteria, effect sizes were generally small to medium
(ranging from r ⫽ .13 for Behavioral Approach to r ⫽ ⫺.30 for
Cognitive Avoidance).
Roesch et al. (2005) and Duangdao and Roesch (2008) used an
Approach/Avoidance classification and a Problem-Focused/
Emotion-Focused classification. In the sample of men with prostate cancer (Roesch et al., 2005), Approach coping was significantly associated with better overall adjustment (r ⫽ .23), and
Avoidance coping was significantly associated with poorer overall
adjustment (r ⫽ ⫺.21), both small to medium effects. Among
people with diabetes (Duangdao & Roesch, 2008), Approach coping was associated with better overall adjustment (r ⫽ .20), less
123
anxiety (r ⫽ ⫺.16), less depression (r ⫽ ⫺.19), and better glycemic control (r ⫽ .17). Avoidance coping was not related to any of
the four outcomes.
On the basis of these previous meta-analyses of coping with
serious illness, it appears that effects sizes tend to be small and that
Approach coping is associated with better outcomes. Avoidance
coping is less consistently related to outcomes; however, when
there is a significant effect, Avoidance coping tends to be associated with poorer outcomes. These studies did not, however, examine effects of any lower order forms of coping, possibly obscuring significant unique effects of these more specific types of
coping.
Classification of Coping
In the present meta-analysis, we use two approaches to address
the problem of coping classification. The first approach uses a
lower order classification based on specific subscales from the
Ways of Coping and the COPE, and the second uses a higher
order, more macroscopic classification that relies on the Approach/
Avoidance distinction. We selected Approach/Avoidance because
in the literature on coping with HIV, most of the higher order
classifications rely on some form of this dichotomy, and this
classification allows us to compare with previous meta-analyses of
coping in seriously ill samples that relied on an Approach/
Avoidance classification (Duangdao & Roesch, 2008; Roesch et
al., 2005; Roesch & Weiner, 2001). The use of these two approaches allows us to determine the potentially unique effects of
lower order coping subscales, but it also addresses the question of
whether and under what conditions the more manageable Approach/Avoidance distinction might be preferable.
Selection of Outcomes: What Is Effective Coping?
Effective coping can be defined a number of ways. Zeidner and
Saklofske (1996) listed eight criteria that could be used to define
effective coping: resolution of the stressful situation, reduction of
physiological reactions, reduction of psychological distress, normative social functioning, return to prestress activities, well-being
of self and others affected by the situation, maintaining positive
self-esteem, and perceived effectiveness. However, these generic
outcomes are unlikely to apply in all situations. According to stress
and coping theory, one approach to determining coping effectiveness is to examine the effect of coping on positive and negative
outcomes that are relevant and appropriate in the situation under
study (Folkman, 1992). In the present review, we focus on the
situation of having HIV and examine four sets of outcomes that are
most commonly used in the HIV stress and coping literature:
positive affect, negative affect, health behaviors, and physical
health.
As in the more general coping literature, studies of coping with
HIV most frequently rely on psychological outcomes, such as
depressive mood (e.g., Folkman et al., 1993; Patterson et al., 1995;
Penedo et al., 2001; Schmitz & Crystal, 2000), anxiety (Catz,
Gore-Felton, & McClure, 2002; Commerford, Orr, Gular, Reznikoff, & O’Dowd, 1994; Kelly et al., 2000), or other negative
affective states, such as anger (Weaver et al., 2005) or perceived
stress (Koopman et al., 2000), as the primary outcome. Physical
health outcomes, such as CD4 (an indicator of strength of the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
124
MOSKOWITZ, HULT, BUSSOLARI, AND ACREE
immune system; Ashton et al., 2005; Patterson et al., 1995), viral
load (Avants et al., 2001), and symptoms (Pakenham & Rinaldis,
2001; Phillips, Sowell, Rush, & Murdaugh, 2001), are also often
included. Some studies examine health behavior outcomes, such as
adherence to antiretroviral medications (Avants et al., 2001; Deschamps et al., 2004; Ironson et al., 2005), safer sexual behaviors
(Gore-Felton et al., 2002; Semple et al., 2000), and decreased drug
use and smoking (Collins et al., 2001). Finally, as in other areas of
psychology, researchers in the area of coping with HIV are beginning to include positive affective outcomes, such as life satisfaction and positive mood, as variables separate from depression and
other negative affects (Fleishman et al., 2000; Siegel & Schrimshaw, 2005).
Contextual, Individual, and Methodological Influences on
Coping Effectiveness
Dispositional and Situational Coping Measurement
As noted above, a central tenet of stress and coping theory is that
coping is not inherently adaptive or maladaptive (Lazarus & Fol...
Purchase answer to see full
attachment